Telefoni feaveaʻi: Malamalama i le sootaga i le va o le faʻamalamalamaina o mafaufauga, mea tau vaisu ma telefoni feaveaʻi (2019)

Initaneti i Amio a le Tagata

Volume 90, Ianuari 2019, itulau 246-258

Andrew D.Presseyb

faatumutumuga

  • Togafitiga i masini atamai e sili atu nai lo mea ua fai ma vaisu i auaunaga tau fesoʻotaʻiga lautele (SNS).

  • O le tagofia o le atamai atamai e eseese e ala i le ausia o aʻoga; SNS e leai.

  • O tagata faʻapipiʻi faʻapitoa i smartphones ma SNS e sili atu le maualuga o le faʻamalamalamaina o le mafaufau.

  • O le aʻafiaga o le absorption o le sili atu mo SNS nai lo smartphones.

  • O le aʻafiaga o le faʻaaogaina o le mafaufau i luga o mea ua fai ma vaisu atamai ua faʻasalalau e vaisu i SNS.

lē faʻatino

The purpose of the present study is to examine the differences between user addiction to telefonipoto devices versus addiction to social network services (SNS), and the role of user perceptions. While a growing corpus of work has demonstrated the potentially deleterious effects of smartphone usage, relatively few studies have differentiated between addiction to the device versus addiction to social network services or measured the influence of user perceptions on smartphone addiction. To contribute to knowledge on this subject, the present study had three key aims. The first was to examine the differences between smartphone addiction and social network services addiction. The second aim was to understand the influence of user perceptions on addiction (measured through cognitive absorption to examine users’ state of involvement and engagement with software and technology). Our final aim was to examine differences for demographic factors for smartphone and social networking services addiction and user perceptions. Based on a survey of pisinisi students at a university in the Mid-Atlantic region of United States, the results showed that addiction to smartphone devices is greater than addiction to social networking services and varies by aoga ausia, while social networking services usage does not vary by gender, age or education. Further, users addicted to smartphones and social networking services experience higher levels of cognitive absorption, particularly by females when using social networking services and greater for social networking services than smartphones. Finally, we find that the impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction is mediated by addiction to SNS services.

    uputatala

    Mobile ubiquity
    Technology addiction
    Problematic smartphone usage
    User perceptions
    Fa'ailoga tagata

    1. faʻatomuaga

    telefoni poto are ubiquitous in modern society; evidence suggests that there were 3.9 billion smartphones globally in 2016, which is estimated to rise to 6.8 billion by 2022 (Ericsson, 2017). Smartphone technology, however, is a prime example of what Mick and Fournier (1998) refer to as a ‘paradox of technology’, which can be both emancipating and enslaving simultaneously. Smartphones afford us the liberty to communicate, socialize and search for information in ways almost unthinkable two decades ago; smartphone technology can also result in user dependency and deleterious user outcomes and behaviors.

    Traditionally the Internet has been the chief subject of focus for studies of technology addiction and problematic behavioral outcomes (De-Sola Gutiérrez, Rodríguez de Fonseca, & Rubio, 2016). In recent years, however, cell phone technology – and particularly the advent of the smartphone – has begun to supplant the Internet as a potential source of amioga ua fai ma vaisu (Lane & Manner, 2011; Lin et al., 2015). Further, smartphone addiction may arguably be more important to study than problematic Internet use as smartphones offer a mobile komepiuta tulaga (with Web browsers and GPS navigational services) with greater portability than other computing devices such as laptops and tablets and addiction hence may be more acute (Demirci, Orhan, Demirdas, Akpinar, & Sert, 2014; Jeong, Kim, Yum, & Hwang, 2016; Kwon, Kim, Cho, & Yang, 2013).

    A debate that is currently emerging in the literature is the distinction between addiction to a device versus addiction to applications and contents, and the relationship between the two (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. 2016), reminiscent of earlier felafolafoaiga concerning the Internet (Griffiths, 1999). While a number of studies have examined smartphone addiction, very few have considered addiction to the device versus addiction to certain services, particularly social network services (SNS), which provide tulaga i luga ole laiga mo fausia sootaga based on shared personal dimensions. While a small corpus of scholarship has examined addiction to various types of content (e.g. tala, entertainment, social networking) (Bian & Leung, 2015; Rosen, Whaling, Carrier, Cheever, & Rokkum, 2013; van Deursen, Bolle, Hegner, & Kommers, 2015), with the exception of Jeong et al. (2016), no prior study has compared different types of content in any detail, or, further, differentiated between addiction to the device versus addiction to particular applications. This subtle difference is important as it helps us to better comprehend smartphone addiction, particularly as certain cell phone activities may be more significantly associated with addiction than others (Roberts, Yaya, & Manolis, 2014).

    In a departure from previous empirical studies, the present study examines the two different arguments beginning to appear in the literature, namely addiction to the device versus addiction to SNS, in a single study. Further, we examine users’ levels of cognitive absorption – their state of involvement and engagement with software and technology – with smartphone and SNS addiction, to help understand the role of user perceptions in computer-mediated environments. Finally, we consider the potential influence of demographic factors on smartphone and SNS addiction.

    le mamanu suesuega was based on a single cross-sectional sample and a lipoti faaletagata lava ia survey. Scales were adopted from previous studies, but were adapted and extended for the context of this study. The survey was implemented online and distributed to pisinisi students at a university in the Mid-Atlantic region of the United States. Hypothesis testing was conducted via t-tests, analysis of variance (ANOVA), regression, and a Sobel test.

    The study is structured in the following way. Following this introduction, we consider the subject of technology addiction and research examining problematic smartphone usage. Next we examine the subject of user perceptions through the concept of cognitive absorption. We then turn to the development of a series of hypotheses. The remainder of the study empirically examines the hypothesis based on data obtained via a survey, including a discussion, conclusions and implications of the study’s findings.

    1.1. Definitions and literature review

    The aims of this study are threefold: to examine the differences between smartphone addiction and SNS addiction; to understand the influence of user perceptions on addiction (measured through cognitive absorption to examine users’ state of involvement and engagement with software and technology); and to examine differences for demographic factors for smartphone and SNS addiction and user perceptions. This section explores the background literature on these topics, focusing on technology addiction, problematic smartphone usage and cognitive absorption.

    1.2. Technology addiction

    Merriam-Webster’s Medical Dictionary (1995: 273) defines addiction as “… an acquired mode of behavior that has become nearly or completely involuntary,” while the Gale Encyclopedia of Medicine (1999) considers addiction as “… a dependence, on a behavior or substance that a person is powerless to stop.” Traditionally, addiction was regarded as relating only to substances (such as alcohol and drugs), but latterly was broadened to include amioga faʻafitauli (including excessive sexual intercourse and pathological gambling). Further, some have argued that any uncontrollable or overused behavior or activity should be regarded as an addiction (Peele, 1985).

    The American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mafaufauga Faalemafaufau (DSM), currently in its fifth edition (DSM-V, 2013), captures commonly agreed upon mental conditions. Clinicians have deliberated at some length on the possible existence of technology addiction, although the DSM does not currently recognize it as a condition, maintaining instead that it manifests as a consequence of other preceding mental conditions, such as reduced manaʻoga faʻaosooso (Yellowlees & Marks, 2007). This said, however, addictions to various facets of technology have attracted some research attention across a broad range of scholarly disciplines for some time, and there have been calls for its formal recognition (Pupuni, 2008).

    I le anotusi o le faʻamatalaga faʻamatalaga aʻoaʻiga, Carillo, Scornavacca, & Za, 2017 points out that psychological dependency (addiction) to faamatalaga ma fesoʻotaʻiga fesoʻotaiga tatau ona le mautonu ma taulaʻi-sini dependency. While the two concepts may be related and may influence individuals’ reasoned IT usage decisions, goal-oriented dependency captures the extent to which an individual’s capacity to reach his or her objectives depends on the use of specific technology. It also tends to focus on the more positive consequences of the use of technology. On the other hand, addiction tends to focus on the more negative effects of technology use as it relates to a tulaga o le mafaufau of maladaptive dependency on the use of a technology to such a degree that typical obsessive-compulsive behavioral symptoms arise. This paper focuses on this facet of the phenomenon.

    A growing body of research has pointed to the presence of addiction to several forms of information technology (Barnes & Pressey, 2014; Carillo et al. 2017; Griffiths, 2001; Lin, 2004; Turel, Serenko, & Giles, 2011; Turel & Serenko, 2010). Turel et al. (2011) report that neurobehavioral support has been offered for the existence of behavioral addictions, including technology addictions, and points to the similarities between substance and behavioral addictions (Helmuth, 2001). One study employing faʻataʻitaʻiga faʻasolosolo mata in online gaming found that urge/craving in substance addiction and urge/craving in online gaming addiction have analogous faiga neurobiological (Ko et al. 2009). O lea Turel et al. (2011, p. 1045) conclude that “It is, therefore, reasonable to apply concepts, models and theories from the substance addiction area to the fairly new field of behavioral addictions”.

    Studies examining the problematic usage of technology have a considerable lineage; for example, Hadley Cantril and Gordon W. Allport questioned the potentially addictive nature of radio programs in their text The Psychology of Radio lomia i totonu 1935. Later scholarship addressed dependencies on certain technologies such as excessive television viewing (Horvath, 2004; Mcllwraith, 1998), excessive video game playing (Keepers, 1990), ‘computer addiction’ (Shotton, 1991), and the addictive potential of the Internet (Brenner, 1997; Griffiths, 1996, 1997; Talavou, 1998), with the latter topic having attracted significant empirical attention (Bozoglan, Demirer, & Sahin, 2014; Bridges & Florsheim, 2008; Charlton & Danforth, 2007; Demirer & Bozoglan, 2016; Kuss, van Rooij, Shorter, Griffiths, & van de Mheen, 2013; Lehenbauer-Baum et al. 2015; Morahan-Martin & Schumacher, 2000; Pontes & Griffiths, 2016; Turel et al. 2011). O se vaega itiiti o Initaneti i vaisu research has also examined specific gaioiga i luga ole laiga, including addiction to faʻatau tuʻi i luga ole laiga (Turel et al. 2011) and virtual worlds (Barnes & Pressey, 2014). One natural extension of this line of scholarly enquiry that has received scholarly attention is problematic smartphone usage.

    1.3. Problematic smartphone usage

    The first study to empirically examine mobile phone addiction is attributed to a master’s thesis (Jang, 2002), conducted in South Korea. Various facets of smartphone addiction have been examined and published in recent years (see Laulau 1 below), with an emphasis on the drivers of problematic smartphone usage. Smartphone addiction may arguably be more important to study than Internet or computer addiction as smartphones offer a mobile computing platform and thus offer greater portability than other computing devices such as laptops and tablets, and addiction may be more acute (Demirci et al. 2014; Jeong et al. 2016; Kwon et al., 2013), resulting in the habitual checking of a device (Lee, 2015; Oulasvirta, Rattenbury, Ma, & Raita, 2012). Some commentators have speculated that smartphones may represent the preeminent technological device encouraging addiction for our time (Shambare, Rugimbana, & Zhowa, 2012).

