эрте өспүрүм келемиштер ядросу accumbens менен Fast тинейджер релиз окуялар (2011)

Neuroscience. 2011 Mar 10;176:296-307. 

булак

Bowles Center for Alcohol Studies and Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27713, USA. [электрондук почта корголгон]

жалпылаган

Subsecond fluctuations in тинейджер (тинейджер transients) in the ядро accumbens are often time-locked to rewards and cues and provide an important learning signal during reward processing. As the mesolimbic тинейджер system undergoes dynamic changes during adolescence in the rat, it is possible that тинейджер transients encode reward and stimulus presentations differently in adolescents. However, to date no measurements of тинейджер transients in awake adolescents have been made. Thus, we used орозо scan cyclic voltammetry to measure тинейджер transients in the ядро accumbens core of male чычкандар (29-30 days of age) at baseline and with the presentation of various stimuli that have been shown to trigger тинейджер бошотуу улуураак чычкандар. Биз ошону таптык тинейджер transients were detectable in жаш аял чычкандар and occurred at a baseline rate similar to adult чычкандар (71-72 days of age). However, unlike adults, жаш аял чычкандар did not reliably exhibit тинейджер transients at the unexpected presentation of visual, audible and odorous stimuli. In contrast, brief interaction with another rat increased тинейджер transients in both жаш аял жана бойго жеткен чычкандар. While this effect habituated in adults at a second interaction, it persisted in the adolescents. These data are the first demonstration of тинейджер transients in жаш аял чычкандар and reveal an important divergence from adults in the occurrence of these transients that may result in differential learning about rewards.

Copyright © 2011 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Adolescence, dopamine, novelty, social interaction, voltammetry, nucleus accumbens

тааныштыруу

Burst firing of dopamine neurons and the resulting dopamine release events (aka dopamine transients) are thought to comprise a key learning signal in the brain (Schultz, 2007; Roesch .Удаалаш., 2010), interfacing external rewards and cues with appetitive behaviors. Dopamine transients occur spontaneously in several dopamine target regions (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2002) and are more prominent at the presentation of unexpected stimuli (Rebec .Удаалаш., 1997; Robinson and Wightman, 2004), social interaction (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2001; Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2002) and rewards (Ройтман .Удаалаш., 2008). The fast dopamine release events are often followed by appetitive behaviors, such as approaching another rat or pressing a lever for a reinforcer (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2002; Плюс ж.б.., 2003; Ройтман .Удаалаш., 2004). Moreover, neutral cues that normally do not evoke dopamine transients can do so when repeatedly paired with a reward (Станбер .Удаалаш., 2005b; Станбер .Удаалаш., 2005a; Day и др., 2007), showing that this neural signal undergoes learning-induced plasticity. Importantly, dopamine transients are a result of volume transmission and achieve high extrasynaptic concentrations that can activate low-affinity, extrasynaptic dopamine receptors (Wightman and Robinson, 2002). Thus, dopamine transients appear to function as a brain signal of potential and established reward that can direct attention and facilitate the acquisition of that reward.

The mesolimbic dopamine system undergoes dynamic changes during adolescence in the rat. For example, expression of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in ventral striatum increases from pre-adolescence to adolescence (e.g., Андерсен .Удаалаш., 1997), with some studies suggesting that binding in adolescence is higher than in adulthood (for references and discussion, see Doremus-Fitzwater ж.б.., 2010; Wahlstrom et al., 2010b). Moreover, firing rates of dopamine neurons (МакКатчен жана Marinelli, 2009) and basal dopamine concentrations (Badanich .Удаалаш., 2006; Philpot .Удаалаш., 2009) show similar U-shaped curves, peaking at adolescence. While dopamine transients have not yet been measured in adolescent rats, enhanced behavioral responses to novelty (Дуглас .Удаалаш., 2003; Байрак жана Kirstein, 2006; Philpot and Wecker, 2008) and social peers (Varlinskaya and Spear, 2008) have been reported in adolescent versus adult rats.

The present study aimed to provide the first measurements of dopamine transients in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) of adolescent rats and compare them to those in adults. We used fast scan cyclic voltammetry, an electrochemical technique with the spatial and temporal resolution required to detect dopamine transients (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2008). We examined dopamine release in early adolescence (29 – 30 days of age) given that rats exhibit enhanced levels of peer-directed social interactions at this age relative to later adolescent and adult ages (Varlinskaya and Spear, 2008). Accordingly, we measured dopamine transients during brief social interactions as well as at baseline and at the presentation of unexpected, novel stimuli that have been reported to trigger dopamine transients in adult rats (Robinson and Wightman, 2004).

эксперименттик жол-жоболору

Animal subjects

All experiments described herein were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH Publications No. 80-23, revised 1996). Male Long Evans rats were purchased from Charles River Laboratories (Raleigh, NC) in cohorts of four at postnatal day (PND) 21. Each rat was individually housed upon arrival with food and water ад либитум. Two of the rats from each cohort were assigned to the adolescent group and underwent surgery on PND 26 (67 ± 2 g) and voltammetric measurements on PND 29 or 30 (76 ± 3 g). The remaining two rats from each cohort were assigned to the adult group and underwent surgery on PND 68 (379 ± 12 g) and voltammetric measurements on PND 71 or 72 (379 ± 14 g); these rats were pair housed from PND 30 – 63.

