Faʻafitauli Lelei o le Amio Lelei i Tagata Taitoatasi ma le Initaneti o Mea Faʻafefiloi: Faʻaaoga Suesuega e Mafai Ona Tutupu (2017)

Can Jiao1 †, Ting Wang1 †, Xiaozhe Peng1 ma Fang Cui1,2 *

  • 1College of Psychology and Sociology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 2Shenzhen Key Laboratory of Affective and Social Cognitive Science, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China

Internet addiction disorder (IAD) is associated with deficits in social communication and avoidance of social contact. It has been hypothesized that people with IAD may have an impaired capacity for empathy. The purpose of the current study was to examine the processing of empathy for others’ pain in IADs. Event-related potentials produced in response to pictures showing others in painful and non-painful situations were recorded in 16 IAD subjects and 16 healthy controls (HCs). The N1, P2, N2, P3, and late positive potential components were compared between the two groups. Robust picture × group interactions were observed for N2 and P3. The painful pictures elicited larger N2 and P3 amplitudes than the non-painful pictures did only in the HC group but not in the IAD group. The results of this study suggest that both of the early automatic and of the later cognitive processes of pain empathy may be impaired in IADs. This study provides psychophysical evidence of empathy deficits in association with IAD. Further studies combining multidimensional measurements of empathy are needed to confirm these findings.

faʻatomuaga

Internet addiction disorder (IAD) has been described as an inability to control internet use despite severe negative consequences and is generally conceptualized as a behavioral addiction (Tam ma Walter, 2013; D’Hondt et al., 2015; Kuss ma Lopez-Fernandez, 2016), representing a specific impairment that involves online and/or offline web misuse, and it is mainly relevant for young generations (Grant et al., 2010; Balconi et al., 2017). Although whether IAD is a mental disorder taʻitasi is still controversial, the public health and social issues related to IAD are clear and the neural correlates of IAD have begun to be explored (D’Hondt and Maurage, 2015).

It has been suggested that IAD may have some commonalities with substance abuses. For example, individuals with IAD show a decreased executive control ability, reflecting in the lack of ability to inhibit the behavior once it has been initiated or to refrain from the behavior after a period of abstinence (Brand et al., 2014; D’Hondt and Maurage, 2015). Electrophysiological studies have shown reduced N2 amplitudes in the NoGo trials in a Go/NoGo task as well as a reduced medial frontal negativity (MFN) amplitude in the incongruent trials of the Stroop task, findings which are suggestive of an impairment of executive control (Dong et al., 2011). Besides, individuals with IAD also show impaired processing of social stimuli, such as faces. He et al. (2011) found that compared to healthy controls (HCs), subjects with IADs showed reduced P1 and N170 in the processing of faces.

Empathy refers to the ability to share and understand others’ emotions or feelings (Decety and Lamm, 2006). Experiencing empathy relies on the integration of two components: an automatic early emotional contagion system and a higher-level cognitive system that allows the self-regulation and elaboration of the situations (Decety ma Jackson, 2004; Mella et al., 2012). The ability to empathize with others has long been viewed as crucial for successful social interactions (Hetu et al., 2012). The amount of internet use has been found to correlate inversely with the ability to interact with other people (Engelberg and Sjoberg, 2004). Accordingly, people with IAD often neglect their social lives (Talavou, 1998a). Hence, empathy may be an influential factor in the progressive diminution of real-life social interactions among internet users (Melchers et al., 2015).

However, studies focusing on the neural basis of empathic processing in IADs are still very rare. Thus far, to our knowledge, only two studies have investigated empathy in IADs. Firstly, Melchers et al. (2015) obtained evidence of a negative relationship between internet addiction and empathy, as evidenced by self-reported empathy and problematic internet use scores (Melchers et al., 2015). Secondly, an event-related potential (ERP) study found that youth with IAD showed a reduced difference in N2 amplitudes between painful and non-painful stimuli comparing to HCs when processing pictures showing others in pain (Wang et al., 2014). The first aforementioned study was an exploratory correlation study and the second one involved a very specific population (Chinese urban left-behind children), limiting its generalizability.

Empathy for pain has been shown to involve two distinct temporally processes. The first is an early automatic, bottom-up process, reflected by the N1, P2, and N2 components that correspond to emotional contagion and affective sharing. Secondly, there is a top-down controlled, cognitive process, reflected by the P3 and late positive potential (LPP) components that regulates empathic responses and makes a clear self-other distinction (Fan and Han, 2008; Mella et al., 2012; Sessa et al., 2014). The bottom-up process refers to the unconscious and automatic emotional contagion and affective sharing triggered involuntarily by observing other’s pain, which is not influenced by instructions or task demands. The top-down controlled process, on the other hand, refers to the process that is under control of the intentions of the observer and can be voluntarily modulated by factors such as the instructions, task demands, prior experience, social relations, etc., (Fan and Han, 2008). Thus, this model can help us to resolve which stage of empathy may be impaired in IADs.