    Laulau 1. The growing corpus of suesuega taua suʻesuʻeina telefoni feavea'i vaisu.

    Tusitala (s)taulaiAcademic Field
    Lin et al. (2017)Diagnosing smartphone addiction through application (app)-recorded data.Clinical psychiatry
    Jeong et al. (2016)Users’ psychological characteristics and the content types that are used.Suesuega i komepiuta
    Sapacz, Rockman, and Clark (2016)Personality and problematic mobile phone use.Suesuega i komepiuta
    Samaha and Hawi (2016)Relationships among smartphone addiction, stress, academic performance, and satisfaction with life.Suesuega i komepiuta
    Cho and Lee (2015)The experiences of student nurses in terms of distractions by smartphones in clinical settings and their opinions regarding use policies for smartphones.Medical (nursing) informatics
    Jeong and Lee (2015)Smartphone addiction and empathy among nursing students.Scientific, technical and medical studies
    Al-Barashdi, Bouazza, and Jabur (2015)Smartphone addiction among undergraduate university students.Saienisi aoao
    Kibona and Mgaya (2015)Smartphone addiction and academic performance.Inisinia ma tekonolosi
    Pearson and Hussain (2015)Smartphone use, addiction, narcissism and personality.Cyber-psychology and learning
    Wang, Wang, Gaskin, and Wang (2015)The role of stress and motivation in problematic smartphone use among college students.Suesuega i komepiuta
    Demirci, Akgongul, and Akpinar (2015)Smartphone use severity, sleep quality, depression and anxiety.Faʻafeusuaiga faʻaleaga
    van Deursen et al. (2015)Smartphone usage types, emotional intelligence, social stress and self-regulation.Suesuega i komepiuta
    Bernroider, Krumay, and Margiol (2014)Impact of smartphone addiction on smartphone usage.Faʻamaumauga
    Bian and Leung (2014)The relationship between loneliness, shyness, smartphone addiction symptoms and social capital.Suesuega i komepiuta
    Davey and Davey (2014)Smartphone addiction among Indian adolescents.Preventative medicine
    Demirci et al. (2014)Smartphone addiction among Turkish university students.Clinical psychopharmacology
    Kim, Lee, Lee, Nam, and Chung (2014)Smartphone addiction among Korean adolescents.Saienisi ma vailaʻau
    Lee, Chang, Lin, and Cheng (2014)Smartphone addiction management system.Suesuega i komepiuta
    Mok et al. (2014)Internet and smartphone addiction among Korean college students.Neuropsychiatry
    Park and Park (2014)Smartphone addiction in early childhood.Social sciences and humanity studies
    Zhang, Chen, and Lee (2014)Motives for smartphone addiction.Faʻamaumauga
    Shin and Dey (2013)Assessing problematic smartphone usage.software engineering and computer science (ubiquitous computing)
    Kwon et al. (2013)Smartphone addiction among Korean adolescents.Saienisi ma vailaʻau
    Kwon et al. (2013b)A self-diagnostic scale to determine smartphone addiction.Saienisi ma vailaʻau
    Takao, Takahashi, and Kitamura (2009)Personality and problematic mobile phone use.Cyber-psychology
    Ehrenberg, Juckes, White, and Walsh (2008)Personality, self-esteem and mobile phone addiction.Cyber-psychology
    Bianchi and Phillips (2005)Psychological predictors of problem mobile phone use.Cyber-psychology

    Collectively, these studies represent diverse academic fields including information systems, computer studies, healthcare, education, and psychology, among others. Only a handful of studies, however, have empirically examined the motives, drivers or user perceptions towards smartphone usage and addiction (Bian & Leung, 2014; Bianchi & Phillips, 2005; Ehrenberg et al. 2008; Jeong & Lee, 2015; Pearson & Hussain, 2015; Takao et al. 2009; van Deursen et al. 2015; Zhang et al. 2014). Of this subset of papers, user perceptions towards smartphone usage and addiction have been looked at from a standpoint of personality drivers (e.g. low self-esteem, neura, extraversion) (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005; Ehrenberg et al. 2008; Pearson & Hussain, 2015; Takao et al. 2009; Zhang et al. 2014), aafiaga taua (e.g. number of friends, taunuʻuga faʻaleaʻoaʻoga, and reading quantity) (Jeong & Lee, 2015), process and social orientation (e.g. smartphone usage types, lagona faalemafaufau, social stress, and self-regulation) (van Deursen et al. 2015), and hybrid studies (e.g. research examining uiga tagata and patterns of smartphone use) (Bian & Leung, 2014).

    The effect of user perceptions and the link to smartphone addiction is a pertinent area of enquiry as it relates to how users engage with technology and can become deeply immersed with it – sometimes to a problematic extent. Understanding user perceptions or beliefs are important as they influence user behavior, and help explain how users become absorbed with technology. Further, understanding what motivates users to uta certain beliefs helps us to understand why they hold those beliefs; while prior research on mobile phone addiction has focused heavily on usage and attitudes, less attention has been placed on belief formation. This is the subject to which we now turn, particularly through introducing the concept of cognitive absorption.

    1.4. Cognitive absorption

    While a number of theories help to illuminate user adoption and acceptance of information technologies – including diffusion of innovations theory, the talitonuga o amioga fuafuaina, theory of reasoned action, Ma le technology acceptance model (TAM) (Ajzen, 1985, 1991; Brancheau & Wetherbe, 1990; Davis, 1989; Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; Rogers, 1995) – they have limited power in explaining auala beliefs around information technologies are shaped (Agarwal & Karahanna, 2000). Agarwal and Karahanna (2000) introduced the concept of cognitive absorption (CA) in order to help overcome this conceptual shortfall. CA shares a conceptual root with some of the first major IT user-acceptance theories including TAM by emphasizing instrumentality as a core driver of user beliefs, and where usage behavior is motivated by “… cognitive complexity beliefs” (Agarwal & Karahanna, 2000, i. 666).

    CA also has the advantage of being grounded in a large corpus of scholarship in the cognitive and faalemafaufau mafaufauga literature, where CA draws its theoretical basis from three related strands of literature: the personality trait dimension of absorption (Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974; Tellegen, 1981, 1982), the state of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Trevino & Webster, 1992), and the notion of cognitive engagement (Webster & Hackley, 1997; Webster & Ho, 1997).

    Defined as “… a state of deep involvement with software” (Agarwal & Karahanna, 2000, p. 673), cognitive absorption can act as a powerful motivating factor towards beliefs related to IT, where highly engaging and engrossing experiences result in users’ ‘deep attention’ and complete immersion and engagement with an activity (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974; Vallerand, 1997).

    Agarwal and Karahanna (2000) proposed CA as a powerful motivating factor towards beliefs related to IT, where highly engaging and engrossing experiences result in ‘deep attention’. CA is driven by an faʻapitoa motivation (i.e. the enjoyment, satisfaction and pleasure as a result of an experience) as opposed to faʻamatalaina motivation (i.e. the expectation of a reward associated with a certain behavior). As “… an end in themselves” (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), intrinsic motivators have greater explanatory power in usage intentions than faaosofia extrinsic (Davis, Bagozzi, & Warshaw, 1992). Cognitive absorption is a multidimensional construct across five dimensions:

    i.

    Temporal dissociation (“the inability to register the passage of time while engaged in interaction”);

    ii.

    Focused immersion (“the experience of total engagement where other attentional demands are, in essence, ignored”);

    iii.

    Heightened enjoyment (“the pleasurable aspects of the interaction”);

    iv.

    Control (“the user’s perception of being in charge of the interaction”); and

    v.

    fia iloa (“the extent the experience arouses an individual’s sensory and cognitive curiosity”).

    We would expect that individuals with high levels of problematic smartphone and SNS service usage, or addiction, will experience higher levels of CA, as this provides some explanation of the deep state of involvement, engagement and attention that may be experienced by some individuals when interacting with computer-mediated environments, which may foster problematic behaviors among some users. Thus, addicted users will most likely have some form of perceptual distortion.

    There is some evidence to support this assertion. The relationship between addiction and perceptual distortion may result in higher levels of CA, particularly as addiction may produce a aafiaga faʻavasega that results in users perceiving websites more positively than non-addicted users (Barnes & Pressey, 2017; Turel et al. 2011). Addiction results in the faʻafetauiga of cognitive processes and the intensification of a particular experience. Hence, users who exhibit higher levels of addiction hold positive perceptions of a system (even if such perceptions are illogical), thus resulting in higher levels of absorption in a system. For example, Turel et al. (2011) found evidence that users with an addiction to online auctions reported higher levels of perceived usefulness, enjoyment and ease of use of an auction site, while Barnes and Pressey (2017) report that addiction to virtual worlds has a positive impact on cognitive absorption.

    In sum, examining the relationship between CA and addiction affords us the capacity to understand the routes via which behaviors concerning technology are manifested and what drives individuals to harbor particular beliefs concerning IT, and “serves as a key antecedent to salient beliefs about an information technology” (Agarwal & Karahanna, 2000, p. 666). This would seem both valuable and timely given the ubiquity of smartphone technology and reports of problematic usage, and would help us to understand why some tagata faʻaoga iloa a deeper state of involvement with a particular technology than others. In the following section we outline our hypotheses related to addiction to smartphone technology.

    2. Hypothesis development

    This section is organized into six areas. Initially, we examine telefonipoto addiction versus addiction to SNS, and this followed by the impact of cognitive absorption on addiction, and the demographic factors related to smartphone addiction. Next, we consider the impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction, and finally the impact of cognitive absorption by gender, age and education.

    2.1. Smartphone addiction versus addiction to SNS

    The majority of studies examining problematic technology usage focus on the apparatus or device itself (radio, TV, computer, cell phone), rather than the content the technology provides (a particular program, software, website, or application). As Roberts and Pirog (2012: 308) note: “research must dig beneath the technology being used to the activities that draw the user to the particular technology.” There is a current debate in the literature as to whether addiction is to the phone or to the services provided upon it; as Pearson and Hussain (2015, p. 19) have observed: “With so many addictive applications available on the smartphone, it is difficult to decipher the mafua ma le aoga relationship of problematic use. The smartphones multi-faceted functionality may be addictive or it may be that users are addicted to a certain media.” Further, it may be a case of co-occurrence, where a user is addicted to both their smartphone and SNS sites.

    As De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. (2016: 2) note: “There is evidence that the smartphone, with its breadth of applications and uses, tends to induce greater abuse than regular cell phones” (see also Taneja, 2014). However, is it a case of an addiction to the device (e.g. smartphone) or addiction to contents and applications? This line of enquiry chimes with earlier discussions around addiction to the Internet where it is recognized that there is “a fundamental difference between addictions i Initaneti ma vaisu on the Internet” (Griffiths, 2012, p. 519). As Griffiths (2000) finau, gaioiga i luga ole laiga will differ in their capacity to be habit forming (see also Talavou, 1999).