Surgical preparation

The surgical procedures are as described previously (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2009) with the following exceptions. Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane; adolescent rats were induced at 3% and maintained at 1 – 1.5% during surgery, while adult rats were induced at 5% and maintained at 2%. Rats were secured in a stereotaxic frame for implantation of a guide cannula above the NAc, a bipolar stimulating electrode in the ventral tegmental area, and an Ag/AgCl reference, as previously described. The coordinates (versus bregma in mm) for the guide cannula were 1.3 anterior and 1.6 lateral (adolescent) or 1.7 anterior and 1.7 lateral (adult). The stimulating electrode was implanted at a 6° angle at the coordinates (versus bregma in mm) 4.1 posterior, 1.3 lateral (adolescent) or 5.2 posterior, 1.2 lateral (adult). Postoperatively, rats were closely monitored and received ibuprofen (15 mg/kg daily, p.o.) and palatable food.

эксперименталдык дизайн

Dopamine release was measured by using fast scan cyclic voltammetry as previously described (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2009). Voltammetric recordings were made in a custom built Plexiglas chamber. Floor space was 21 × 21 cm with an angled insert that extended from the floor to 10 cm against the wall at a 48° angle; this prevented the rats from hitting the electrode and headstage assembly against the wall of the chamber. Rats were manually restrained while the carbon-fiber was lowered into the NAc core via the guide cannula, and then left undisturbed for 15 – 20 min. Next, voltammetric recordings were made every 2 – 4 min to assess the presence of naturally-occurring dopamine transients and electrically evoked dopamine release (16 – 24 pulses, 40 – 60 Hz, 120 μA, 2 msec/phase, biphasic). Once we verified that the electrode was positioned near dopamine terminals, the experiment began; this was typically 60 – 85 min after initial placement in the chamber. Recording was continuous for 50 min. During the first 25 min, 5 stimuli were presented for 3 s at 5-min intervals in a random order: house light off, tone, white noise, coconut odor and lemon odor. The odors were presented in the following manner (Robinson and Wightman, 2004): the experimenter dipped a cotton applicator into an extract (McCormick), opened the recording chamber door, held the applicator 1 – 2 cm from the rat’s nose for 3 s, then withdrew and closed the chamber door. After presentation of all stimuli, recording continued for 5 min, then another male Long Evans rat was placed in the cage with the test rat for 60 s and allowed to interact. For the adolescent group, the conspecific rat was age-matched to the test rat; for the adults, the partner was a slightly smaller adult (87 ± 6% of test rat weight) in an attempt to prevent the emergence of aggressive interactions. A second 60-s interaction period occurred with the same partner 10 min later. After recording, rats were given a lethal dose of urethane (> 1.5 g/kg, i.p.) then perfused with a formalin solution. The brains were removed, frozen, sectioned (40-μm thickness) and stained with thionin to determine recording sites.

Evaluation of dopamine transients

Dopamine transients during recording were statistically identified as previously described (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2009). In brief, each voltammetric scan was background-subtracted and compared to a template of electrically-evoked dopamine release. After identification of dopamine release events, each event was examined to determine the signal-to-noise ratio, with the signal being the maximal concentration of dopamine ([DA]макс) and the noise calculated as the rms amplitude in the 10 scans (1 s) used in the background subtraction. Only dopamine transients with a signal-to-noise ratio of 5 or higher were included in the study. To determine dopamine release during stimulus presentations, the frequency of transients was calculated for a 20-s period encompassing the stimulus presentation and compared with the frequency during the 4 min immediately preceding the presentation (basal). To determine dopamine release during social interaction, the frequency of transients was calculated for an 80-s period encompassing the interaction period and compared with the frequency during the 4 min immediately preceding the interaction (basal). The maximal concentration of each dopamine release event was calculated by converting the current (nA) to concentration (μM) after экстракорпоралдык calibration of the electrode (Logman et al., 2000).

Behavioral scoring

Behavioral response to stimuli presentations (from initiation of presentation to 2 s following the presentation) was rated from video on a scale of 0 – 3 (0, no movement; 1, sniffing/head movement; 2, orientation movement/startle; 3, locomotion). Social interaction was scored as the overall duration in s of the following behaviors directed toward the other rat: grooming, sniffing, pursuing, ano-genital inspecting, climbing over/under. Trials were scored for social behaviors occurring in the 45 s immediately following closure of the sound-attenuating chamber door after inserting the partner rat into the chamber; this time span was chosen to avoid any confounding influence of opening and closing the chamber doors. Trials were also scored for duration in s of non-social locomotor activity via quadrant crossings during the same 45-s period.