IAD may be comorbid with other psychiatric states, especially depression and anxiety (Sanders et al., 2000; Yen et al., 2007; Wei et al., 2012; Lai et al., 2015). Depressed individuals show reduced awareness of others’ emotion, impaired emotion recognition, and deficits in empathy and perspective taking [for review, (Kupferberg et al., 2016)]. Anxiety can also reduce affective empathic responses to others’ pain (Negd et al., 2011). Hence, comorbid depression and anxiety may be influential confounding factors in the present study. Thus, we employed exclusion criteria for detection of signs of depression or anxiety.

The aim of the current study was to explore how the processing of others’ pain may be different among individuals with IAD and HCs. We hypothesized that the IADs would be less responsive, or less discriminative to other’s pain than the HCs. If the early automatic stage of empathy is impaired, it should be evident in the N1 (Ibanez et al., 2011; Lyu et al., 2014), P2 (Rutgen et al., 2015), and/or N2 (Cui et al., 2016a) components. Conversely, if the voluntary, top-down processing is impaired, then it should be evident in the P3 and/or LPP (Ibanez et al., 2011).

Mea ma Metotia

Talosaga Tetele

All research procedures were approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of Shenzhen University Medical School according to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were given written informed consent after they fully understand the study.

Tagata e Auai

A total number of 16 participants with IAD and 16 HCs were recruited from local universities. There was no significant difference between the two groups with respect to age, handedness, and education. We used Young’s Internet Addiction Test (IAT) to screen for IAD (Talavou, 1998b). All IAD subjects were with a score of ≥ 40 on the IAT (Note: IAT scores on 40–60 indicate mild internet addiction; 60–80 indicate moderate internet addiction; and 80–100 indicate server internet addiction). Moreover, because IAD may be comorbid with other psychiatric states, especially depression and anxiety, we excluded IAD participants who scored ≥ 40 on either the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (SDS) (Zung, 1965) or the Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS) (Zung, 1971) (the cutoff scores are 53 for SDS and 50 for SAD in Chinese norm). Exclusion criteria for both IAD and control participants were as follows: pregnancy, history of head injury, and other neurological disorders substance abuse or dependence in past 6 months.

Suauu

The visual stimuli used were pictures showing a person’s hands/forearms/feet in painful or non-painful situations, which have been used in previous ERP studies (Meng et al., 2012; Meng et al., 2013). All the situations depicted in these pictures were ordinary events in daily life. All the events showing in the non-painful pictures were corresponding to those in the painful pictures, but without the nociceptive component (Figure 1A). There were 60 painful pictures and 60 non-painful pictures in total. All of them had the same size of 9 × 6.76 cm (width × height) and 100 pixels per inch. Luminance, contrast, and color were matched between painful and non-painful pictures. Previous studies have confirmed that painful and non-painful pictures were significantly different on the dimensions of pain intensity, arousal level, and emotional valence, according to self-reported rating (Meng et al., 2012).

 
Faʻaaliga 1
www.frontiersin.org  

Faʻaaliga 1. (A) Examples of the pictures (Left panel: painful picture; Right panel: non-painful picture); (B) Structure of one trial. Each trial started with a 500 ms fixation, after a 400–700 ms random blank interval, the picture appeared for a maximum of 2000 ms and the participants were supposed to judge if the picture was a painful one or a non-painful one as soon and accurately as possible. The picture disappeared when a response was given. The ISI between trials was 800–1200 ms randomly.

 
 

Faataʻitaʻiga Faʻapitoa

Stimulus display and behavioral data acquisition were conducted using E-Prime software (Version 2.0, Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Boston, MA, United States). During the task, participants sat comfortably in an electrically shielded room approximately 90 cm from a 15-inch color computer screen.

The participants were asked to observe pictures. In each trial, a fixation was presented on a white screen for 500 ms, followed a 400- to 700-ms blank interval. Then the target picture would be presented for a maximum of 2000 ms. The ISI between trials was 800–1200 ms randomly (Figure 1B). Participants were instructed to judge whether this picture showing a painful or non-painful situation as quickly as possible by pressing “F” or “J” buttons on the keyboard placed in front of them. The button pressing was counter-balanced among participants. The picture would disappear as soon as a response was given. There are four sessions in the experiment. Each session contains 60 trials, including 30 painful pictures and 30 non-painful pictures. Each picture repeated twice in total. Four conditions were generated accordingly: IADs observing the painful pictures (IAD_P); IADs observing the non-painful pictures (IAD_NP); HCs observing the painful pictures (HC_P); and HCs observing the non-painful pictures (HC_NP). After the task, both groups of participants were asked to fulfill the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI). The IRI has been one of the most widely used indices of empathy, which is a questionnaire that assesses the empathy trait using four subscales: perspective taking, fantasy, empathic concern, and personal distress (PD) (Davis, 1983).

EEG Acquisition and Preprocessings

Electroencephalography (EEG) data were recorded from a 63-electrodes scalp cap using the 10–20 system (Brain Products, Munich, Germany). The channel TP10 was used as the reference during recording. Two electrodes located above and below the left eye were used to measure the electrooculogram (EOG). EEG and EOG activities were amplified at 0.01–100 Hz band-passes and sampled at 500 Hz. All electrode impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ.