    To-date, however, no study has distinguished between addiction to the smartphone or addiction to the activities they afford users or has integrated these two perspectives, despite being alluded to in earlier research. De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. (2016: 1) mention: “Research in this field has generally evolved from a global view of the cell phone as a device to its analysis via applications and contents.” Central to this debate – and germane to the present study – is whether the problem is the smartphone or its applications and contents (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. 2016).

    Roberts et al. (2014) assert that owing to the expanding variety of activities that can be performed on cell phones, it is crucial that we comprehend which activities are more likely to be habit forming than others. Before undertaking such research it would seem a pertinent starting point to establish if there is a difference in addiction to the smartphone versus addiction to a set of core applications (such as SNS). SNS use has been shown to be a strong predictor of smartphone addiction, and stronger than other favored smartphone uses such as gaming (Jeong et al. 2016), and is the basis for its inclusion in the present study. Is it a case of addiction to one important aspect of smartphone usage, SNS, or is it a case of addiction more broadly or globally? We maintain that addiction to the device more generally will be more acute than addiction to SNS more specifically; therefore no single application may be more addictive generally than the device in atoa. Consequently we propose the following:

    H1

    Addiction to the Smartphone will be greater than addiction to SNS.

    2.2. The impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone and SNS addiction

    We now consider the impact of cognitive absorption on addiction to SNS and smartphones separately. The most popular choice of app used on smartphones is widely reported as SNS, driven by the social relations they afford (Barkhuus & Polichar, 2011; Pearson & Hussain, 2015; Salehan & Negahban, 2013), and SNS use is reported as an antecedent of smartphone addiction (Salehan & Negahban, 2013). On this latter point, one explanation for the relationship between SNS availability and usage and smartphone adoption and addiction would be the level of cognitive absorption experienced by users and the highly engaging and engrossing experiences that result in users’ ‘deep attention’ and complete immersion (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974; Vallerand, 1997), particularly as SNS expand and grow in sophistication. Hence, we posit that cognitive absorption will be greater for SNS than smartphones, as one of the outcomes of high levels of cognitive absorption is an inability to self-regulate potentially harmful or damaging behaviors, particularly those driven through the popularity of SNS.

    H2

    The direct impact of cognitive absorption on addiction will be greater for SNS than smartphones.

    2.3. Demographic factors and smartphone addiction

    Younger people and females may be more at risk of smartphone addiction, while the evidence regarding the impact of education is more mixed. Initially, women tend to spend more daily time on their phones than males (Roberts et al., 2014), and there is compelling evidence to suggest that females experience a greater degree of cell phone dependence and problematic usage than males (Beranuy et al. 2009; Geser, 2006; Hakoama & Hakoyama, 2011; Jackson et al. 2008; Jenaro, Flores, Gomez-Vela, Gonzalez-Gil, & Caballo, 2007; Leung, 2008; Lopez-Fernandez, Honrubia-Serrano, & Freixa-Blanxart, 2012; Sanchez Martinez & Otero, 2009). Women (particularly those with low self-esteem) are deemed the most vulnerable group regarding smartphone addiction (Pedrero et al. 2012), and may rely more on smartphones than males in order to reduce social anxiety (Lee et al. 2014). Evidence also suggests that females send more text messages than their male counterparts and also tend to compose lengthier texts than males (Pawłowska & Potembska, 2012). Notwithstanding, some studies have found little or no differences in cell phone dependence by gender (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005; Junco, Merson, & Salter, 2010; Pearson & Hussain, 2015).

    There is evidence to suggest that males and females use their phones in distinct and different ways. Geser (2006, p. 3) asserts that “the motivations and goals of cell phone usage mirror rather conventional tulaga o alii ma tamaitai.” While females tend to favor the emotional and personal exchange that phones allow, and hence value the social functionality of a device more than males – particularly upega tafailagi fesoʻotaiga (such as Facebook) (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005; De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. 2016; Geser, 2006; Lenhart, Purcell, Smith, & Zickuhr, 2010) – males tend to value professional networking sites (such as LinkedIn) (Lenhart et al. 2010). Further, in their study of tamaiti aoga kolisi i le US, Roberts et al. (2014) found that females spent significantly more time on Facebook than their male colleagues, and that certain social networking sites were significant drivers of cell phone addiction. Hence we may argue that social motives are a prime driver in females’ use of smartphones.

    Younger people, particularly adolescents, are prone to compulsive phone usage, while the total time spent on mobile phones decreases with age (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. 2016), attributed to a reduced capacity for pulea e le tagata o ia lava (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005). Research has also pointed to the age one first obtains a cell phone and the increased likelihood of problematic usage in the future (Sahin, Ozdemir, Unsal, & Temiz, 2013).

    Evidence regarding the relationship between aoga ausia and problematic cell phone usage is equivocal. While some studies point to a link between problematic cell phone usage and level of education (particularly among those undertaking extended periods of study) (Tavakolizadeh, Atarodi, Ahmadpour, & Pourgheisar, 2014), others refute this (Billieux, 2012), and even report a link between low educational levels and problematic cell phone usage (Leung, 2007). Hence explanations that have been made concerning a link between problematic cell phone usage and educational attainment have not been particularly compelling.

    We hypothesize the following:

    H3

    SNS service addiction will vary by: (a) gender; (b) age; and (c) education.

    H4

    Smartphone addiction will vary by: (a) gender; (b) age; and (c) education.

    2.4. The impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction

    Since SNS can be considered as a subset of the functionality and services provided on smartphones, and a dominant aspect of time spent on the device, the impact of cognitive absorption on phone addiction per se should (logically) be mediated by SNS addiction. In other words, addiction to SNS (such as Facebook, Instagram, Pinterest, etc.) will act as a draw or enticement for overall addiction to the device and a conduit for processing cognitive avega.

    There is support for this relationship in the related literature. As noted earlier, by far the most popular choice of smartphone app is SNS (Barkhuus & Polichar, 2011; Pearson & Hussain, 2015; Salehan & Negahban, 2013), and evidence points to a positive relationship between mobile phone addiction and SNS. Therefore, the rapid rise in smartphone adoption and usage corresponds with the significant faʻateleina of SNS, where SNS use drives smartphone addiction (Salehan & Negahban, 2013). Hence, the greater the pull of SNS, the greater the overall level of addiction to the device. Given that we would anticipate the direct impact of cognitive absorption on addiction will be greater for SNS than smartphones, as argued earlier we would expect addiction to SNS to mediate the relationship between cognitive absorption and smartphone addiction. Thus, we posit:

    H5

    The impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction will be mediated by addiction to SNS.

    2.5. The impact of cognitive absorption by gender, age and education

    Some evidence suggests that different genders perceive and identify with technology in different ways. Females are thought to emphasize people-oriented and socially-driven usage motives, while males are regarded as more task-oriented (Claisse & Rowe, 1987). Pertinent to the present study, differences have been reported between male and female users for certain psychological traits and compulsive smartphone usage. Lee et al. (2014) found that three psychological traits (need for touch, nofoaga o faʻatonutonu, ma fegalegaleaiga lautele anxiety) differed between male and female smartphone users.

    As noted earlier, compared to males, females tend to favor the emotional and personal exchange that phones allow, and hence value the social functionality of a device, particularly social networking sites (Bianchi & Phillips, 2005; De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. 2016; Geser, 2006; Lenhart et al. 2010). Further, females tend to use smartphones for communal reasons, while males favor agentic purposes (Lenhart et al. 2010). Hence, could females experience a deeper state of engrossment (or cognitive absorption) with SNS than males?

    aʻo Agarwal and Karahanna’s (2000) study of cognitive absorption experienced via the Internet found no differences according to gender, their study was conducted in a pre-SNS world. If, as assumed earlier, female users will experience higher levels of SNS addiction than males due to their predilection for the personal and emotional exchange that smartphones allow and their social functionality, then we can posit that female users will experience greater levels of cognitive absorption than male users.

    In terms of age and education, there is no evidence to suggest that either factor plays a role in the levels of cognitive absorption experienced by users. We would therefore expect no differences in cognitive absorption by age or educational attainment.

    Given the above, we hypothesize:

    H6a

    The effect of cognitive absorption will be stronger for females than males when using SNS.

    H6b

    The effect of cognitive absorption when using SNS will not differ by age.

    H6c

    The effect of cognitive absorption when using SNS will not differ by educational attainment.

    2.6. Cognitive absorption and user addiction

    Our final series of hypotheses relate to user smartphone and SNS addiction and the levels of cognitive absorption experienced. As noted, we would anticipate that users experiencing high levels of problematic smartphone and SNS service usage, or addiction, will exhibit higher levels of CA. This would explain why some tagata faʻaoga iloa a deep state of involvement, engagement and attention when interacting with computer-mediated environments while other users do not. Hence a heightened degree of CA is driving addiction and fostering amioga faʻafitauli among some users, and acting as a form of perceptual distortion. Thus, we posit:

    H7

    Users with addiction to a smartphone will have higher levels of CA.

    H8

    Users with addiction to SNS will have higher levels of CA.

    3. Metotia faʻapitoa mo suʻesuʻega

    3.1. Research design

    le mamanu suesuega adopted involved a single cross-sectional convenience sample using a lipoti faaletagata lava ia survey. The study employed scales from previous research to measure the constructs in the study, although these were adapted and extended for the context of the study – social network applications and telefoni poto. The measure of cognitive absorption was adapted from Agarwal and Karahanna (2000) and comprises five factors: temporal dissociation (“the inability to register the passage of time while engaged in interaction”), focused immersion (“the experience of total engagement where other attentional demands are, in essence, ignored”), heightened enjoyment (“the pleasurable aspects of the interaction”), pule (“the user’s perception of being in charge of the interaction”), and fia iloa (“the extent the experience arouses an individual’s sensory and cognitive curiosity”). The wording of the cognitive absorption questions focused on the focal activity of “using upegafesootai faaagafesootai apps on my smartphone”. The measures for addiction to the smartphone ma addiction to social networking services were adapted from Charlton and Danforth (2007). The measure for each was identical in content but differed in terms of focus on “my smartphone” or “social networking apps”. Five items were included from Charlton and Danforth (2007), supplemented with a further two items to better fit the context of the study “I feel lost without [social networking apps/my smartphone]” and “I tend to get easily distracted by [social networking apps/my smartphone].” All construct items were measured on seven-point Fua faatatau Likert from 1 = Strong Disagree to 7 = Strongly Agree, where 4 = Neither Agree Nor Disagree. The scales items used in the survey are provided in the faʻaopoopoga. Demographic and background information was collected for gender, age, highest level of educational achievement, daily SNS usage, and daily smartphone usage.

    3.2. Data collection and analysis

    The survey was implemented online via Qualtrics, and was distributed to students studying pisinisi at a university in the Mid-Atlantic region of the United States in February 2015. A total of 140 valid responses were collected. The sample was 68.6% female and 31.4% male. A total of 75% of the sample were 34 years of age or under, whilst 42.9% had a Aʻoaʻoga Maualuga and 13.6% a Master degree.