статистикалык анализ

Statistical analysis of dopamine transient frequency between groups and experimental conditions was calculated by using nonparametric multivariate regression analysis (genmod procedure with Poisson distribution, repeated measures and Wald pairwise contrasts; SAS, SAS Institute Inc., Research Triangle Park, NC). Changes in amplitude of dopamine transients and signal-to-noise ratios between groups and experimental epochs were calculated using similar regression analysis with gamma distribution. Behavioral measures were compared between groups by using repeated-measures 2-way ANOVA or within groups by using paired t-tests (GraphPad Prism, GraphPad Software Inc., La Jolla, CA).

натыйжалары

Twelve adolescent and ten adult rats were used for this study. Three adolescent rats were not included due to difficulty at surgery or during voltammetry, and one adult rat was not included due to an incorrect electrode placement. In addition, social interaction data from one adolescent was discarded due to voltammetric difficulties that did not affect the other stimulus presentations. Final n’s were 9 adolescents and 9 adults for the stimulus presentations and 8 adolescents and 9 adults for the social interactions.

Dopamine transients at baseline

Basal rates of dopamine transients were overlapping between adolescent and adult rats in the NAc core. When transient frequency was determined across all baseline samples (files in which no stimulus presentations or rat interactions occurred), adolescent rats exhibited 1.5 ± 0.4 transients/min and adults exhibited 2.5 ± 0.6. However, within each group was a range of transient frequency, as previously reported in mature rats (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2009; Robinson and Wightman, 2007; Wightman et al., 2007): in adolescents, basal frequency ranged from 0.2 – 4.0 transients per min, while in adults the range was 0 – 5.0 transients per min. Importantly, electrically-stimulated release revealed the presence of functional dopamine terminals at the voltammetric recording site even when basal dopamine transients were absent or infrequent.

Dopamine transients at the presentation of unexpected stimuli

Next, we investigated whether dopamine transients were more likely to occur at the unexpected presentation of stimuli in adolescent rats. Figure 1A shows that brief presentations of stimuli increased the frequency of dopamine transients by 50% above baseline levels in adult rats (Wald contrast of adult baseline vs. stimuli, p < 0.05), replicating previous work (Robinson and Wightman, 2004). In contrast, the rate of dopamine release events did not change significantly among the adolescent rats (Wald contrast of adolescent baseline vs. stimuli, p > 0.86). When compared between groups, baseline rates did not differ (Wald contrast of adolescent vs. adult baseline, p > 0.19), while rates during stimulus presentations were higher in adult rats (Wald contrast of adolescent vs. adult stimuli, p < 0.05). Adult rats were more likely to have dopamine release time-locked to the stimuli: 8/9 adults exhibited dopamine release to at least one stimulus presentation, while 6/9 adolescents did so. Moreover, adult rats that exhibited time-locked dopamine transients did so at 3.8 ± 0.4 stimuli (median 4), while adolescents that exhibited time-locked transients did so at 2.3 ± 0.4 stimuli (median 2).

Figure 1

Dopamine release events in adolescent and adult rats during the presentation of unexpected stimuli. (A) In adolescent rats dopamine transient frequency did not change from basal rates during presentation of stimuli. In contrast, the frequency of dopamine ...

We next determined whether transients occurring during stimulus presentations were larger than those during baseline. Figure 1B shows the distribution of the maximal concentrations of dopamine transients. Regression analysis revealed that amplitudes did not differ between adolescent and adult rats (Wald contrasts, all p values > 0.05). In adolescent rats, the maximal concentrations of transients during stimuli presentations were not significantly different from those during baseline. In adult rats, dopamine concentrations were slightly higher during stimulus presentations, a rightward shift that approached statistical significance (Wald contrast of adult baseline vs. stimuli, p < 0.06). The median and average concentrations of dopamine transients are described in стол 1.

стол 1

Maximal concentrations of dopamine release events in the nucleus accumbens core of adolescent and adult rats.

Although group data indicated that dopamine release events were more likely to occur at unexpected stimulus presentations in adults versus adolescent rats, individual plots illustrate the differences among stimuli as well as within groups. Figure 1C shows the change in frequency of dopamine transients during the presentation of a stimulus versus the 4-min of baseline preceding that stimulus. In adults, the stimuli varied in their effectiveness to increase dopamine transient rates; they are ranked from most to least effective as follows: dopamine transients occurred at 208% of basal rates at coconut odor; 184% at tone; 161% at lemon odor; 142% at white noise; and 91% at light out. In adolescent rats, stimuli were overall less effective to trigger dopamine transients; they are ranked from most to least effective as follows: dopamine transients occurred at 158% of basal rates at the tone; 127% at lemon odor; 123% at coconut odor; 84% at white noise; and 23% at light out.

