Stimuli Sesswali Viżwali — CU or jew Reward? Perspettiva għall-Interpretazzjoni tas-Sejbiet tal-Immaġini tal-Moħħ dwar Imgieba Sesswali Umani (2016)

Mateusz Gola1,2 *, Małgorzata Wordecha2,3, Artur Marchewka3 u, Guillaume Sescousse4

  • 1Swartz Center for Computational Newroscience, Istitut għall-Kompjutazzjoni Newrali, Università ta’ California San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA
  • 2Istitut tal-Psikoloġija, Akkademja tax-Xjenzi Pollakka, Varsavja, il-Polonja
  • 3Laboratory of Brain Imaging, Neurobiology Center, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology of Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
  • 4Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands

There is an increasing number of neuroimaging studies using visual sexual stimuli (VSS), especially within the emerging field of research on compulsive sexual behaviors (CSB). A central question in this field is whether behaviors such as excessive pornography consumption share common brain mechanisms with widely studied substance and behavioral addictions. Depending on how VSS are conceptualized, different predictions can be formulated within the frameworks of Reinforcement Learning or Incentive Salience Theory, where a crucial distinction is made between ikkundizzjonat u, bla kundizzjonijiet stimuli (related to reward anticipation vs. reward consumption, respectively). Surveying 40 recent human neuroimaging studies we show existing ambiguity about the conceptualization of VSS. Therefore, we feel that it is important to address the question of whether VSS should be considered as conditioned stimuli (cue) or unconditioned stimuli (reward). Here we present our own perspective, which is that in most laboratory settings VSS play a role of jippremjaw, as evidenced by:

(1) experience of pleasure while watching VSS, possibly accompanied by genital reaction;

(2) reward-related brain activity correlated with these pleasurable feelings in response to VSS;

(3) a willingness to exert effort to view VSS similarly as for other rewarding stimuli such as money; and

(4) conditioning for cues predictive of VSS.

We hope that this perspective article will initiate a scientific discussion on this important and overlooked topic and increase attention for appropriate interpretations of results of human neuroimaging studies using VSS.

There is an increasing number of neuroimaging studies using visual sexual stimuli (VSS, Figure 1A). VSS are often used as pleasant, arousing stimuli that have an intrinsic positive value (see Wierzba et al., 2015). Brain reactivity triggered by VSS is often interpreted within popular theoretical frameworks describing learning processes or motivated behavior such as Reinforcement Learning (Sutton u Barto, 1998; Botvinick et al., 2009) or Incentive Salience Theory (Robinson u Berridge, 1993; Berridge, 2012). Importantly, these theories make a key distinction between conditioned stimuli (CS) u, unconditioned stimuli (UCS), which are related to reward anticipation/wanting vs. reward consumption/liking, respectively. Accordingly, it is important to make explicit whether VSS play a role of CS or UCS, i.e., whether they are incentive cues predicting an upcoming reward, or whether they are rewarding by themselves. This issue has been surprisingly overlooked in past studies, despite its important implications. We reviewed 40 human studies published between 2013 and 2016, using VSS in combination with neuroscience methods (fMRI, EEG, ERP, PET, MEG or TMS; Figure 1B):

• Nine studies described VSS as cues/CS: (Minnix et al., 2013; Politis et al., 2013; Steele et al., 2013; Kühn u Gallinat, 2014; Oei et al., 2014; Voon et al., 2014; Wetherill et al., 2014; Prause et al., 2015; Seok u Sohn, 2015).

• Sixteen studies described VSS as rewards/UCS: (Costumero et al., 2013, 2015a,b; Graf et al., 2013; Klucken et al., 2013, 2015, 2016; Sescousse et al., 2013a; Cassidy et al., 2014; Li et al., 2014; Mascaro et al., 2014; Oei et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2015; Banca et al., 2016; Brand et al., 2016; Schöne et al., 2016).

• One study described VSS as both as CS and UCS: (Oei et al., 2014).

• Fifteen studies did not use any such labels: (Abler et al., 2013; Chung et al., 2013; Habermeyer et al., 2013; Hernández-González et al., 2013; Sylva et al., 2013; Wehrum et al., 2013; Borg et al., 2014; Prause et al., 2014; Kim and Jeong, 2013, 2014; Wehrum-Osinsky et al., 2014; Flaisch et al., 2015; Amezcua-Gutiérrez et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2016; Knott et al., 2016).

 
IL-figura 1
www.frontiersin.org  

Figura 1. (A) Blue bars indicate the number of human studies using neuroscience methods (fMRI, EEG, ERP, PET, MEG or TMS) and visual sexual stimuli (VSS) published between 2000 and 2016 according to PubMed (accessed on March 31st 2016). Red bars indicate the number of neuroscience studies on compulsive sexual behaviors (CSB): 1 in 2013 (Steele et al., 2013), 2 in 2014 (Kühn u Gallinat, 2014; Voon et al., 2014), 1 in 2015 (Prause et al., 2015), and 3 in 2016 (Banca et al., 2016; Brand et al., 2016; Klucken et al., 2016). (B) Number of studies published between 2013 and 2016 interpreting VSS as cue, reward or none of these labels (unspecified). Note that in Oei et al. (2014) VSS were defined both as “reward cues” and “rewarding stimuli”, so it was counted in both categories “Cue” and ”Reward”.