    We examined the reliability of the measures using Cronbach’s Alpha; the scales for addiction to the smartphone and addiction to social networking apps had Cronbach’s Alphas of 0.835 and 0.890 respectively, well above the 0.7 threshold recommended by Nunnally (2010). The Cronbach’s Alpha for the cognitive absorption measure was 0.909, whilst those for its subcomponents ranged from 0.722 to 0.949, all of which are considered acceptable. Discriminant validity was also examined through variance inflation factors. Examining the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) for the measured variables in our study, we find that all VIF values are well below 10, ranging from 1.032 to 1.404, indicating that multicollinearity is not a problem (Hair, Black, Babin, & Anderson, 2014). Common method bias was examined using Harman’s single factor test. The first factor explained only 35% of the variance for the sample and thus common method bias did not appear to be present.

    In order to assess the levels of addiction for the ANOVA used for testing H7 ma H8, we created three addiction groups using a method similar to Morahan-Martin and Schumacher (2000). Our addiction groups were assessed in terms of the number of “active” symptoms from the seven-item addiction scale. For a symptom to be “active”, the Likert scale response should be in excess of the mid-point, 4 (Charlton & Danforth, 2007; Morahan-Martin & Schumacher, 2000). The three addiction groups were: no addiction (no symptoms), low addiction (one or two symptoms) and high addiction (three or more symptoms).

    O le tele o fuainumera fuainumera was conducted in SPSS 22. The hypothesis suʻega suʻega utilized a number of statistical procedures: t-tests, analysis of variance (ANOVA), regression and a Sobel test calculated using the method of Baron ma Kenny (1986).

    4. Iʻuga

    4.1. Smartphone addiction versus addiction to SNS

    Our first series of tests sought to identify any difference between user addiction to the telefonipoto and addiction to SNS by means of a paired samples t-test between the summary variables for smartphone addiction and SNS addiction (see Laulau 2). The results indicate that there is a eseʻesega taua between these two forms of addiction, with a mean difference of 3.44 and t-value of 7.303 (p < .001, Msmartphone_addiction = 25.43, MSNS_addiction = 21.99). Hence H1 – addiction to the smartphone will be greater than addiction to SNS – is supported.

    Laulau 2. Test for differences in addiction.

    EseUigaSDSEMean DifferenceSD (Difference)SE (Difference)tdfp
    Smartphone Use Addiction25.439.190.783.445.570.477.303139<.001
    SNS Use Addiction21.999.750.82

    4.2. The impact of cognitive absorption on addiction

    In order to examine the impact of cognitive absorption (CA) on the two forms of addiction, we ran two bivariate regression models: one examining the impact of CA on smartphone addiction and the other testing the impact of CA on SNS addiction. The results are shown in Laulau 3. As we can see, SNS use addiction is more strongly influenced by CA than smartphone use addiction, with a larger beta coefficients and a higher F-value (SNS use addiction: R2 = 0.254; F = 47.061; p < .001; β = 0.746, p < .001; smartphone use addiction: R2 = 0.240; F = 43.444, p < .001; β = 0.683, p < .001).

    Laulau 3. Regression models for SNS and telefonipoto vaisu.

    Independent VariableβSEβ (Std.)t-tauap
    Model 1. DV: SNS Use Addiction
    R2 = 0.254 (F = 47.061, p < .001, dftoʻatamaʻi = 1, dftoega = 138, dfAofa'i = 139)
    Cognitive Absorption.746.109.5046.860<.001
    Model 2. DV: Smartphone Use Addiction
    R2 = 0.240 (F = 43.444, p < .001, dftoʻatamaʻi = 1, dftoega = 138, dfAofa'i = 139)
    Cognitive Absorption.683.103.4906.599<.001

    To test whether the difference in the beta values is statistically significant we use the test of Paternoster, Brame, Mazerolle, and Piquero (1998) ma le fua lea:

    The result is Z = 1.766, which is significant at the 5% level, confirming that CA has a more taua aafiaga on SNS addiction than on smartphone addiction. Therefore, H2 – the direct impact of cognitive absorption on addiction will be greater for SNS than smartphones – is supported.

    4.3. Demographic factors and addiction

    We turn now to demographic facets of smartphone addiction. In order to test for differences according to gender, education and age, we use ANOVA to examine differences between groups for the summary addiction variables for both smartphones and SNS. In order to examine addiction by age group it was necessary to recode the older age groups into a single group for 35 + years due to inadequate data. We therefore had three age groups: 18–24 years, 25–34 years and 35 + years. Similarly, aoga ausia was recoded into three groups due to inadequate data: Aoga maualuga Graduate or Below, Aʻoaʻoga Maualuga or Equivalent, and Master Degree poʻo le tutusa.

    The ANOVA tests for SNS service addiction revealed no significant differences according to age (F = 1.368; p = .258), gender (F = 0.327, p = .568) or educational attainment (F = 1.488, p = .229). Hence, H3 – SNS service addiction will vary by: (a) gender; (b) age; and (c) education – is rejected.

    In terms of smartphone addiction and demographic factors, the ANOVA tests find significant differences between groups for educational attainment (F = 3.098, p = .048). A post-hoc test using the Bonferroni procedure found that the lowest educational group, High School Graduate or Below, had significantly higher addiction than Bachelor’s Degree or Equivalent (difference = 4.093, MAoga maualuga = 27.462, MBachelors = 23.333, p = .042). No significant differences are revealed according to gender (F = 0.102, p = .750) or age (F = 1.008, p = .368). Hence, H4 – smartphone addiction will vary by: (a) gender; (b) age; and (c) education – is partially accepted, with evidence for smartphone addiction differences for education.

    4.4. The impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction

    The mediating effect of addiction to SNS on the relationship between cognitive absorption and smartphone addiction was examined using the Sobel test (Baron & Kenny, 1986; Sobel, 1986). The results of this test are shown in Laulau 4, indicating that cognitive absorption is indeed significantly mediated by addiction to SNS (Z = 6.865, SE = 0.063, p < .001). This shows that cognitive absorption is amplified by addiction to SNS and carried through to addiction to the smartphone. Thus, H5 – the impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction will be mediated by addiction to SNS – is supported.

    Laulau 4. Test of the mediating effect of SNS use addiction.

    Relationship testedaSEabSEbSobelSEp
    Cognitive Absorption →
    SNS Use Addiction →
    Phone Use Addiction
    0.5730.0720.7600.0566.8650.063<.001

    Manatua: Path a: Cognitive Absorption → SNS Use Addiction; path b: SNS Use Addiction → Phone Use Addiction.

    4.5. The impact of cognitive absorption by gender, age and education

    Our next set of tests looked at CA and the differences users experienced by gender, age and education. In terms of gender, we find that females experience greater levels of CA than men when using SNS (MCA_Male = 4.517; MCA_Female = 4.925; see Laulau 5a). Assuming equality of variances, an independent samples t-test for differences in CA between males and females was found to be significant (mean difference = 0.408; t = 2.421; p = .017). However, further tests by CA subcomponent found that this difference was driven by time dissociation, the only element of CA that displays a significant difference for gender, with a mean difference of 0.735 (t = 2687; p = .008; see Laulau 5b). Thus, H6a – the effect of cognitive absorption will be stronger for females than males when using SNS – is supported.

    Laulau 5a. Gender and CA – faamatala fuainumera.

    EseitupaNUigaSDSE
    CAtane444.5170.8610.130
    fafine964.9250.9530.097
    FItane444.0460.8250.124
    fafine964.4131.1230.115
    TDtane444.8591.5670.236
    fafine965.5941.4720.150
    CUtane444.4661.4640.221
    fafine964.6251.4960.153
    COtane444.4321.0390.157
    fafine964.6461.1300.115
    HEtane444.7901.0540.159
    fafine965.1591.2220.125

    Laulau 5b. Independent samples T-Test for gender and CA.

    Levene’s Test for Equality of Variancest-test for Equality of Means
    FS.I.G.TdfSig. (2-tailed)Mean DifferenceStd. Error Difference
    CAEqual variances assumed.027.870-2.421138.017-O40795.16849
    Equal variances not assumed-2.51591.786.014-O40795.16220
    FIEqual variances assumed3.048.083-1.940138.054-O36705.18918
    Equal variances not assumed-2.170110.830.032-O36705.16911
    TDEqual variances assumed1.919.168-2.687138.008-O73466.27346
    Equal variances not assumed-2.62478.928.010-O73466.27999
    CUEqual variances assumed.000.991-O588138.557-O15909.27052
    Equal variances not assumed-O59385.141.555-O15909.26839
    COEqual variances assumed.913.341-1.066138.288-O21402.20072
    Equal variances not assumed-1.10090.264.274-O21402.19452
    HEEqual variances assumed1.238.268-1.730138.086-O36908.21334
    Equal variances not assumed-1.82895.834.071-O36908.20194

    In order to examine cognitive absorption by age group it was necessary to recode the older age groups into a single group for 35 + years due to inadequate data (as mentioned above). We therefore had three age groups: 18–24 years, 25–34 years and 35 + years. Equal variances were assumed for the ANOVA tests. The ANOVA tests revealed a small number of significant differences between age groups, namely for fia iloa and Control (F = 4.444, p = .013; and F = 5.008, p = .008 respectively). Post-hoc tests using the Bonferroni procedure found that for Curiosity, the 18–24 years age group is significantly greater than the 25–34 years age group (mean difference = 0.800, p = .016). For Control, the 35 + years age group was found to have levels significantly larger than the 25–34 years age group (mean difference = 0.731, p = .006). There were no other significant differences in the data. Thus, we find that H6b – the effect of cognitive absorption when using SNS will not differ by age – is partially supported.

    The ANOVA tests to ascertain differences in cognitive absorption according to educational attainment found no significant results. Therefore, H6c – the effect of cognitive absorption when using SNS will not differ by educational attainment – is supported.

    4.6. Cognitive absorption and user addiction

    Our final series of tests examine the relationship between level of addiction and the extent of CA experienced by users of smartphones and SNS. In order to conduct these tests we created groups of users with high, low and no addiction, as outlined in the methodology section.

    Our initial ANOVA test examined CA differences and smartphone addiction (see Laulau 6), finding that CA is significantly different by group for level of smartphone addiction (F = 19.572, p < .001). While the Control subcomponent of CA was not significant (F = 2.359, p = .98), all other subcomponents of CA were significant, the largest effect being that of time distortion (F = 35.229, p < .001), followed by focused immersion (F = 7.514, p = .001), curiosity (F = 5.255, p = .006), and heightened enjoyment (F = 4.484, p = .009). Overall, users lipotia high levels of smartphone addiction reported higher levels of CA than users reporting low levels of addiction, while users with low levels of smartphone addiction reported higher levels of CA than users with no level of smartphone addiction. Hence H7 – users with addiction to a smartphone will have higher levels of cognitive absorption – is supported.