Ошондой болсо да, Figure 1C demonstrates distinct individual variability in emitting a dopamine response to particular stimuli within each age group. To determine whether this neurochemical variability was associated with behavioral variability, we scored each rat for movement associated with the stimulus presentation, as shown in стол 2 (one adult rat was excluded from this analysis due to partial loss of the video record). We then used the Spearman nonparametric correlation to compare behavioral and dopaminergic responses to stimuli within each age group. We found that the frequency of dopamine transients was not correlated with behavioral activation in either age group at any stimulus, regardless of whether the stimulus was analyzed individually or as a group (data not shown, all p values > 0.05), indicating that the behavioral and neurochemical responses to stimuli were not directly related. In addition, the behavior scores of the rats that did not emit any dopamine transients time-locked to stimulus presentations (3/9 adolescents and 1/9 adults) completely overlapped with the behavior scores of the rats that did exhibit dopamine transients. However, comparison of behavioral responses between age groups revealed that adolescents as a group displayed significantly less movement at unexpected stimulus presentations than the adult rats (Mann-Whitney test, p < 0.05).

стол 2

Behavioral scores for adolescent and adult rats at the presentation of unexpected stimuli (0 = no movement; 1 = sniffing/head movement; 2 = orientation movement/startle; 3 = locomotion).

Dopamine transients during social interaction

Next we measured dopamine release during 60-s interaction with another male rat. In contrast to the presentations of the non-social stimuli, regression analysis determined that dopamine transients significantly increased from baseline during the first interaction with another rat in both adolescent and adult rats. The average rate of transients increased 3-fold from baseline, from 1.0 ± 0.3 to 3.0 ± 0.9 transients/min in adolescent rats (Wald contrast of adolescent baseline vs. interaction, p < 0.05) and from 2.0 ± 0.5 to 7.3 ± 1.3 in adults (Wald contrast of adult baseline vs. interaction, p < 0.001). Ten min later, dopamine release was measured during a second interaction with the same rat. In adults, the increase in dopamine transient rate was no longer significant (p > 0.32); although the average rate of transients increased from 1.8 ± 0.5 to 3.7 ± 1.3 transients/min, this change was more variable across rats. In contrast, adolescents exhibited the same increase in dopamine transients during the second presentation as during the first, from 0.8 ± 0.2 to 3.1 ± 0.9 transients/min (p < 0.01). Individual rat data by group and episode are shown in Figure 2A.

Figure 2

Dopamine release events in adolescent and adult rats during brief interaction with a size-matched rat. (A) Dopamine transients were more frequent in both adolescent and adult rats during the first interaction with another rat (“Rat”) versus ...

We investigated whether differences in behavior during the first and second interaction episode might explain the lack of increase in dopamine release in adult rats during the second interaction. стол 3 shows the time spent in social investigation and locomotion during the 45 s of scored interaction period. In this experimental chamber (21 × 21 cm floorspace), adolescent rats spent less time in active social interactions (2-way repeated-measures ANOVA: main effect of group, p < 0.05) and more time in locomotion (2-way repeated-measures ANOVA: main effect of group, p < 0.05) than did adult rats. However, we observed that the adolescent rats had more space to move apart from each other while the larger adult rats were more likely to be in physical proximity. Thus, the critical comparisons were paired t-tests to compare within-group behavior during the first versus second interaction periods. These analyses revealed no behavioral differences between the first and second interaction periods in either age group (all p values > 0.05). Moreover, neither the absolute frequency of dopamine transients nor the increased frequency from baseline correlated with social interactions or locomotion during the interaction period (data not shown, all p values > 0.05).

стол 3

Time (s) spent in social investigation and locomotion during interaction with another rat.

The maximal concentration of dopamine transients during interaction with another rat was compared to concentrations during baseline. For this analysis, data from both interaction episodes were pooled to increase statistical power. Figure 2B shows that the distribution of dopamine transient amplitude was shifted toward larger release events in both adolescent and adult rats during conspecific interaction; average and median amplitudes are listed in стол 1. Because social interactions can produce movement-associated noise in the voltammetric signal, we also investigated noise levels and found that they were indeed higher during interaction episodes in both adult and adolescent rats (Wald contrasts of baseline vs. interaction, p < 0.05 for each group). Nevertheless, the signal-to-noise ratios did not differ across groups (стол 1: Wald contrasts, all p values > 0.05), suggesting that noise issues did not contribute to the differential findings between adolescent and adult rats.

All recordings were made in the core of the NAc, as shown in Figure 3.

Figure 3

Dopaminergic recording sites within the nucleus accumbens core of adolescent and adult rats, shown on representative coronal slices at 1.2 and 1.6 mm anterior to bregma (adapted from Paxinos жана Уотсон, 1986).

талкулоо

Dopamine neurotransmission is key to many aspects of motivated behavior, including stimulus salience, reward prediction and behavioral facilitation. As motivated behavior differs between adolescents and adults, the present study surveyed fast dopamine release events in early adolescent rats in comparison to adults. We report that while basal rates of dopamine transients do not differ significantly between the two age groups, transients in response to unexpected stimuli are fewer in adolescent rats as compared to adults. In contrast, both frequency and amplitude of dopamine transients increase in both age groups at interaction with another rat; however, the change in frequency habituates in adults but not adolescents at the second presentation of the partner rat. Thus, fast dopamine release events at the presentation of stimuli differ in adolescent rats versus adults, and this physiological difference may be associated with age-dependent differences in the processing of social and nonsocial stimuli.