Electroencephalography (EEG) data were preprocessed and analyzed using MATLAB R2011b (MathWorks) and EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). EEG data at each electrode were re-referenced to the average of the left and right mastoids before further analysis. Then the signal passed with 0.01–30 Hz band-pass filter. Time windows of 200 ms before and 1000 ms after the onset of picture stimuli were segmented from EEG and the whole epoch was baseline-corrected by the 200 ms prior to the onset of the picture. EOG artifacts were corrected using an independent component analysis (ICA) (Jung et al., 2001). Epochs with amplitude values exceeding ± 50 μV at any electrode were excluded from the average, and all trials with incorrect responses were excluded from further analysis [Rejected epochs: 16.75 ± 6.04 (HCs); 18.25 ± 2.35 (IADs)].

sitatisika

For the reaction time and accuracy, a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA was performed with pictures (painful picture and non-painful picture) as the within-subject factor and groups (IAD and HC) as the between-subject factor. Descriptive data were presented as the (mean ± SE). The significance level was set at p <0.05.

This study focused on the ERPs elicited by pictures showing others in painful and non-painful situations. We analyzed the components of the frontal N1 (90–150 ms), frontal P2 (180–220 ms), fronto-central N2 (200–280 ms), parietal P3 (300–400 ms), and centro-parietal LPP (550–650 ms) according to grand-averaged ERP, the topographies, and relevant literature (Decety et al., 2010; Meng et al., 2013). Mean amplitudes were measured for each component. Notice that the time windows selected were mainly based on the grand-averaged ERP of all trials for painful and non-painful conditions from both groups. The time windows of several components were slightly different from the literature [40–50 ms before or after the time windows selected in the previous papers (Meng et al., 2012, 2013)]. To demonstrate that the results were not artifacts of the prior selected time windows, we conducted the following-up analyses using varying time windows (move forward 40 ms and move backward 40 ms) for the components that reported significance. All of the results revealed a similar pattern of results (we reported the results from the time window in the middle). By conducting analyses using time windows of varying duration/onset/offset, it could show that the significance of the results is a consistent effect (Bacigalupo and Luck, 2015; Luck and Gaspelin, 2017).

Further statistical analysis was conducted in IBM SPSS Statistics 22 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States). Previous studies using similar stimuli suggested the early component N1, P2, N2, and the late components P3, LPP were particularly related to observing other’s pain. On the basis of the topographical distribution of grand-averaged ERP activity and the previous studies, different sets of electrodes for each component were chosen (Meng et al., 2012, 2013; Lyu et al., 2014). F3, Fz, F4, FC3, FCz, and FC3 were selected for the analysis of N1 and N2; FC3, FCz, FC4, C3, Cz, and C4 were selected for the analysis of P2; CP3, CPz, CP4, P3, Pz, and P4 were selected for the analysis of P3; C3, Cz, C4, CP3, CPz, and CP4 were selected for the analysis of LPP. Repeated-measures ANOVA with pictures (painful and non-painful) as the within-subject factor and groups (IAD and HC) as the between-subject factor were performed for the mean amplitudes of all selected electrodes sites for each component. All statistical analyses met the requirements of parametrical statistical tests. Degrees of freedom for F-ratios were corrected according to the Greenhouse–Geisser method. Statistical differences were considered significant at p <0.05; faʻailoga comparisons were Bonferroni corrected at p <0.05.

To examine whether the effect we observed in ERP data was related to the participant’s empathic trait, firstly, we calculated the differences between the amplitudes of ERPs elicited by the painful stimuli and the non-painful stimuli in the time windows of N2 and P3. The amplitudes were calculated as the mean of the amplitudes of all selected electrodes (F3, Fz, F4, FC3, FCz, and FC3 for N2; CP3, CPz, CP4, P3, Pz, and P4 for P3). Secondly, we run Pearson correlation analyses between the differences of ERP amplitudes and the scores of the four subscales of IRI, separately.

i'uga

Fa'afitauli

For the accuracy rate, the main effect of picture [F(1,30) = 1.854, p = 0.183, η2p

= 0.058), vaega (F(1,30) = 0.557, p = 0.461, η2p = 0.018], and the interaction of picture × group [F(1,30) = 0.146, p = 0.705, η2p = 0.005] were not significant (range of accuracy rate: 79–99%, mean ± SE: 91.25 ± 4.8%). For reaction time, we found a significant main effect of picture [F(1,30) = 23.662, p <0.001, η2p = 0.441]. Both groups response faster to painful situation comparing to the non-painful situation (IAD_P: 633.488 ± 54.928 ms; IAD_NP: 669.714 ± 74.255 ms; HC_P: 645.528 ± 55.207 ms; HC_NP: 684.085 ± 61.851 ms). The main effect of group [F(1,30) = 0.413, p = 0.525, η2p = 0.014] and the interaction of picture × group [F(1,30) = 0.023, p = 0.880, η2p

= 0.001] were not significant (range of RTs: 554–861 ms; mean ± SE: 659.5 ± 62.6 ms).