    Laulau 6. Analysis of variance of cognitive absorption and phone use addiction.

    pei lava1. High addiction (n = 67)2. Low addiction (n = 47)3. No addiction (n = 26)P (F-value)ANOVA
    Cognitive Absorption5.154.763.94<.001 (19.572)1 > 2**, 1 > 3***, 2 > 3*b
    Focused Immersion4.584.243.69.001 (7.514)1 > 3**a
    pule4.774.484.26.098 (2.359)Non-sig.b
    Time Distortion6.065.333.62<.001 (35.229)1 > 2**, 1 > 3***, 2 > 3**b
    fia iloa4.874.603.79.006 (5.255)1 > 3**a
    Heightened Enjoyment5.235.084.43.009 (4.854)1 > 3*b

    Manatua: a Bonferroni; b Tamhane’s T2 used due to heteroscedasticity; ***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05; dfbetweengroups = 2, dfwithingroups = 137, dfAofa'i = 139.

    Next, we examined CA differences and users’ levels of SNS addiction (see Laulau 7). We find that CA is significantly different by addiction group (p < .001). Users reporting high levels of SNS addiction reported higher levels of CA than users with low levels of SNS addiction and users with no levels of SNS addiction. Again, the Control subcomponent of CA was not significant, neither was the subcomponent Curiosity. Given these results, H8 – users with addiction to SNS will have higher levels of cognitive absorption – is supported.

    Laulau 7. Analysis of variance of cognitive absorption and SNS use addiction.

    pei lava1. High addiction (n = 53)2. Low addiction (n = 37)3. No addiction (n = 50)P (F-value)ANOVA
    Cognitive Absorption5.254.824.35<.001 (13.902)1 > 2*, 1 > 3***b
    Focused Immersion4.724.253.94.001 (7.871)1 > 3***a
    pule4.854.404.45.092 (2.425)Non-sig.a
    Time Distortion6.145.554.50<.001 (18.777)1 > 2**, 1 > 3***b
    fia iloa4.914.584.26.080 (2.571)Non-sig.a
    Heightened Enjoyment5.385.184.63.004 (5.827)1 > 3***a

    Manatua: a Bonferroni; b Tamhane’s T2 used due to heteroscedasticity; ***p < .001; **p < .01; *p < .05 dfbetweengroups = 2, dfwithingroups = 137, dfAofa'i = 139.

    Laulau 8 provides a summary of the results of hypothesis testing. As we can see, of the ten hypotheses tested, the results of our study provide support for seven of them (H1, H2, H5, H6a, H6c, H7 ma H8). We also find partial support for two further hypotheses (H4 ma H6b); in particular, smartphone addiction was found to vary by education (supporting H4b), with addiction of high school respondents exceeding that of those with bachelor’s degrees, while cognitive absorption in SNS was found to differ between age groups for curiosity and control. One hypothesis is not supported by our data (H3).

    Laulau 8. Summary of hypothesis tests.

    Vaʻaigaiʻuga
    H1: Addiction to the smartphone will be greater than addiction to SNS.lagolagoina
    H2: The direct impact of cognitive absorption on addiction will be greater for SNS than smartphones.lagolagoina
    H3: SNS service addiction will vary by: (a) gender; (b) age; and (c) education.Le lagolagoina
    H4: Smartphone addiction will vary by: (a) gender; (b) age; and (c) education.Partially supported
    H5: The impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction will be mediated by addiction to SNS.lagolagoina
    H6a: The effect of cognitive absorption will be stronger for females than males when using SNS.lagolagoina
    H6b: The effect of cognitive absorption when using SNS will not differ by age.Partially supported
    H6c: The effect of cognitive absorption when using SNS will not differ by educational attainment.lagolagoina
    H7: Users with addiction to a smartphone will have higher levels of cognitive absorption.lagolagoina
    H8: Users with addiction to SNS will have higher levels of cognitive absorption.lagolagoina

    5. Findings and discussion

    The present paper contributes empirical evidence relating to addiction to telefoni poto versus addiction to upegafesootai faaagafesootai apps. While there are clearly related streams of research regarding addiction to a smartphone device and addiction to upega tafailagi fesoʻotaiga these are not fully integrated, although the issue is alluded to in recent studies (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. 2016; Jeong et al. 2016; Pearson & Hussain, 2015). No study to-date, however, has distinguished between addiction to smartphones versus addiction to the activities they afford users, or else attempted to integrate these two perspectives. We find that user addiction to smartphones is greater than addiction to SNS; hence there is presently a greater level of addiction to the device in general than to each of the services provided upon it.

    While we find that SNS service addiction does not vary by gender, age or education, we find that smartphone addiction does vary by education. Specifically we find that users with the lowest level of aoga ausia exhibited the highest levels of smartphone addiction. Why smartphone users with relatively lower levels of educational attainment should experience higher levels of addiction is not entirely clear; perhaps this group has a reduced capacity to self-regulate their compulsive smartphone usage.

    These findings emphasize the importance of a more nuanced understanding of smartphone addiction in future studies and theorizing on problematic smartphone usage, particularly as there are clearly crucial differences between addictions i smartphones versus addictions on smartphones. As Emanuel (2015) notes: we “are addicted to the information, entertainment, and personal connections [that a smartphone] delivers”, but clearly problematic usage will relate to the task being undertaken. As smartphones become ever more complex – as well as the ubiquity of the tasks they can perform now and in the future – we need to understand the differences in the nature of addictions between smartphones and SNS (as well as many other tasks being performed).

    A pertinent question to ask is how do addictions to smartphones and SNS form? Behavioral addictions (such as smartphone addiction) related to faʻaaogaina or dependence are use disorders that are driven by substance abuse (e.g. drugs, alcohol, tobacco), or in the case of smartphones, the ‘substance’ is the fesoʻotaʻiga lautele and entertainment services they provide. As a society, many people have an attraction to incessant entertainment and maintain social connections, and smartphones are arguably the most popular devices to achieve this. Yet despite how much mobile phones “… have transformed social practices and changed the way we do pisinisi … surprisingly we have little perception on their effect in our [lives]” (Katz & Akhus, 2002). This is surprising given the growing reports of respondents claiming to use a smartphone to avoid fegalegaleaiga lautele, or else habitually using their device in a social setting (Belardi, 2012; Merlo, Stone, & Bibbey, 2013). Connectivity has also become a key driver of agafesootai change; at an extreme level, however, being flooded by messages, texts, emails, and updates may cause us to depersonalize those around us, treating them as digital entities (Turkle, 2017).

    The habitual use of devices such as smartphones is also driven by the ‘fear of missing out’ (Baral, 2017). Habits are formed through a process of aʻoaʻoga faʻamalosi around certain behaviors that have previously rewarded us; smartphones are helping to ensure that users do not miss events or updates, thus reducing omiga lautele. As Elliot Berkman – professor of psychology at the University of Oregon – notes, “smartphones can be an escape from leai se ita because they are a window into many worlds other than the one right in front of you” (Baral, 2017). Suppressing the habitual use of smartphones for some users may result in anxiety and irritability. User perceptions will also play a role in driving addiction, which we consider in more detail next.

    The present study is the first extant study to emphasize the role of user perceptions (measured through level of cognitive absorption (Agarwal & Karahanna, 2000)) on smartphone addiction. We find that the direct impact of CA on addiction is greater for SNS than smartphones, probably owing to one of the outcomes of high levels of cognitive absorption – an inability to self-regulate potentially harmful or damaging behaviors, particularly those driven through the popularity of SNS. Further, we find that the impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction is mediated by addiction to SNS. In other words, addiction to SNS (such as Facebook, Instagram, Pinterest, etc.) will act as an inducement or enticement for overall addiction to the device and a conduit for processing cognitive absorption. Hence, the rapid rise in smartphone adoption and usage corresponds with the significant faʻateleina of SNS, where SNS use drives smartphone addiction.

    We find that users with addiction to a smartphone also exhibit higher levels of CA than users with low or no levels of smartphone addiction. Further, we also find that users with addiction to SNS have higher levels of CA. These findings provide strong evidence as to why some users become addicted while others do not exhibit problematic usage, although more research is needed to explore the drivers of user addiction to smartphone devices and SNS.

    Liliu i le demographic factors of cognitive absorption, we initially find that the effect of cognitive absorption is stronger for females than males when using SNS; the time dissociation subcomponent of CA was seen to be driving this, indicating that females experience an inability to register the passage of time while engaged in SNS usage compared to males. This manifests as time appearing to pass more quickly, losing track of time, and spending more time on social networking apps than intended. In terms of CA and age, some differences were observed but these were relatively minor and thus suggest no particularly clear findings. Finally, the effect of CA when using SNS does not differ by educational attainment.

    In sum, this paper makes three theoretical contributions. Initially, we address the ‘device versus content’ debate of smartphone addiction, thus, responding to recent calls to investigate this phenomenon (De-Sola Gutiérrez et al. 2016; Jeong et al. 2016; Pearson & Hussain, 2015). No prior study has compared different types of content in any detail, or, further, differentiated between addiction to a device versus addiction to particular applications; this subtle difference is important as it helps us to better comprehend smartphone addiction (Jeong et al. 2016). Secondly, we analyzed gender differences and smartphone addiction including user perceptions. Thirdly, the present study extends our understanding of cognitive absorption and user perceptions related to smartphone addiction. Collectively, the present study contributes to the dark side of mobile phone technology and user addiction, and the role of user perceptions in computer-mediated environments.

    Taken together, these findings emphasize the importance of taking into account the eseesega ono between any device with computer gaosi mana and its applications or contents. Future research on smartphone addiction and problematic smartphone usage therefore needs to be more nuanced and take into account these potentially important differences, particularly given the ubiquity of contemporary computing devices. Related to this is the role of user perceptions; user perceptions when engaging with smartphones may differ depending on the task being undertaken, indicating that future research should take this into account when investigating different aspects of problematic smartphone usage.

    6. Faaiuga

    As Rudi Volti (1995) has observed “[our] inability to understand technology and perceive its effects on our society and on ourselves is one of the greatest, if most subtle, problems of an age that has been so heavily influenced by suiga tekonolosi.” The paradox of telefonipoto technology is that it has the capacity of simultaneously liberating users and also subjugating them, which may result in problematic user behaviors and even addiction. As such, it would seem imperative to understand the effect that smartphone technology has on users and society, particularly the dark side of technology. This study extends the body of work on technology and smartphone addiction in a number of directions. The results of the present study demonstrate that there are eseesega eseese between addiction to smartphone devices and SNS in terms of user addiction; addiction to smartphone devices is greater than addiction to SNS (t = 7.303, p < .001), smartphone addiction varies by aoga ausia (F = 3.098, p = .048), while SNS usage does not vary by, gender, age or education. These results emphasize the importance of not limiting research to the study of users’ behavior with a device itself in isolation, but also to pay credence to its use and the particular activities undertaken. We also find important differences in user perceptions; users addicted to smartphones and SNS experience higher levels of cognitive absorption (F = 19.592, p < .001; and F = 13.902, p < .001 respectively), cognitive absorption is felt more strongly by females than males when using SNS (t = 2.421, p = .017), the impact of cognitive absorption is greater for SNS than smartphones (Z = 1.766, p = .039), and the impact of cognitive absorption on smartphone addiction is mediated by addiction to SNS (Z = 6.865, p < .001).