Both frequencies and amplitudes of dopamine transients in the NAc core were similar between male rats in early adolescence (age 29 – 30 days) and adulthood (age 71 – 72 days). These findings are consistent with electrophysiological recordings of dopamine neurons in anesthetized rats. Firing rates of dopamine neurons increase from birth to mid-late adolescence, then decline across adulthood (Pitts et al., 1990; Теппер .Удаалаш., 1990; Lavin and Drucker-Colin, 1991; Marinelli .Удаалаш., 2006; МакКатчен жана Marinelli, 2009), with peak activity occurring in mid to late adolescence. Indeed, McCutcheon and Marinelli (2009) reported that basal firing rates are similar at the early adolescent and adult ages targeted in the present study, and the data herein suggest that rates of neuronal bursting are also similar, as dopamine transients arise from burst firing of dopamine neurons (Suaud-Chagny et al., 1992; Sombers .Удаалаш., 2009). Notably, adolescent rats exhibited variability in the basal frequency of dopamine transients, ranging from sites with few to no spontaneous transients to sites with several per minute. This finding is similar to “hot” and “cold” recording sites reported in adults (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2009; Robinson and Wightman, 2007; Wightman et al., 2007) and may reflect variability in burst rates of dopamine neurons (Хайланд .Удаалаш., 2002). Consistent with developmental firing rates, microdialysis studies have reported increased dopamine levels in later adolescence (45 days of age) as compared to early adolescence or adulthood (Badanich .Удаалаш., 2006; Philpot .Удаалаш., 2009). Thus, while our findings provide the first evaluation of basal rates of dopamine transients between adolescent and adult rats, more timepoints are needed during this dynamic developmental period as dopamine transients might be more prominent during mid-late adolescence.

Consistent with our previous report (Robinson and Wightman, 2004), we found that the frequency of dopamine transients increased at the unexpected presentation of stimuli in adult male rats, often time-locked to the initial presentation of the stimulus. In the former study, we presented odor and auditory stimuli similar to those used herein; in both studies, these stimuli increased the frequency of dopamine transients in the NAc above basal rates, which was interpreted as a neurochemical signal of potential salience of the stimuli to the animal. However, the increase in dopamine transient frequency in response to unexpected presentation of stimuli observed in adults was not reliably present in the NAc core of early adolescent rats, and the adolescents as a group displayed less behavioral response to the stimuli than did adults. Although phasic activation of dopaminergic neurons is not correlated with specific motor movements, the types of stimuli that tend to promote burst firing and dopamine transients often induce behavioral activation (Nishino .Удаалаш., 1987; Доступности жана Schultz, 1990; Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2002). Thus, the present findings indicate a developmental difference in the dopaminergic and behavioral response to this type of novel stimulus presentation, which may both be due to a lack of salience of these stimuli for the adolescent rats. Importantly, the reduced behavioral response to stimuli was not simply a reduction in the ability to move (perhaps due to the tether or the voltammetric headstage), as the adolescent rats exhibited both locomotion and socially directed behaviors during social interaction. Moreover, there was no association between behavioral response and the frequency of dopamine transients to stimuli when analyzed across individual rats within each age group. The relative insensitivity to unexpected, novel stimuli found in the present study contrasts with prior findings that adolescent rats often exhibit higher levels of exploration of novel environments and novel objects than adults (e.g., Дуглас .Удаалаш., 2003; Байрак жана Kirstein, 2006; Philpot and Wecker, 2008), with novel object exploration reported to peak in rats in mid-adolescence (35 – 36 days of age, Spear .Удаалаш., 1980). Additional studies would be necessary to determine ontogenetic differences between behavioral activation and concurrent dopamine release events induced by the brief stimulus presentations used herein versus responses to novel environments and static, novel objects placed into familiar environments as used in the previous studies.

Both the frequency and amplitude of dopamine transients in the NAc core reliably increased during brief interaction with another rat in both adolescent and adult groups. Consistent with our previous report (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2002), the dopaminergic response habituated in adult rats at the second presentation of the conspecific rat. In contrast, the increased frequency of dopamine transients persisted in adolescent rats. This lack of habituation may reflect the increased reward associated with social interaction exhibited in adolescent versus adult rats (e.g., Дуглас жана башкалар, 2004). Indeed, social activity has been shown to peak in early adolescence compared to later adolescence and adulthood, an effect that is magnified when rats are isolate-housed for the days before the testing (Варлинская жана Найза, 2008), as was done in the present study. Interestingly, the two age groups differed on the overall amount of social activity versus general locomotion. While previous reports indicate that isolate-housed adolescent rats exhibit more social behavior and locomotion than adults during discrete social interaction trials, with these effects being particularly pronounced among early adolescents (Варлинская жана Найза, 2008), we found that locomotion was higher and social-directed behavior was lower in the early adolescents we tested when compared with their adult counterparts. This may be due to the size of the apparatus: at 21 × 21 cm, the larger adult rats were more likely to be in close proximity to the conspecific partner than the smaller adolescent rats, making social contact almost inevitable for these adult animals. In addition, having only the test rat tethered may have affected its behavioral repertoire during social interaction. Finally, the short time period of interaction (60 s) used herein captures only initial interactions which may produce different age-related patterns of social behavior when compared to the longer interaction periods (270–600 s) typically used in terms of rodent social interactions (e.g., Варлинская жана Найза, 2008; Glenn et al, 2003).