For IRI scores, we run independent t-tests to compare the scores of IAD group and HC group for all four subscales. It was found that on the subscale “PD,” the scores of the IAD group were significantly smaller than the scores of the HC group [IAD: 8.125 ± 0.875; HC: 10.375 ± 0.651; t(30) = -2.063, p = 0.048]. The differences between two groups of the other three subscales were not significant (p > 0.116) (Siata 1).

 
TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org  

TABLE 1. Participants demographics for IAD participants and healthy controls.

 
 

Event-Related Potentials (ERPs)

N1. The main effect of picture [F(1,30) = 3.180, p = 0.085, η2p

= 0.096], the main effect of group [F(1,30) = 0.465, p = 0.500, η2p = 0.015] and the interaction of picture × group [F(1,30) = 0.131, p = 0.720, η2p

= 0.004] were not significant.

F2. The main effect of picture [F(1,30) = 1.550, p = 0.223, η2p

= 0.049], the main effect of group [F(1,30) = 0.098, p = 0.756, η2p = 0.003] and the interaction of picture × group [F(1,30) = 0.729, p = 0.400, η2p

= 0.024] were not significant.

N2. The main effect of picture was significant [F(1,30) = 6.406, p = 0.017, η2p

= 0.176]. Painful pictures elicited significantly more negative amplitudes than the non-painful pictures (-6.301 ± 0.745 μV and -5.650 ± 0.769 μV). The main effect of group was not significant [F(1,30) = 0.039, p = 0.845, η2p = 0.001]. The interaction of group × picture was significant [F(1,30) = 6.838, p = 0.016, η2p

= 0.177]. Pairwise comparisons showed that the amplitudes elicited by the painful pictures were significantly more negative than the amplitudes elicited by the non-painful pictures only in the HC group (-6.481 ± 1.088 μV and -5.176 ± 1.054 μV, p = 0.001) but not in the IAD group (-6.124 ± 1.088 μV and -6.122 ± 1.054 μV, p = 0.577) (Figures 2, 4A ma le Laulau 2).

 
Faʻaaliga 2
www.frontiersin.org  

Faʻaaliga 2. The Grand average on Fz and FCz and topography of N2 in all four conditions [Painful pictures in HC group (HC_P); Non-painful pictures in HC group (HC_NP); Painful pictures in IAD group (IAD_P); and Non-painful pictures in IAD group (IAD_NP)]. The time window of the topography was corresponding to the gray square covered area.

 
 
Faʻaaliga 3
www.frontiersin.org  

Faʻaaliga 3. The Grand average on P3, Pz, and P4; the topography of P3 in all four conditions (the time window of the topography was corresponding to the gray square covered area).

 
 
Faʻaaliga 4
www.frontiersin.org  

Faʻaaliga 4. Interactions of picture × group on N2 (A) ma le P (B) (***p <0.001; **p < 0.01; n.s., not significant).

 
 
TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org  

TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics for event-related potential (ERP) data.

 
 

P3. The main effect of picture was significant [F(1,30) = 17.668, p <0.001, ηp2 = 0.3371]. Painful pictures elicited significantly larger amplitudes than the non-painful pictures (7.350 ± 0.799 μV and 5.998 ± 0.679 μV). The main effect of group was not significant [F(1,30) = 0.989, p = 0.328, η2p

= 0.032]. The interaction of group × picture was significant [F(1,30) = 6.283, p = 0.018, η2p

= 0.173]. Pairwise comparisons showed that the difference between the painful and the non-painful pictures was only significant in the HC group (8.473 ± 1.130 μV and 6.316 ± 0.961 μV, i < 0.001) but not in the IAD group (6.227 ± 1.130 μV and 5.681 ± 0.961 μV, i = 0.240) (Figures 3, 4B ma le Laulau 2).

LPP. The main effect of picture was significant [F(1,30) = 22.517, p <0.001, η2p

= 0.429]. Painful pictures elicited significantly larger amplitudes than non-painful pictures (7.469 ± 0.761 μV and 5.787 ± 0.674 μV). The main effect of group [F(1,30) = 1.128, p = 0.297, η2p = 0.036] and the interaction of picture × group [F(1,30) = 2.055, p = 0.162, η2p

= 0.064] were not significant.

Subjective Reports and Their Correlations with Neural Activity

Results of the correlation analyses showed that the difference of N2 (painful and non-painful) was significantly correlated with the scores of the “PD” of the IRI [r (30) = -0.407, p = 0.021] (Figure 5).

 
Faʻaaliga 5
www.frontiersin.org  

Faʻaaliga 5. Correlation between amplitudes of N2 (painful > non-painful) and PD scores.

 
 

Talanoaga

The present study explored the neural underpinnings of empathic responses to other’s pain in the IADs. The IAD group was found to be less discriminative than the HC group to other’s pain in both of the early automatic and of the later cognitive controlled processing stages, supporting by the ERP data. These results are consistent with the suggestion that IAD is associated with empathy deficit (Melchers et al., 2015).