    6.1. Implications

    This study makes a number of contributions to theory, policy and practice. While many studies have examined aspects of smartphone addiction (see, for example, Bian & Leung, 2015; Rosen et al. 2013; van Deursen et al. 2015) few have differentiated between devices and applications in order to better understand problematic smartphone usage. Further, while earlier research has examined uiga tagata and compulsive smartphone usage (Lee et al. 2014; Wang et al. 2015), user perceptions have been overlooked. Measured through cognitive absorption, we find that users addicted to smartphones experience a deeper state of engagement and involvement – what might be described as a ‘cognitive corridor’. Therefore, to the best of our knowledge, this study is the first extant sailiga suʻesuʻe to draw a distinction between devices and applications in the context of problematic smartphone usage, as well as user perceptions.

    Se numera o policy implications can also be drawn. Initially, we must show caution in describing an activity as ‘addictive’, particularly smartphone usage, which may actually be due to broader issues (e.g. manaʻoga faʻaosooso disorders). This said, however, excessive smartphone usage may result in social withdrawal and damaged sootaga faaletagata lava ia. Similar to related debates around Initaneti i vaisu and its regulation (Barnes & Pressey, 2014), the regulation of smartphone usage is problematic and a subject of recent debate in the media, although beyond the remit of the present study. Whatever label may be ascribed to excessive use of smartphone technology – dependency, compulsive and habitual use, or addiction – it is a subject of concern in most atinaʻe atunuʻu, particularly given smartphone usage is almost entirely self-regulated. As devices and platforms become increasingly sophisticated, they have a greater capacity to encourage user engagement and involvement, which, in turn, can result in excessive usage. Further, research has demonstrated that compulsive smartphone usage may cause ‘technostress’ (Lee et al. 2014) – the inability to cope with new computing technologies (Brod, 1984), and users experiencing feelings of anxiety owing to communication and faʻamatalaga faʻamatalaga (Ragu-Nathan, Tarafdar, Ragu-Nathan, & Tu, 2008).

    The results of the present study also have faʻatinoga aoga. Smartphones have become an indispensable aspect of everyday life for many people, and whilst this technology affords the capacity to engage in upegafesootai faaagafesootai, entertainment, and educational pursuits, it can also however, result in overdependence and compulsive usage, and ultimately popolega mafaufau for some users (James & Drennan, 2005; Lee et al. 2014). The industry implications for devices that can facilitate ever higher levels of cognitive absorptive are stark, with media reports questioning whether smartphones should actually carry health warnings, and concerns of problematic smartphone usage among young people (Pells, 2017; Siddique, 2015). A further practical application to support individuals with problematic smartphone usage would be an informative polokalama feaveai which records the application usage to its user, which should help with tulafono faatonutonu a le tagata lava ia.

    6.2. Limitations and future research

    This study has several limitations. Regarding internal validity, the research is based on participants’ lipoti a le tagata lava ia, which may be vulnerable to common-method variance. This said, self-reports may be the most valid mechanism to assess individuals’ psychological characteristics, as subjects are best placed to provide insights into their own beliefs than outside observers. The problems associated with common-method variance, however, might be exaggerated (Spector, 2006). Secondly, regarding external validity, the study is based on a cross-sectional survey, administered to a US student sample, which may interfere with the detection of demographic relationships in the study. Future research might employ longitudinal mamanu suʻesuʻe and broader sample profiles in an attempt to replicate the findings of the present study. In order to do so, further research should focus on specific services and seek a stratified sample that is more sui of the SNS population. Thirdly, given that two dimensions yielded nonsignificant results concerning the relationship between cognitive absorption and the two addictions, we may begin to question whether the current cognitive absorption construct is suitable in its present form for studies of addiction to social network services, and how immersion in social networks might differ from immersion in other potentially amioga ua fai ma vaisu. Scale development and refinement in this context provides another potential avenue for future research to improve internal validity.

    There is considerable scope for future research on addiction to smartphones and their applications and the dark side of technology more generally, particularly among those members of society who are vulnerable including adolescents and those from younger age groups. One question that demands further attention is whether extended smartphone usage is actually increasing Internet addiction. Further, certain facets of problematic smartphone usage (for example, so-called smartphone zombies), are yet to receive detailed scrutiny. We must move on from studies of ‘global’ addiction to a device to more nuanced studies that distinguish between the device and its applications and user addictions, as well as an understanding of users’ cognitive perceptions of technology. Finally, given the ubiquity of smartphone devices it is important to comprehend the paradox of technology both in its capacity to liberate and to subjugate.

    Appendix. Survey Items

    Cognitive Absorption

    Temporal Dissociation

    Time appears to go by very quickly when I am using social networking apps on my smartphone.

    Sometimes I lose track of time when I am using social networking apps on my smartphone.

    Time flies when I am using social networking apps on my smartphone.

    Most times when I use social networking apps on my smartphone, I end up spending more time that I had planned.

    I often spend more time using social networking apps on my smartphone than I had intended.

    Focused Immersion

    While using social networking apps on my smartphone I am able to block out most other out-of-world distractions.

    While using social networking apps on my smartphone, I am absorbed in what I am doing.

    While using social networking apps on my smartphone, I am immersed in the task I am performing.

    When using social networking apps on my smartphone, I get distracted by other out-of-world attentions very easily

    While using social networking apps on my smartphone, my attention does not get diverted out-of-world very easily.

    Heightened Enjoyment

    I have fun using social networking apps on my smartphone.

    Using social networking apps on my smartphone provides me with a lot of enjoyment.

    I enjoy using social networking apps on my smartphone.

    Using social networking apps on my smartphone bores me.

    pule

    When using social networking apps on my smartphone I feel in control.

    I feel that I have no control over the use of social networking apps on my smartphone.

    Social networking apps on my smartphone allows me to control my computer interaction.

    fia iloa

    Using social networking apps on my smartphone excites my curiosity.

    Using social networking apps on my smartphone makes me curious.

    Using social networking apps on my smartphone arouses my imagination.

    Addiction to the Device

    I sometimes neglect important things because of my interest in my telefonipoto.

    My soifuaga lautele has sometimes suffered because of me interacting my smartphone.

    Using my smartphone sometimes interfered with other activities.

    When I am not use my smartphone, I often feel agitated.

    I have made unsuccessful attempts to reduce the time use my smartphone.

    I feel lost without my upegafesootai faaagafesootai polokalama.

    I tend to get easily distracted by social networking apps.

    Addiction to the App

    I sometimes neglect important things because of my interest in social networking apps.

    My social life has sometimes suffered because of me interacting with social networking apps.

    Using social networking apps sometimes interfered with other activities.

    When I am not using social networking apps, I often feel agitated.

    I have made unsuccessful attempts to reduce the time I interact with social networking apps.

    I feel lost without my smartphone.

    I tend to get easily distracted by smartphone.

    mau faasino

     

    Al-Barashdi et al., 2015

    H.S. Al-Barashdi, A. Bouazza, N.H. JaburSmartphone addiction among university undergraduates: A literature review
    Journal of Scientific Research and Reports, 4 (3) (2015), pp. 210-225

    American Psychiatric Association, 2013

    American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5)
    (5th ed.), American Psychiatric Association Publishing, Washington, DC (2013)

    Baral, 2017

    S. BaralHow to break the habit of checking your phone all the time
    Mic (2017)
    Retrieved July 31, 2018:

    Barkhuus and Polichar, 2011

    L. Barkhuus, V.E. PolicharEmpowerment through seamfulness: Smart phones in everyday life
    Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 15 (6) (2011), pp. 629-639

    Barnes and Pressey, 2014

    S.J. Barnes, A.D. PresseyCaught in the Web? Addictive behavior in cyberspace and the role of goal-orientation
    Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 86 (2014), pp. 93-109

    Barnes and Pressey, 2017

    S.J. Barnes, A.D. PresseyVirtual world addiction and problematic consumption: The distortive effects of addiction on perceptions of cognitive absorption and subsequent usage outcomes
    American Marketing Association Summer Educators’ Conference, San Francisco (2017)

    Baron ma Kenny, 1986

    R.M. Baron, D.A. KennyThe moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations
    Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 51 (6) (1986), pp. 1173-1182

    Belardi, 18 2012

    B. Belardi (Ed.), Consumers crave iPhone more than Facebook, sex (18 June 2012)
    Retrieved July 31, 2018 from PR Newswire website:

    Beranuy Fargues et al., 2009

    M. Beranuy Fargues, A. Chamarro Lusar, C. Graner Jordania, X. Carbonell SanchezValidación de dos escalas breves para evaluar la adicción a Internet y el abuso del móvil
    Psicothema, 21 (2009), pp. 480-485

    Bernroider et al., 2014

    E.W.N. Bernroider, B. Krumay, S. MargiolNot without my smartphone! Impacts of smartphone addiction on smartphone usage
    25th Australasian conference on information systems, Auckland, New Zealand (2014)

    Bianchi ma Phillips, 2005

    A. Bianchi, J.G. PhillipsPsychological predictors of problem mobile phone use
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 8 (1) (2005), pp. 39-51

    Bian and Leung, 2014

    M. Bian, L. LeungLinking loneliness, shyness, smartphone addiction symptoms, and patterns of smartphone use to social capital
    Social Science Computer Review (2014), pp. 1-19

    Bian and Leung, 2015

    M. Bian, L. LeungLinking loneliness, shyness, smartphone addiction symptoms, and patterns of smartphone use to social capital
    Social Science Computer Review, 33 (1) (2015), pp. 61-79

    Billieux, 2012

    J. BillieuxProblematic use of the mobile phone use: A literature review and the pathways model
    Current Psychiatry Reviews, 8 (2012), pp. 1-9

    Pupuni, 2008

    J. BlockMataupu mo DSM-V: vaisu i luga ole Initaneti
    American Journal of Psychiatry, 165 (2008), pp. 306-307

    Bozoglan et al., 2014

    B. Bozoglan, V. Demirer, I. SahinProblematic Internet use: Functions of use, cognitive absorption, and depression
    Computers in Human Behavior, 37 (2014), pp. 117-123

    Brancheau and Wetherbe, 1990

    J.C. Brancheau, J.C. WetherbeThe adoption of spreadsheet software: Testing innovation diffusion theory in the context of end-user computing
    Information Systems Research, 1 (1) (1990), pp. 41-64

    Brenner, 1997

    V. BrennerPsychology of computer use: XLVII. Parameters of internet use, abuse and addiction: The first 90 days of the internet usage survey
    Psychological Reports, 80 (1997), pp. 879-882

    Bridges and Florsheim, 2008

    E. Bridges, R. FlorsheimHedonic and utilitarian shopping goals: The online experience
    Journal of Business Research, 61 (4) (2008), pp. 309-314

    Brod, 1984

    C. BrodTechnostress: The human cost of the computer revolution
    Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA (1984)

    Cantril and Allport, 1935

    H. Cantril, G.W. AllportThe psychology of radio
    Harper, New York (1935)

    Carillo et al., 2017

    K. Carillo, E. Scornavacca, S. ZaThe role of media dependency in predicting continuance intention to use ubiquitous media systems
    Information & Management, 54 (3) (2017), pp. 317-335