Social interaction can induce movement-associated electrical noise as the headstage assembly touches the chamber wall or the other rat which may lead to underestimation of dopamine transient frequency during social interaction. Importantly, signal-to noise levels were not different between age groups or between the first and second interaction periods, indicating that the persistent increase in dopamine transient frequency observed in adolescent rats during the second interaction period was not a noise-related artifact. Similarly, investigative and locomotor behavior was essentially the same during both interactions within each age group, so behavioral differences also do not explain the difference in habituation of the dopaminergic response between adolescent and adult rats. Indeed, in our previous study (Робинсон .Удаалаш., 2002), the observation was made that adult rats exhibited more intense socially directed behaviors during a second interaction with a partner rat despite emitting fewer dopamine transients, suggesting that the dopamine transients are not necessary to promote partner-directed behaviors. If the dopamine transients are interpreted as signals of reward prediction (Schultz жана Dickinson, 2000; Schultz, 2007; Roesch .Удаалаш., 2010), the habituation of dopamine release events in adults to repeated partner presentations may reflect a decrease in reward or greater predictability of the second presentation, and the persistence of dopamine release in the adolescent rats may reflect enhanced reward or surprise to repeated interaction with a partner.

The mesolimbic dopamine system is involved in appetitive behaviors and reward acquisition (for reviews, see Depue and Iacono, 1989; Panksepp, 1998; Депу жана Коллинз, 1999; Ikemoto жана Panksepp, 1999; Schultz жана Dickinson, 2000; Schultz, 2007). As several aspects of this dopamine pathway undergo dynamic changes during adolescence, it is not surprising that behavioral and neurochemical responses to rewards and novel stimuli that may well predict rewards are also dynamic (for reviews, see Бөлмөлөр ж.б.., 2003; Эрнст .Удаалаш., 2009; Wahlstrom et al., 2010b; Wahlstrom et al., 2010a). Our finding that the dopaminergic response to social interaction did not habituate in early adolescent rats is consistent with many studies that have documented heightened sensitivity to rewards during adolescence, including social and drug reward (for review and references, see Doremus-Fitzwater et al, 2010; Найза менен Varlinskaya, 2010). Novel stimuli likewise are salient and may trigger dopamine release and behavioral facilitation because they could predict reward or threat; however, we did not observe increased dopamine release to brief presentation of novel stimuli in early adolescent rats. Thus, the present study may have sampled dopamine release at a time in development (early adolescence) during which social reward sensitivity is optimal but response to novelty is not. This interpretation leads to several avenues of further research, including examination of dopamine release to novel and social stimuli at more timepoints across adolescence. We also plan to investigate dopamine release during explicit cue-reward learning (e.g., Pavlovian conditioning) to determine whether the types of stimuli presentations used herein, such as a light or odor, can evoke a dopaminergic response in adolescent rats when they predict reward (Day, ж.б. 2007; Roesch .Удаалаш., 2007).

In summary, fast dopamine release events, or dopamine transients, are differentially expressed in early adolescence versus adulthood. While rates and concentrations of transients were similar at baseline, we observed less activation of dopamine release by unexpected, nonsocial stimuli and more persistent activation by social stimuli in adolescent rats as compared to adults. These differences in dopamine release events likely contribute to developmental differences in sensitivity to cues and rewards, particularly social reward. It will be valuable to build on these findings by evaluating more timepoints during adolescence as well as monitoring dopamine release during explicit reward-associated learning.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Dr. Thomas Guillot III for assistance with neuroanatomical coordinates, to Rachel Hay and Sebastian Cerdena for behavioral scoring, to Vahid Sanii for electrode calibration, and to Chris Wiesen at the UNC Odum Institute for Research in Social Science for statistical expertise. This work was funded by NIH (R01DA019071 to L.P.S.) and the Bowles Center for Alcohol Studies at the University of North Carolina.