It should be noticed that in the literature of ERP studies focusing on empathy for pain, there were studies that reported a positive shift of the painful condition comparing to the non-painful condition (Fan and Han, 2008; Sheng and Han, 2012). There were other studies reported an insignificant result in the early components, and the positive shift was only observed in the later components such as P3 and LPP (Meng et al., 2013). Besides, there are also studies reported a more negative shift in the early components and a more positive shift in the later components (Cui et al., 2016a,b). This inconsistency implies that only using the amplitudes of ERP components to indicate the neural responses was unstable. We proposed to use the discrimination between the painful and non-painful stimuli to indicate how well the stimuli were processed. If the painful and non-painful stimuli were differentiated under one condition but not in the other condition, we can say the stimuli was better processed in the former one. This logic has been applied in the literature (Ibanez et al., 2011; Cui et al., 2016a,b).

The early component N1 has been shown to discriminate painful from non-painful stimuli and has been described as an index of automatic activation of affective arousal (Lyu et al., 2014). Some studies have reported that observing other’s pain induced a more positive N1 component than the non-painful stimuli (Fan and Han, 2008; Han et al., 2008; Decety et al., 2010; Ibanez et al., 2011), whereas others reported no effect of observing others’ pain on N1 amplitude (Mella et al., 2012; Lyu et al., 2014). This inconsistency across studies may be due to methodological differences, such as different sets of stimuli. However, these inconsistent findings also suggest the effect of the pictures on N1 was not stable and can be easily influenced by contextual factors. In the current study, we did not find significant differences in N1 in response to viewing painful versus non-painful pictures in either the IAD or the HC group.

The N2 component has been suggested to reflect the early automatic sensitivity to other’s pain (Chen et al., 2012). The amplitude of N2 has been reported to correlate with the subjective ratings of affective empathy and Empathic Concern Scale scores (Sessa et al., 2014). Interestingly, we observed a significant group × picture interaction effect on N2, wherein a difference in painful versus non-painful image stimulus was observed in the HCs, but not in the IADs. This finding suggests that the individuals with IAD may have a reduced sensitivity to others’ pain, in terms of elicitation of affective arousal and emotional sharing.

In addition, we found that the difference of N2 evoked by painful and non-painful pictures was significantly correlated with the scores in the PD subscale of IRI. The larger the difference between painful and non-painful conditions was the higher PD score the participant had. The PD scale was designed to measure the discomfort generated in response to observing others in pain. Previous studies suggested that the automatic affective sharing with other’s emotional experience may lead to PD (Preston and de Waal, 2002; Gallese, 2003; Lamm et al., 2007). This significance correlation suggested that the discrimination between painful and non-painful stimuli in the time window of N2 reflected the level of discomfort induced by affective sharing with other’s pain. Besides, when comparing the IRI score between the two groups, the only significant difference was that the scores of PD: the HCs’ scores were significantly higher than the IADs’ scores. This result also supported that the affective sharing with other’s pain was different in the two groups.

We observed a similar group × picture interaction on the P3 component wherein greater P3 amplitude was triggered in response to viewing painful pictures than non-painful pictures only in the HC group, but not in the IAD group. P3 amplitude has been associated with motivational significance, arousal level, and the influence of these factors on mental resource allocation (Olofsson et al., 2008). Generally, highly salient, arousing, or motivational stimuli elicit larger P3 (Delplanque et al., 2004; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). It is found that P3 amplitudes elicited in the physicians were relatively insensitive to the distinction between painful and non-painful stimuli compared to other non-physician control participants, perhaps due to physician habituation (Decety et al., 2010). A similar P3 insensitivity in the IAD group suggested that individuals with IAD may allocate less attentional resources to the processing of other’s pain and may be less emotionally involved with other’s pain.

Besides, it was worthy to mention that the results presented here do not necessarily indicate a causal relationship between the empathic deficits and IAD. Does the addiction to internet result in the lack of empathy or the persons lacking empathy is more vulnerable to addiction? As suggested by a recent review and a few studies, empathy might have a protective function in resisting addictions (Massey et al., 2017). For example, one study found that a greater capacity to recognize facial expressions of sadness, anger, and fear in others were independently associated with a lower likelihood of smoking during pregnancy for women with a genetic predisposition to the sensitivity of social context (Massey et al., 2015). Children with a severe deficit in affective empathy may be at increased risk for early substance use (Frick and White, 2008; Swendsen et al., 2010). Moreover, in the IAD population, the number of males was significantly higher than that of females while females reported having a significantly higher level of empathy than males (Han et al., 2008; Jang and Ji, 2012; Becker et al., 2017). As such, the present study only determines the existence of empathic deficits in IADs but more longitudinal studies are needed to determine the causal relationship between empathy and IADs.