    Charlton ma Danforth, 2007

    J.P. Charlton, I.D.W. DanforthEseesega vaisu ma maualuga auai i le tulaga o le initoneti taʻaloga taʻalo
    Computers in Human Humanities, 23 (3) (2007), pp. 1531-1548

    Cho and Lee, 2015

    S. Cho, E. LeeDevelopment of a brief instrument to measure smartphone addiction among nursing students
    Computers, Informatics, Nursing, 33 (5) (2015), pp. 216-224

    Claisse and Rowe, 1987

    G. Claisse, F. RoweThe telephone in question: Questions on communication
    Computer Networks and ISDN Systems, 14 (2–5) (1987), pp. 207-219

    Csikszentmihalyi, 1990

    M. CsikszentmihalyiFlow: The psychology of optimal experience
    Harper and Row, New York (1990)

    Davey and Davey, 2014

    S. Davey, A. DaveyAssessment of smartphone addiction in indian adolescents: A mixed method study by systematic-review and meta-analysis approach
    International Journal of Preventative Medicine, 5 (12) (2014), pp. 1500-1511

    Davis, 1989

    F.D. DavisPerceived usefulness, perceived ease of use, and user acceptance of information technology
    MIS Quarterly, 13 (3) (1989), pp. 319-339

    Davis et al., 1992

    F.D. Davis, R.P. Bagozzi, P.R. WarshawExtrinsic and intrinsic motivation to use computers in the workplace
    Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22 (1992), pp. 1111-1132

    De-Sola Gutiérrez et al., 2016

    J. De-Sola Gutiérrez, F. Rodríguez de Fonseca, G. RubioCell-phone addiction: A review
    Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7 (2016), p. 175

    Deci ma Ryan, 1985

    E.L. Deci, R.M. RyanIntrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior
    Plenum, New York (1985)

    Demirci et al., 2015

    K. Demirci, M. Akgongul, A. AkpinarRelationship of smartphone use severity with sleep quality, depression, and anxiety in university students
    Faʻamaumauga o Faʻasalalau Faʻaleaga, 4 (2) (2015), pp. 85-92

    Demirci et al., 2014

    K. Demirci, H. Orhan, A. Demirdas, A. Akpinar, H. SertValidity and reliability of the Turkish version of the smartphone addiction scale in a younger population
    Bulletin of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 24 (3) (2014), pp. 226-234

    Demirer and Bozoglan, 2016

    V. Demirer, B. BozoglanPurposes of Internet use and problematic Internet use among Turkish high school students
    Asia-Pacific Psychiatry, 8 (4) (2016), pp. 269-277

    van Deursen et al., 2015

    A.J.A.M. van Deursen, C.L. Bolle, S.M. Hegner, P.A.M. KommersModeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior. The role of smartphone usage types, emotional intelligence, social stress, self-regulation, age, and gender
    Computers in Human Behavior, 45 (2015), pp. 411-420

    Ehrenberg et al., 2008

    A. Ehrenberg, S.C. Juckes, K.M. White, S.P. WalshPersonality and self-esteem as predictors of young people’s technology use
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 11 (6) (2008), pp. 739-741

    Emanuel, 2015

    R. EmanuelThe truth about smartphone addiction
    College Student Journal, 49 (2015), p. 291

    Ericsson, 2017

    EricssonEricsson mobility report, June 2017
    Ericsson, Stockholm (2017)

    Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975

    M. Fishbein, I. AjzenBelief, attitude, intention and BehaviorBehavior: An introduction to theory and research
    Addison-Wesley, Reading MA (1975)

    Geser, 2006

    H. GeserAre girls (even) more addicted? Some gender patterns of cell phone usage
    Sociology in Switzerland: Sociology of the mobile phone (2006)
    Retrieved date, from

    Griffiths, 1996

    MD GriffithsNicotine, tobacco and addiction
    Nature, 384 (1996), p. 18

    Griffiths, 1997

    M. GriffithsPsychology of computer use: XLIII. Some comments on ‘addictive use of the internet’ by young
    Psychological Reports, 80 (1) (1997), pp. 81-82

    Griffiths, 1999

    MD GriffithsInternet addiction: Fact or fiction?
    The Psychologist, 12 (1999), pp. 246-250

    Griffiths, 2000

    MD GriffithsDoes Internet and computer ‘addiction’ exist? Some case study evidence
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 3 (2000), pp. 211-218

    Griffiths, 2001

    MD GriffithsSex on the internet: Observations and implications for internet sex addiction
    Le Tusi o Suesuega i Feusuaiga, 38 (4) (2001), i. 333-342

    Griffiths, 2012

    MD GriffithsInternet sex addiction. A review of empirical research
    Addiction Theory and Research, 20 (2012), pp. 111-124

    Hair et al., 2014

    J.F. Hair, W.C. Black, B.J. Babin, R.E. AndersonMultivariate data analysis
    Pearson Education, Harlow (2014)

    Hakoama and Hakoyama, 2011

    M. Hakoama, S. HakoyamaThe impact of cell phone use on social networking and development among college students
    The American Association of Behavioral and Social Sciences Journal, 15 (2011), pp. 1-20

    Helmuth, 2001

    L. HelmuthBeyond the pleasure principle
    Saienisi, 294 (5544) (2001), pp. 983-984

    Horvath, 2004

    C.W. HorvathMeasuring television addiction
    Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 48 (3) (2004), pp. 378-398

    Jackson et al., 2008

    L.A. Jackson, Y. Zhao, A. Kolenic, H.E. Fitsgerald, R. Harold, A. von EyeRace, gender, and information technology use: The new digital divide
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 11 (4) (2008), pp. 437-442

    James and Drennan, 2005

    D. James, J. DrennanExploring addictive consumption of mobile phone technology
    S. Purchase (Ed.), ANZMAC 2005: Broadening the boundaries: Conference proceedings, 5 December–7 December 2005. Australia, Western Australia: Fremantle (2005)

    Jang, 2002

    H.J. JangPsychological Characteristics of adolescents addictive using cellular phone
    [dissertation]
    Sungshin Women’s University, Seoul, South Korea (2002)

    Jenaro et al., 2007

    C. Jenaro, N. Flores, M. Gomez-Vela, F. Gonzalez-Gil, C. CaballoProblematic internet and cell-phone use: Psychological, behavioral and health correlates
    Addiction Research and Theory, 15 (2007), pp. 309-320

    Jeong et al., 2016

    S.-E. Jeong, H. Kim, J.-Y. Yum, Y. HwangWhat type of content are smartphone users addicted to?: SNS vs. games
    Computers in Human Behavior, 54 (2016), pp. 10-17

    Jeong and Lee, 2015

    H. Jeong, Y. LeeSmartphone addiction and empathy among nursing students
    Advanced Science and Technology Letters, 88 (2015), pp. 224-228

    Junco et al., 2010

    R. Junco, D. Merson, D.W. SalterThe effect of gender, ethnicity, and income on college students’ use of communication technologies
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 13 (6) (2010), pp. 619-627

    Katz and Akhus, 2002

    J.E. Katz, M. AkhusPerpetual contact: Mobile communication, private talk, public performance
    Cambridge University Press (2002)

    Keepers, 1990

    G.A. KeepersPathological preoccupation with video games
    Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29 (1) (1990), pp. 49-50

    Kibona and Mgaya, 2015

    L. Kibona, G. MgayaSmartphones’ effects on academic performance of higher learning students
    Journal of Multidisciplinary Engineering Science and Technology, 2 (4) (2015), pp. 777-784

    Kim et al., 2014

    d. Kim, Y. Lee, J. Lee, J.K. Nam, Y. ChungDevelopment of Korean smartphone addiction proneness scale for youth
    PLoS One, 9 (5) (2014), pp. 1-8

    Ko et al., 2009

    C.-H. Ko, G.-C. Liu, S. Hsiao, J.-Y. Yen, M.-J. Yang, W.-C. Lin, al iā.Gaioiga o gaioiga e fesoʻotai ma faʻasologa o taʻaloga i luga o le initaneti
    Journal of Psychiatric Research, 43 (7) (2009), pp. 739-747

    Kuss et al., 2013

    D.J. Kuss, A.J. van Rooij, G.W. Shorter, M.D. Griffiths, D. van de MheenInternet addiction in adolescents: Prevalence and risk factors
    Computers in Human Behavior, 29 (2013), pp. 1987-1996

    Kwon et al., 2013

    M. Kwon, D.-J. Kim, H. Cho, S. YangThe smartphone addiction scale: Development and validation of a short version for adolescents
    PLoS One, 8 (12) (2013), pp. 1-7

    Kwon et al., 2013b

    Kwon, J.-Y. Lee, W.-Y. Won, J.-W. Park, J.-A. Min, C. Hahn, al iā.Development and validation of a smartphone addiction scale (SAS)
    PLoS One, 8 (2) (2013), pp. 1-7

    Lane and Manner, 2011

    W. Lane, C. MannerThe impact of personality traits on smartphone owner-ship and use
    International Journal of Business and Social Science, 2 (2011), pp. 22-28

    Lee, 2015

    E. LeeToo much information: Heavy smartphone and Facebook utilization by African American young adults
    Journal of Black Studies, 46 (1) (2015), pp. 44-61

    Lee et al., 2014

    Y.-K. Lee, C.-T. Chang, Y. Lin, Z.-H. ChengThe dark side of smartphone usage: Psychological traits, compulsive behavior and technostress
    Computers in Human Behavior, 31 (2014), pp. 373-383

    Lehenbauer-Baum and Fohringer, 2015

    M. Lehenbauer-Baum, M. FohringerTowards classification criteria for internet gaming disorder: Debunking differences between addiction and high engagement in a German sample of World of Warcraft players
    Computers in Human Behavior, 45 (2015), pp. 345-351

    Lenhart et al., 2010

    A. Lenhart, K. Purcell, A. Smith, K. ZickuhrSocial media and young adults
    Pew Internet and American Life Project (2010)
    Toe aumai mai

    Leung, 2007

    L. LeungLeisure, boredom, sensation seeking, self-esteem, addiction symptoms and patterns of cell phone use
    E.A. Konijn, M.A. Tanis, S. Utz, A. Linden (Eds.), Mediated interpersonal communication, Lawrence Eribaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ (2007), pp. 359-381

    Leung, 2008

    L. LeungLinking psychological attributes to addiction and improper use of the mobile phone among adolescents in Hong Kong
    Journal of Children and Media, 2 (2008), pp. 93-113

    Lin, 2004

    X.H. LinInternet addiction disorder, on-line behavior and personality: An exploratory study
    International Journal of Psychology, 39 (5–6) (2004)
    272-272

    Lin et al., 2017

    Y.H. Lin, P.H. Lin, C.L. Chiang, Y.H. Lee, C.C. Yang, T.B. Kuo, al iā.Incorporation of mobile application (app) measures into the diagnosis of smartphone addiction
    Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 78 (7) (2017), pp. 866-872