кыскартуулар

[DA]макс
maximal concentration of dopamine
НАК
ядро accumbens

шилтемелер

  • Андерсен SL, коюлду M, Benzo JM, Хостеттер JC, Teicher MH. тинейджер кабылдагыч ашыра жана четтетүү боюнча Sex айырмачылыктар. Neuroreport. 1997;8: 1495-1498. [PubMed]
  • Badanich KA, Адлер KJ, Kirstein CL. ядро septi accumbens өспүрүмдөрү кокаин шартталган жер артыкчылык жана кокаин-азгырган тинейджер улуу айырмаланат. Eur J Pharmacol. 2006;550: 95-106. [PubMed]
  • Chambers RA, Тейлор JR, Potenza MN. Өспүрүм менен түрткү өрчүү neurocircuitry: көз карандылык аялуу оор мезгил. American J Psychiat. 2003;160: 1041-1052.
  • Day JJ, Ройтман MF, Уайтман RM, Клейн RM. Associative окутуу ядросу accumbens менен тинейджер сигналдаштыруунун динамикалык жылыштарга ортомчулук кылат. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10: 1020-1028. [PubMed]
  • Depue RA, Iacono WG. Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders. Annu Аян Psychol. 1989;40: 457-492. [PubMed]
  • Depue RA, Collins PF. Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22: 491-517. discussion 518–469. [PubMed]
  • Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Motivational systems in adolescence: Possible implications for age differences in substance abuse and other risk-taking behaviors. Brain Cogn. 2010;72: 114-123. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Дуглас LA, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Өспүрүм жана бойго жеткен эркек жана ургаачы келемиштерде роман-объекттин ордун кондициялоо: социалдык обочолонуунун таасири. Physiol Behav. 2003;80: 317-325. [PubMed]
  • Дуглас LA, Varlinskaya EI, Spear LP. Өспүрүм жана бойго жеткен эркек жана ургаачы келемиштердеги социалдык өз ара мамилелердин сыйлоо касиеттери: субъекттердин жана өнөктөштөрдүн обочолонгон турак-жайына каршы социалдык таасири. Dev Psychobiol. 2004;45: 153-162. [PubMed]
  • Ernst M, Ромео RD, Андерсен SL. бир нейрон системалар моделдин бир терезеден: жүйөлүү жүрүш өнүктүрүү өспүрүм жылдын Нейробиология. Pharmacol BIOCHEM Behav. 2009;93: 199-211. [PubMed]
  • Glenn RF, Tucci SA, Thomas A, Edwards JE, File SE. Age-associated sex differences in response to food deprivation in two animal tests of anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Аян 2003;27: 155-161. [PubMed]
  • Hyland BI, Reynolds JN, Hay J, Perk CG, Miller R. Firing modes of midbrain dopamine cells in the freely moving rat. Neuroscience. 2002;114: 475-492. [PubMed]
  • Ikemoto S, Panksepp J. The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Рез Brain Рез Аян 1999;31: 6-41. [PubMed]
  • Lavin MA, Drucker-Colin R. Ontogeny of the electrophysiological activity of dopaminergic cells with special reference to the influence of adrenal medullary grafts on aging. Brain Рез. 1991;545: 164-170. [PubMed]
  • Logman MJ, Budygin EA, Gainetdinov RR, Wightman RM. Quantitation of in vivo measurements with carbon fiber microelectrodes. J Neurosci ыкмалары. 2000;95: 95-102. [PubMed]
  • Marinelli M, Rudick CN Ху XT, Ак FJ. тездиги жана психологиялык кесепеттерин: тинейджер нейрон жандануусу. CNS Neurol Disord Drug максаттар. 2006;5: 79-97. [PubMed]
  • МакКатчен се, Marinelli M. Жашы маселелер. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;29: 997-1014. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Nishino H, Ono T, Muramoto K, Fukuda M, Sasaki K. Neuronal activity in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) during motivated bar press feeding in the monkey. Brain Рез. 1987;413: 302-313. [PubMed]
  • Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press; New York: 1998.
  • Paxinos G, Уотсон C. stereotaxic координаттарында келемиш мээ. Academic; New York: 1986.
  • Phillips PE, Stuber GD, Heien ML, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Subsecond dopamine release promotes cocaine seeking. Nature. 2003;422: 614-618. [PubMed]
  • Philpot RM, Wecker L. Өспүрүмдөрдүн жаңычылдыкка умтулуу жүрүм-турумунун жооптун фенотипине жана аппараттын масштабынын эффекттерине көз карандылыгы. Behav neurosci. 2008;122: 861-875. [PubMed]
  • Philpot RM, Wecker L, Kirstein CL. Repeated ethanol exposure during adolescence alters the developmental trajectory of dopaminergic output from the nucleus accumbens septi. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2009;27: 805-815. [PubMed]
  • Питтс DK, Freeman AS, Chiodo LA. Тинейджер нейрон Ontogeny: electrophysiological изилдөөлөр. Synapse. 1990;6: 309-320. [PubMed]
  • Rebec GV, Christensen JR, Guerra C, Bardo MT. Regional and temporal differences in real-time dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens during free-choice novelty. Brain Рез. 1997;776: 61-67. [PubMed]
  • Robinson DL, Wightman RM. Nomifensine amplifies subsecond dopamine signals in the ventral striatum of freely-moving rats. J Neurochem. 2004;90: 894-903. [PubMed]
  • Robinson DL, Heien ML, Wightman RM. Frequency of dopamine concentration transients increases in dorsal and ventral striatum of male rats during introduction of conspecifics. J Neurosci. 2002;22: 10477-10486. [PubMed]