In conclusion, the current findings suggested that IADs showed reduced sensitivity to other’s pain. Specifically, the reduced painful versus non-painful image stimulus differences in N2 and P3 amplitude in IADs, relative to HCs, suggest that they have reduced affective arousal and emotional sharing as well as the allocation of attentional resources to others’ pain, respectively. These findings may help to elucidate the impaired social functioning observed in IADs.

tapulaa

One limitation of the present study was that there were no subjective measures of social deficits. Although the ERP index support that IADs are less discriminative than HCs to other’s pain, the lack of behavioral measurement weakens our argument. This lack of significance in behavioral data may due to the small sample sets (n = 16 in each group). More subjective measures of empathic ability or larger sample set should be collected in further researches. For example, instead of simply asking the participants to judge whether the picture is a painful one, we can ask them to rate how painful the person feels or how unpleasant other’s pain makes them feel. The correlations between these subjective measurements and ERP index can better associate the finding of neural activities to behavioral deficits.

Tusitala Tusitala

FC designed the experiment. CJ and TW collected and analyzed the data. CJ, TW, and XP wrote the main manuscript. FC and CJ prepared figures. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

faatupeina

This study was funded the National Natural Science Foundation of China: 31500877, 31600889, and the Outstanding Young Faculty Award of Guangdong Province: YQ2014149.

Feteʻenaʻiga o Tupulaga e Tetee ai

Fai mai tusitala o le suʻesuʻega sa faia i le leai o soʻo se fefaʻatauaʻiga faʻapisinisi pe tau tupe e mafai ona avea o se feteenaʻiga e ono aʻafia ai.

mau faasino

Bacigalupo, F., and Luck, S. J. (2015). The allocation of attention and working memory in visual crowding. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1180–1193. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Balconi, M., Venturella, I., and Finocchiaro, R. (2017). Evidences from rewarding system, FRN and P300 effect in internet-addiction in young people SHORT TITLE: rewarding system and EEG in internet-addiction. Brain Sci. 7:E81. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7070081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Becker, J. B., McClellan, M. L., and Reed, B. G. (2017). Sex differences, gender and addiction. J. Neurosci. Re. 95, 136–147. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Brand, M., Young, KS, ma Laier, C. (2014). Faʻamatalaga muamua ma le initaneti: o se faʻataʻitaʻiga masani ma le iloiloga o suʻesuʻega neuropsychological ma neuroimaging. I luma. Hum. Neurosci. 8: 375. doi: 10.3389 / fnhum.2014.00375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Chen, C., Yang, C. Y., and Cheng, Y. (2012). Sensorimotor resonance is an outcome but not a platform to anticipating harm to others. Soc. Neurosci. 7, 578-590. Pule: 10.1080 / 17470919.2012.686924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Cui, F., Ma, N., and Luo, Y. J. (2016a). Moral judgment modulates neural responses to the perception of other’s pain: an ERP study. Sci. Sui. 6: 20851. Pule: 10.1038 / srep20851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Cui, F., Zhu, X., Duan, F., and Luo, Y. (2016b). Instructions of cooperation and competition influence the neural responses to others’ pain: an ERP study. Soc. Neurosci. 11, 289-296. Pule: 10.1080 / 17470919.2015.1078258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Toma. 44, 113-126. Pule: 10.1037 / 0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Decety, J., and Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 3, 71-100. Pule: 10.1177 / 1534582304267187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Decety, J., and Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. ScientificWorldJournal 6, 1146–1163. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Decety, J., Yang, C. Y., and Cheng, Y. (2010). Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: an event-related brain potential study. Neuroimage 50, 1676-1682. doi: 10.1016 / j.neuroimage.2010.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Metotia 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Delplanque, S., Lavoie, M. E., Hot, P., Silvert, L., and Sequeira, H. (2004). Modulation of cognitive processing by emotional valence studied through event-related potentials in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 356, 1-4. Nu: 10.1016 / j.neulet.2003.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

D’Hondt, F., Billieux, J., and Maurage, P. (2015). Electrophysiological correlates of problematic Internet use: critical review and perspectives for future research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Faaa. 59, 64-82. doi: 10.1016 / j.neubiorev.2015.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

D’Hondt, F., and Maurage, P. (2015). Electrophysiological studies in Internet addiction: a review within the dual-process framework. Faʻaaoga. Faʻatasi. 64, 321-327. doi: 10.1016 / j.addbeh.2015.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Dong, G., Zhou, H., and Zhao, X. (2011). Male Internet addicts show impaired executive control ability: evidence from a color-word Stroop task. Neurosci. Lett. 499, 114-118. Nu: 10.1016 / j.neulet.2011.05.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Engelberg, E., and Sjoberg, L. (2004). Internet use, social skills, and adjustment. Cyberpsychol. Faʻatasi. 7, 41-47. Pule: 10.1089 / 109493104322820101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Fan, Y., and Han, S. (2008). Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: an event-related brain potential study. Neuropsychologia 46, 160-173. doi: 10.1016 / j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Frick, P. J., and White, S. F. (2008). Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J. Child Psychol. Fomaʻi 49, 359-375. Pule: 10.1111 / j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Gallese, V. (2003). The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Faʻataʻitaʻiga 36, 171-180. Pule: 10.1159 / 000072786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Grant, JE, Potenza, MN, Weinstein, A., ma Gorelick, DA (2010). Fa'atomuaga i vaisu tau amio. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 36, 233-241. Pule: 10.3109 / 00952990.2010.491884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Han, S., Fan, Y., and Mao, L. (2008). Gender difference in empathy for pain: an electrophysiological investigation. Brain Res. 1196, 85-93. doi: 10.1016 / j.brainres.2007.12.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