The Incentive Salience Theory framework, proposed by Robinson and Berridge (1993), distinguishes two basic components of motivated behavior—“wanting” and “liking”. The latter is directly linked to the esperjenza value of the reward (UCS), while the former is related to the mistennija value of the reward, often carried by a predictive cue (CS). Studies on substance and gambling addiction show that learned cues (CS) related to addiction evoke increased responses in the ventral striatum as well as increased motivated behavior (i.e., shorter reaction times, higher accuracy) among addicted individuals, while responses to the reward itself remain unchanged or undergo blunting over time (Berridge, 2012; Robinson et al., 2015).

Thus, the conceptualization of VSS as cues or rewards in experimental designs is not just a semantic debate, because it has important consequences for the interpretation of neuroimaging results. One important consequence is on the emerging field of neuroscientific research on compulsive sexual behaviors (CSB; Love et al., 2015; Kraus et al., 2016a,b; Figura 1). A central question in this field is whether CSBs (such as excessive pornography consumption Gola et al., 2016a,b) share common brain mechanisms with widely studied substance and behavioral addictions (Love et al., 2015; Gola u Potenza, 2016; Gola et al., 2016c; Kraus et al., 2016b). Depending on how VSS are conceptualized, different predictions can be formulated. If one assumes that VSS play a role of cue, then increased ventral striatal reactivity among subjects with CSB (in comparison with controls) would speak in favor of the addiction hypothesis, while under the assumption that VSS play a role of reward, it is the opposite result (decreased ventral striatal reactivity) that would speak in favor of the same hypothesis. Therefore we feel that it is important to address the question of whether VSS should be considered as cues (CS) or rewards (UCS) in human studies. Here we present our own perspective, hoping that it will initiate a scientific discussion on this topic.

To answer this question we think it is important to distinguish the meaning of VSS in real life vs. in the laboratory setting (Figure 2). In many real life situations, VSS such as the naked body of a sexually attractive partner increase sexual arousal and lead to approach behaviors initializing dyadic sexual activity and ending with orgasm (Georgiadis u Kringelbach, 2012; Gola et al., 2015a). In this case, we argue that VSS play a role of cue (CS), while orgasm plays the role of (primary) reward (UCS). The reasoning is similar in most cases of solitary sexual activity. Most common VSS are pornographic videos or photos (cue/CS), which increase sexual arousal, and lead to masturbation ending with orgasm (reward/UCS). In contrast, during laboratory experiments, subjects are usually not allowed to initiate any sexual activity (such as masturbation) and natural UCS—orgasm—is unavailable. Even if subjects would be allowed to masturbate during the study, laboratory conditions are far less comfortable than the usual context of pornography consumption or dyadic sexual activity. Thus, individuals participating in laboratory experiments do not expect any other reward than being exposed to VSS. Therefore, we posit that in laboratory setting VSS play a role of reward (UCS; Figure 2). The conceptualization of VSS as rewards in the context of laboratory experiments comes with several predictions. Among healthy subjects we should observe: (1) experience of pleasure while watching VSS, possibly accompanied by genital reaction; (2) reward-related brain activity correlated with these pleasurable feelings in response to VSS; (3) a willingness to exert effort to view VSS similarly as for other rewarding stimuli such as money; and (4) conditioning of cues (CS) predictive of VSS. Below we review evidence supporting these predictions.

 
IL-figura 2
www.frontiersin.org  

Figure 2. Visual sexual stimuli (VSS) act as cues in real life, but rewards in the lab. According to our perspective, in most real life situations (such as sexual activity with partner or solitary pornography consumption) VSS such as the naked body of a sexually attractive partner or pornographic content play a role of cue (CS). VSS increase sexual arousal and lead to behaviors initializing dyadic or solitary sexual activity and ending with reward—i.e., orgasm (UCS). In contrast, in most laboratory settings sexual activity and orgasm are unavailable. We claim that VSS then play a role of reward (UCS), similarly to some real life situations such as the visit to a strip club. In such contexts, individuals do not expect any other reward than being exposed to VSS, and are willing to exert effort or pay money to receive desired VSS, while being susceptible to conditioning for cues predictive of these VSS. For the purpose of illustration of our ideas this figure presents a simplified representation of real life where other scenarios of VSS use are possible, i.e., pornography consumption may lead to dyadic sexual activity or vice versa. Credits of sample photos: Lies Thru a Lens; Strip club in Montreal, Quebec, in Saint Henri borough; Lola Bel Aire, striptease from Miss Exotic World 2008, CC BY 2.0. For license terms see: CC BY 2.0 (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0/).