  • Robinson DL, Hermans A, Seipel AT, Wightman RM. Monitoring Rapid Chemical Communication in the Brain. Chem Rev. 2008;108: 2554-2584. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Robinson DL, Howard EC, McConnell S, Gonzales RA, Wightman RM. Disparity between tonic and phasic ethanol-induced dopamine increases in the nucleus accumbens of rats. Ичкилик Clin Exp Рез. 2009;33: 1187-1196. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Robinson DL, Phillips PE, Budygin EA, Trafton BJ, Garris PA, Wightman RM. Sub-second changes in accumbal dopamine during sexual behavior in male rats. Neuroreport. 2001;12: 2549-2552. [PubMed]
  • Robinson DL, Wightman RM. Rapid dopamine release in freely moving rats. In: Michael AC, Borland LM, editors. Electrochemical Methods for Neuroscience. CRC Press; Boca Raton: 2007. pp. 17–34.
  • Roesch MR, Calu DJ, Esber GR, Schoenbaum G. All that glitters … dissociating attention and outcome expectancy from prediction errors signals. J Neurophysiol. 2010;104: 587-595. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Roesch MR, Calu DJ, Schoenbaum G. Dopamine neurons encode the better option in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10: 1615-1624. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Roitman MF, Wheeler RA, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Real-time chemical responses in the nucleus accumbens differentiate rewarding and aversive stimuli. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11: 1376-1377. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Roitman MF, Stuber GD, Phillips PE, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Dopamine operates as a subsecond modulator of food seeking. J Neurosci. 2004;24: 1265-1271. [PubMed]
  • Доступности R, маймыл midbrain боюнча Schultz W. тинейджер нейрондор: Өзүн-өзү демилгелеген колу кыймылдар учурунда активдүү тийүү жоопторду кырдаалдар. J Neurophysiol. 1990;63: 592-606. [PubMed]
  • Schultz W. ар кандай убакыт курстарда нече тинейджер иштейт. Annu Аян Neurosci. 2007;30: 259-288. [PubMed]
  • Schultz W, Dickinson A. Neuronal coding of prediction errors. Annu Аян Neurosci. 2000;23: 473-500. [PubMed]
  • Sombers LA, Beyene M, Carelli RM, Wightman RM. Synaptic overflow of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens arises from neuronal activity in the ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci. 2009;29: 1735-1742. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Spear LP, Varlinskaya EI. Sensitivity to ethanol and other hedonic stimuli in an animal model of adolescence: implications for prevention science? Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52: 236-243. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Spear LP, Shalaby IA, Brick J. Chronic administration of haloperidol during development: behavioral and psychopharmacological effects. Psychopharmacol. 1980;70: 47-58.
  • Байрак KH, Kirstein CL. өспүрүм жана жашы жеткен Келемиштердин жүрүм табуунун Effects. Dev Psychobiol. 2006;48: 10-15. [PubMed]
  • Stuber GD, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Extinction of cocaine self-administration reveals functionally and temporally distinct dopaminergic signals in the nucleus accumbens. Нейрон. 2005a;46: 661-669. [PubMed]
  • Stuber GD, Roitman MF, Phillips PE, Carelli RM, Wightman RM. Rapid dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens during contingent and noncontingent cocaine administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005b;30: 853-863. [PubMed]
  • Suaud-Chagny MF, Chergui K, Chouvet G, Gonon F. Relationship between dopamine release in the rat nucleus accumbens and the discharge activity of dopaminergic neurons during local in vivo application of amino acids in the ventral tegmental area. Neuroscience. 1992;49: 63-72. [PubMed]
  • Теппер JM, Трент F, Nakamura S. Т¼р¼тт¼н кийинки чычкан nigrostriatal dopaminergic нейрондордун электр ишин өнүктүрүү. Brain Рез Dev Brain Рез. 1990;54: 21-33.
  • Варлинская Е.И., Найза Л.П. Өспүрүмдөр жана чоңдор Спраге-Доули келемиштериндеги социалдык өз ара аракеттенүү: социалдык жокчулуктун таасири жана контексттик таанышуу. Behav Brain Жен. 2008;188: 398-405. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Wahlstrom D, White T, Luciana M. Neurobehavioral evidence for changes in dopamine system activity during adolescence. Neurosci Biobehav Аян 2010a;34: 631-648. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Wahlstrom D, Collins P, White T, Luciana M. Developmental changes in dopamine neurotransmission in adolescence: behavioral implications and issues in assessment. Brain Cogn. 2010b;72: 146-159. [КУП акысыз макала] [PubMed]
  • Wightman RM, Heien ML, Wassum KM, Sombers LA, Aragona BJ, Khan AS, Ariansen JL, Cheer JF, Phillips PE, Carelli RM. Dopamine release is heterogeneous within microenvironments of the rat nucleus accumbens. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26: 2046-2054. [PubMed]
  • Wightman RM, Robinson DL. Transient changes in mesolimbic dopamine and their association with ‘reward’ J Neurochem. 2002;82: 721-735. [PubMed]