He, J. B., Liu, C. J., Guo, Y. Y., and Zhao, L. (2011). Deficits in early-stage face perception in excessive internet users. Cyberpsychol. Faʻatasi. Soc. Netw. 14, 303-308. Pule: 10.1089 / cyber.2009.0333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Hetu, S., Taschereau-Dumouchel, V., and Jackson, P. L. (2012). Stimulating the brain to study social interactions and empathy. Brain Stimul. 5, 95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Ibanez, A., Hurtado, E., Lobos, A., Escobar, J., Trujillo, N., Baez, S., et al. (2011). Subliminal presentation of other faces (but not own face) primes behavioral and evoked cortical processing of empathy for pain. Brain Res. 1398, 72-85. doi: 10.1016 / j.brainres.2011.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Jang, M. H., and Ji, E. S. (2012). Gender differences in associations between parental problem drinking and early adolescents’ internet addiction. J. Spec. Pediatr. Nurs. 17, 288-300. Pule: 10.1111 / j.1744-6155.2012.00344.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Jung, T. P., Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2001). Analysis and visualization of single-trial event-related potentials. Hum. Brain Mapp. 14, 166-185. Nu: 10.1002 / hbm.1050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Kupferberg, A., Bicks, L., and Hasler, G. (2016). Social functioning in major depressive disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Faaa. 69, 313-332. doi: 10.1016 / j.neubiorev.2016.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Kuss, DJ, ma Lopez-Fernandez, O. (2016). Initaneti vaisu ma faʻafitauli faʻaoga Initaneti: o se iloiloga faʻapitoa o suʻesuʻega falemaʻi. Lalolagi J. Psychiatry 6, 143–176. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i1.143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Lai, C. M., Mak, K. K., Watanabe, H., Jeong, J., Kim, D., Bahar, N., et al. (2015). The mediating role of internet addiction in depression, social anxiety, and psychosocial well-being among adolescents in six Asian countries: a structural equation modelling approach. Malo le Soifua Maloloina 129, 1224–1236. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2015.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Lamm, C., Nusbaum, H. C., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2007). What are you feeling? Using functional magnetic resonance imaging to assess the modulation of sensory and affective responses during empathy for pain. FUAFUA ONA 2: e1292. doi: 10.1371 / journal.pone.0001292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Luck, S. J., and Gaspelin, N. (2017). How to get statistically significant effects in any ERP experiment (and why you shouldn’t). Psychophysiology 54, 146-157. doi: 10.1111 / psyp.12639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Lyu, Z., Meng, J., and Jackson, T. (2014). Effects of cause of pain on the processing of pain in others: an ERP study. Faʻaaliga. Brain Res. 232, 2731–2739. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3952-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Massey, S. H., Estabrook, R., O’Brien, T. C., Pine, D. S., Burns, J. L., Jacob, S., et al. (2015). Preliminary evidence for the interaction of the oxytocin receptor gene (oxtr) and face processing in differentiating prenatal smoking patterns. Neurosci. Lett. 584, 259-264. Nu: 10.1016 / j.neulet.2014.10.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Massey, S. H., Newmark, R. L., and Wakschlag, L. S. (2017). Explicating the role of empathic processes in substance use disorders: a conceptual framework and research agenda. Fualaau Faʻamaina Faʻaʻona. doi: 10.1111/dar.12548 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Melchers, M., Li, M., Chen, Y., Zhang, W., ma Montag, C. (2015). O le faʻaitiitia o lagona alofa e fesoʻotaʻi ma le faʻafitauli faʻafitauli o le Initaneti: faʻamaoniga faʻamaoniga mai Saina ma Siamani. Asia J. Fomaʻi. 17, 56-60. doi: 10.1016 / j.ajp.2015.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Mella, N., Studer, J., Gilet, A. L., and Labouvie-Vief, G. (2012). Empathy for pain from adolescence through adulthood: an event-related brain potential study. I luma. Toma. 3: 501. Pule: 10.3389 / fpsyg.2012.00501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Meng, J., Hu, L., Shen, L., Yang, Z., Chen, H., Huang, X., et al. (2012). Emotional primes modulate the responses to others’ pain: an ERP study. Faʻaaliga. Brain Res. 220, 277–286. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3136-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Meng, J., Jackson, T., Chen, H., Hu, L., Yang, Z., Su, Y., et al. (2013). Pain perception in the self and observation of others: an ERP investigation. Neuroimage 72, 164-173. doi: 10.1016 / j.neuroimage.2013.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Negd, M., Mallan, K. M., and Lipp, O. V. (2011). The role of anxiety and perspective-taking strategy on affective empathic responses. Faʻatasi. Res. Ther. 49, 852–857. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Nieuwenhuis, S., Aston-Jones, G., and Cohen, J. D. (2005). Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. Toma. Bull. 131, 510-532. Pule: 10.1037 / 0033-2909.131.4.510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Olofsson, J. K., Nordin, S., Sequeira, H., and Polich, J. (2008). Affective picture processing: an integrative review of ERP findings. Biol. Toma. 77, 247-265. Pule: 10.1016 / j.biopsycho.2007.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Preston, S. D., and de Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Faʻatasi. Brain Sci. 25, 1-20.