    Lin et al., 2015

    Y.H. Lin, Y.C. Lin, Y.H. Lee, P.H. Lin, S.H. Lin, L.R. Chang, al iā.Time distortion associated with smartphone addiction: Identifying smartphone addiction via a mobile application (App)
    Journal of Psychiatric Research, 65 (2015), pp. 139-145

    Lopez-Fernandez et al., 2012

    O. Lopez-Fernandez, M.L. Honrubia-Serrano, M. Freixa-BlanxartAdaptación española del ‘Mobile Phone Problem Use Scale’ para población adolescente
    Adicciones, 24 (2012), pp. 123-130

    Mcllwraith, 1998

    R.D. Mcllwraith“I’m addicted to television”: The personality, imagination, and TV watching patterns of self-identified TV addicts
    Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 42 (1998), pp. 371-386

    Merlo et al., 2013

    L.J. Merlo, A.M. Stone, A. BibbeyMeasuring problematic mobile phone use: Development and preliminary psychometric properties of the PUMP scale
    Journal of Addiction (2013)

    Merriam-Webster’s Medical Dictionary, 1995

    Merriam-Webster’s Medical DictionaryMerriam-webster, Springfield, MA
    (1995)

    Mick and Fournier, 1998

    D.G. Mick, S. FournierParadoxes of technology: Consumer cognizance, emotions, and coping strategies
    Journal of Consumer Research, 25 (September) (1998), pp. 123-143

    Mok et al., 2014

    J.-Y. Mok, S.-W. Choi, D.-J. Kim, J.-S. Choi, J. Lee, H. Ahn, al iā.Latent class analysis on internet and smartphone addiction in college students
    Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 10 (2014), pp. 817-828

    Morahan-Martin ma Schumacher, 2000

    J. Morahan-Martin, P. SchumacherFa'alavelave ma fa'amaopoopoina ole fa'aogaina ole Initaneti ole fa'aogaina ile va o tamaiti a'oga kolisi
    Computers in Human Humanities, 16 (1) (2000), pp. 2-13

    Faʻaalia, 2010

    JC Leai se meaPsychometric theory 3E
    Tata McGraw-Hill Education, New York (2010)

    Oulasvirta et al., 2012

    A. Oulasvirta, T. Rattenbury, L. Ma, E. RaitaO amioga masani e faʻaoga tele ai le atamai
    Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 16 (1) (2012), pp. 105-114

    Park and Park, 2014

    C. Park, Y.R. ParkThe conceptual model on smart phone addiction among early childhood
    International Journal of Social Science and Humanity, 4 (2) (2014), pp. 147-150

    Paternoster et al., 1998

    R. Paternoster, R. Brame, P. Mazerolle, A.R. PiqueroUsing the correct statistical test for the equality of regression coefficients
    Criminology, 36 (4) (1998), pp. 859-866

    Pawłowska and Potembska, 2012

    B. Pawłowska, E. PotembskaGender and severity of symptoms of mobile phone addiction in Polish gymnasium, secondary school and university students
    Current Problems in Psychiatry, 12 (4) (2012), pp. 433-438

    Pearson ma Hussain, 2015

    C. Pearson, Z. HussainSmartphone use, addiction, narcissism, and personality: A mixed methods investigation
    International Journal of Cyber Behavior, Psychology and Learning, 5 (1) (2015), pp. 17-32

    Pedrero Perez et al., 2012

    E.J. Pedrero Perez, M.T. Rodriguez Monje, J.M. Ruiz Sanchez De LeonAdicción o abuso del teléfono móvil: Revisión de la literature
    Adicciones, 24 (2012), pp. 139-152

    Peele, 1985

    S. PeeleThe meaning of addiction: Compulsive experience and its interpretation
    Lexington, Lexington, MA (1985)

    Pells, 2017

    R. PellsGiving your child a smartphone is like giving them a gram of cocaine, says top addiction expert
    Le Tutoʻatasi (2017)
    [last accessed: 05/10/2017]

    Pontes ma Griffiths, 2016

    H.M. Pontes, M.D. GriffithsPortuguese validation of the internet gaming disorder scale–short-form
    Cyberpsychology, Amioga, ma Fesoʻotaʻiga Lautele, 19 (4) (2016), pp. 288-293

    Ragu-Nathan et al., 2008

    T.S. Ragu-Nathan, M. Tarafdar, B.S. Ragu-Nathan, Q. TuThe consequences of technostress for end users in organizations: Conceptual development and empirical validation
    Information Systems Research, 19 (4) (2008), pp. 417-433

    Roberts and Pirog, 2012

    J.A. Roberts, S.F. Pirog IIIA preliminary investigation of materialism and impulsiveness as predictors of technological addictions among young adults
    Faʻamaumauga o Faʻasalalau Faʻaleaga, 2 (1) (2012), pp. 56-62

    Roberts et al., 2014

    J.A. Roberts, L.H.P. Yaya, C. ManolisThe invisible addiction: Cell-phone activities and addiction among male and female college students
    Faʻamaumauga o Faʻasalalau Faʻaleaga, 3 (4) (2014), pp. 254-265

    Rogers, 1995

    E.M. RogersDiffusion of innovations
    (4th ed.), The Free Press, New York (1995)

    Rosen et al., 2013

    L.D. Rosen, K. Whaling, L.M. Carrier, N.A. Cheever, J. RokkumThe media and technology usage and attitudes scale: An empirical investigation
    Computers in Human Humanities, 29 (6) (2013), pp. 2501-2511

    Sahin et al., 2013

    S. Sahin, K. Ozdemir, A. Unsal, N. TemizEvaluation of mobile phone addiction level and sleep quality in university students
    Pakistan Journal of Medical Sciences, 29 (2013), pp. 913-918

    Salehan ma Negahban, 2013

    M. Salehan, A. NegahbanSocial networking on smartphones: When mobile phones become addictive
    Computers in Human Behavior, 29 (2013), pp. 2632-2639

    Samaha and Hawi, 2016

    M. Samaha, N. HawiRelationships among smartphone addiction, stress, academic performance, and satisfaction with life
    Computers in Human Behavior, 57 (2016), pp. 321-325

    Sanchez Martinez and Otero, 2009

    M. Sanchez Martinez, A. OteroFactors associated with cell phone use in adolescents in the community of Madrid (Spain)
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 12 (2009), pp. 131-137

    Sapacz et al., 2016

    M. Sapacz, G. Rockman, J. ClarkAre we addicted to our cell phones?
    Computers in Human Behavior, 57 (2016), pp. 153-159

    Shambare et al., 2012

    R. Shambare, R. Rugimbana, T. ZhowaAre mobile phones the 21st century addiction?
    African Journal of Business Management, 62 (2) (2012), pp. 573-577

    Shin and Dey, 2013

    C. Shin, A.K. DeyAutomatically detecting problematic use of smartphones, UbiComp’13
    (2013), pp. 335-344

    Shotton, 1991

    M.A. ShottonThe costs and benefits of ‘computer addiction’
    Behavior and Information Technology, 10 (3) (1991), pp. 219-230

    Siddique, 2015

    H. SiddiqueSmartphones are addictive and should carry health warning, say academics
    Le Guardian (2015)
    [last accessed: 05/10/2017]

    Sobel, 1986

    M.E. SobelSome new results on indirect effects and their standard errors in covariance structure
    Sociological Methodology, 16 (1986), pp. 159-186

    Spector, 2006

    P.E. SpectorMethod variance in organizational research
    Organizational Research Methods, 9 (2) (2006), pp. 221-232

    Takao et al., 2009

    T. Takao, S. Takahashi, M. KitamuraAddictive personality and problematic mobile phone use
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 12 (2009), pp. 1-9

    Taneja, 2014

    C. TanejaThe psychology of excessive cellular phone use
    Delhi Psychiatry Journal, 17 (2014), pp. 448-451

    Tavakolizadeh et al., 2014

    J. Tavakolizadeh, A. Atarodi, S. Ahmadpour, A. PourgheisarThe prevalence of excessive mobile phone use and its relation with mental health status and demographic factors among the students of Gonabad university of medical sciences in 2011-2012
    Razavi International Journal of Medicine, 2 (1) (2014), pp. 1-7

    Tellegen, 1981

    A. TellegenPracticing the two disciplines for relaxation and enlightenment: Comment on ‘role of the feedback in electromyograph feedback: The relevance of attention’ by qualls and sheehan
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110 (2) (1981), pp. 217-226

    Tellegen, 1982

    A. TellegenBrief manual for the multidimensional personality questionnaire
    Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Unpublished Manuscript (1982)

    Tellegen and Atkinson, 1974

    A. Tellegen, G. AtkinsonOpenness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (‘absorption’), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility
    Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83 (3) (1974), pp. 268-277

    Trevino and Webster, 1992

    L.K. Trevino, J. WebsterFlow in computer- mediated communication: Electronic mail and voice mail evaluation and impacts
    Communication Research, 19 (5) (1992), pp. 539-573

    Turel ma Serenko, 2010

    O. Turel, A. SerenkoIs mobile email addiction overlooked?
    Communications of the ACM, 53 (5) (2010), pp. 41-43

    Turel et al., 2011

    O. Turel, A. Serenko, P. GilesIntegrating technology addiction and use: An empirical investigation of online auction users
    MIS Quarterly, 35 (4) (2011), pp. 1043-1061

    Turkle, 2017

    S. TurkleAlone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from each other, Hachette UK
    (2017)

    Vallerand, 1997

    R.J. VallerandToward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation
    Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29 (1997), pp. 271-360

    Volti, 1995

    R. VoltiSociety and technological change
    (3rd ed.), St. Martin’s Press, New York (1995)

    Wang et al., 2015

    J.-L. Wang, H.-Z. Wang, J. Gaskin, L.-H. WangThe role of stress and motivation in problematic smartphone use among college students
    Computers in Human Behavior, 53 (2015), pp. 181-188

    Webster and Hackley, 1997

    J. Webster, P. HackleyTeaching effectiveness in technology-mediated distance learning
    Academy of Management Journal, 40 (6) (1997), pp. 1282-1309

    Webster and Ho, 1997

    J. Webster, H. HoAudience engagement in multimedia presentations
    ACM SIGMIS – Data Base, 28 (2) (1997), pp. 63-77

    Yellowlees and Marks, 2007

    P.M. Yellowlees, S. MarksProblematic internet use or internet addiction?
    Computers in Human Humanities, 23 (3) (2007), pp. 1447-1453

    Talavou, 1998

    KS YoungVaisu i luga ole Initaneti: Le tulaʻi mai o se maʻi fou ile falemaʻi
    CyberPsychology and Behavior, 1 (3) (1998), pp. 237-244

    Talavou, 1999

    KS YoungInternet addiction: Symptoms, evaluation and treatment
    Innovations in Clinical Practice: A Source Book, 17 (1999), pp. 19-31

    Zhang et al., 2014

    K.Z.K. Zhang, C. Chen, M.K.O. LeeUnderstanding the role of motives in smartphone addiction
    Pacific Asia conference on information systems proceedings, paper 131 (2014)