Scholar Google

Rutgen, M., Seidel, E. M., Riecansky, I., and Lamm, C. (2015). Reduction of empathy for pain by placebo analgesia suggests functional equivalence of empathy and first-hand emotion experience. J. Neurosci. 35, 8938-8947. Nu: 10.1523 / JNEUROSCI.3936-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Sanders, C., Field, T. M., Diego, M., and Kaplan, M. (2000). The relationship of internet use to depression and social isolation among adolescents. Tupulaga Talavou 35, 237-242

Scholar Google

Sessa, P., Meconi, F., Castelli, L., and Dell’Acqua, R. (2014). Taking one’s time in feeling other-race pain: an event-related potential investigation on the time-course of cross-racial empathy. Soc. Cogn. Afaina. Neurosci. 9, 454–463. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Sheng, F., and Han, S. (2012). Manipulations of cognitive strategies and intergroup relationships reduce the racial bias in empathic neural responses. Neuroimage 61, 786-797. doi: 10.1016 / j.neuroimage.2012.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Swendsen, J., Conway, K. P., Degenhardt, L., Glantz, M., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., et al. (2010). Mental disorders as risk factors for substance use, abuse and dependence: results from the 10-year follow-up of the National Comorbidity Survey. onāga i vailāʻau 105, 1117-1128. Pule: 10.1111 / j.1360-0443.2010.02902.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Tam, P., and Walter, G. (2013). Problematic internet use in childhood and youth: evolution of a 21st century affliction. Australas. Psychiatry 21, 533-536. Pule: 10.1177 / 1039856213509911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Wang, T., Ge, Y., Zhang, J., Liu, J., and Luo, W. (2014). The capacity for pain empathy among urban Internet-addicted left-behind children in China: an event-related potential study. Faʻatauga. Hum. Faʻatasi. 33, 56-62. doi: 10.1016 / j.chb.2013.12.020

CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Wei, H. T., Chen, M. H., Huang, P. C., and Bai, Y. M. (2012). The association between online gaming, social phobia, and depression: an internet survey. BMC Psychiatry 12:92. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-92

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Yen, J. Y., Yen, C. F., Chen, C. C., Chen, S. H., and Ko, C. H. (2007). Family factors of internet addiction and substance use experience in Taiwanese adolescents. Cyberpsychol. Faʻatasi. 10, 323-329. Pule: 10.1089 / cpb.2006.9948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Young, KS (1998a). Caught in the Net: How to Recognize the Signs of Internet Addiction – And a Winning Strategy for Recovery. New York, NY: Wiley.

Scholar Google

Young, KS (1998b). Initaneti vaisu: o le tulaʻi mai o se faʻamaʻi faʻamaʻi fou. Cyberpsychol. Faʻatasi. 1, 237–244. doi: 10.1007/s10899-011-9287-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Zung, W. W. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Faʻau. Gen. Psychiatry 12, 63-70. Pule: 10.1001 / archpsyc.1965.01720310065008

CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

Zung, W. W. (1971). A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Fomaʻi mafaufau 12, 371–379. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(71)71479-0

CrossRef Full Text | Scholar Google

 

Keywords: internet addiction disorder (IAD), empathy, ERPs, N2, P3

Citation: Jiao C, Wang T, Peng X and Cui F (2017) Impaired Empathy Processing in Individuals with Internet Addiction Disorder: An Event-Related Potential Study. I luma. Hum. Neurosci. 11: 498. doi: 10.3389 / fnhum.2017.00498

Received: 07 August 2017; Accepted: 27 September 2017;
Fa'asalalau: 10 Oketopa 2017.

Faatonutonu e:

Alessio Avenanti, Università di Bologna, Italy

Toe iloiloina e:

Ruolei Gu, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences (UCAS), China
Markus Rütgen, University of Vienna, Austria

Copyright © 2017 Jiao, Wang, Peng and Cui. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Laisene Creative Commons (CC BY). O le faʻaaogaina, tufatufa poʻo le toe gaosia i isi fonotaga e faatagaina, pe'ā tuʻuina atu le uluaʻi tusitala poʻo le laiseneina ma o le uluaʻi lomiga i totonu o lenei tusi o tala o loʻo taʻua, e tusa ai ma aʻoaʻoga faʻapitoa taliaina. Leai se faʻaaogaina, tufatufa poʻo le toe gaosia e faʻatagaina lea e le tausisia nei tuutuuga.

*Correspondence: Fang Cui, [imeli puipuia]

O nei tusitala na saofagā tutusa i lenei galuega.