 

In studies collecting hedonic ratings of VSS, subjects consistently report that watching VSS is a subjectively pleasurable experience when these match the subjects’ sexual preferences (Chivers and Bailey, 2005; Rupp u Wallen, 2009; Jacob et al., 2011; Wierzba et al., 2015). In addition, these hedonic ratings have been shown to be accompanied by genital reactions as measured by penile plethysmography in male participants (Stoléru et al., 1999; Redouté et al., 2000; Ferretti et al., 2005). Erectile reaction among males takes some time so it is easier to observe it with long-lasting VSS such as videos or long presentations of pictures (Ferretti et al., 2005), however even brief presentations of static sexual photographies are related with subjective pleasure and arousal (Ferretti et al., 2005; Wierzba et al., 2015).

Many studies have shown that passive VSS viewing evokes ventral striatum activity (Arnow et al., 2002; Stark et al., 2005; Sabatinelli et al., 2007; Demos et al., 2012; Georgiadis u Kringelbach, 2012; Stoléru et al., 2012; Wehrum-Osinsky et al., 2014). It is difficult to assess whether striatal activity reflects cue related jixtiequ or reward related Predisposizzjoni in these studies given that the ventral striatum is known to respond to both appetitive cues (CS) and rewards (UCS; Flagel et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2011) However, the observed correlation between striatal activity and hedonic ratings triggered by VSS in various studies (Walter et al., 2008; Sescousse et al., 2010, 2013b) favors the hypothesis that VSS act like rewarding stimuli. In this respect, VSS play a similar role as monetary rewards: they activate similar brain areas including the ventral striatum, and trigger comparable hedonic reactions and motivated behaviors (Sescousse et al., 2010, 2013b, 2015). The main difference is that VSS are primary rewards (i.e., they have an intrinsic and innate reward value), whereas money is a secondary reward (whose value is learned by exchange against other rewards). This difference leads to a partially different mapping onto the brain reward system, and different strengths of activation (Sescousse et al., 2010, 2013b, 2015).

Even though most studies using VSS have used passive viewing paradigms, a few investigations have employed more advanced experimental designs aiming to measure participants’ willingness to exert effort towards VSS. In a series of studies, we have used a modified version of the monetary incentive delay task (Knutson et al., 2001) to include VSS (Sescousse et al., 2010, 2013a, 2015; Gola et al., 2015b, 2016c). In this task subjects see two types of cues that are predictive of either VSS or monetary gains. These cues are followed by a discrimination task in which subjects have to press the correct button (out of two) within a time limit of 1 s. The receipt of a monetary gain or VSS is partly dependent on their performance on this task, such that reaction times can be interpreted as an indirect measure of the motivation to obtain these rewards. Importantly, cues predicting VSS elicit similar reaction times as those predicting monetary rewards, demonstrating that participants are willing to exert effort to view VSS, and that their motivation is similar for both rewards (Sescousse et al., 2010). This willingness to exert effort, which is a hallmark of reward (Thorndike, 1965), has been observed in other studies using effort (but also delay) discounting paradigms with VSS (Prévost et al., 2010). In addition, we have shown that individual differences in the effort exerted for money vs. VSS is strongly correlated with the relative brain activity evoked by corresponding cues in the ventral striatum (Sescousse et al., 2015; Gola et al., 2016c). This precise fine-tuning of brain activity and reaction times by VSS predicting cues further confirms that VSS have intrinsically rewarding properties.

Finally, recent studies have shown that abstract CS (such as colorful patterns or dots) associated with VSS maintain their incentive salience even when they are not predictive of VSS anymore (Banca et al., 2016; Klucken et al., 2016). Fl - istudju minn Banca et al. (2016), abstract visual patterns acquired positive predictive value (CS+) or neutral predictive value (CS−) by being repetitively paired with VSS or neutral stimuli, respectively. In the following phase of the experiment, subjects had to make choices between those CS and novel abstract stimuli, while both CS were now paired with increased chances of monetary gains (but not VSS anymore). Despite both CS having equal chances of leading to monetary gains, CS+ were chosen more frequently than CS− on average (mostly by subjects with CSB), demonstrating the strong rewarding properties of VSS.

As we have shown above, there is a consistent body of evidence supporting our view that in laboratory settings VSS play a role of reward rather than cue. Moreover, even in everyday life VSS do not always play a role of cue for sexual activity and orgasm. Long before the development of photography people have liked art such as sculptures and paintings depicting nudity. Perhaps (similarly to modern times) this type of art was a source of pleasure rather than cue for sexual activity. In the era of photography people showed willingness to pay for pictures and videos with erotic and pornographic content, then internet technology provided everyone with easy and free access to a whole variety of VSS (Cooper, 1998). Perhaps most of contemporary VSS (such as internet pornography) play a role of cue for solitary or dyadic sexual activity, but in some cases VSS are sought after for themselves, again demonstrating their intrinsic rewarding value. A good example in everyday life are calendars with erotic pictures, that people buy and expose in their workplace or at home. Similarly, the popularity of strip clubs, in which people are willing to pay to watch nude dancers with whom they are not allowed to engage in sexual activity, illustrate the potency of VSS as hedonic stimuli (Figure 2).

Based on the above arguments, we argue that VSS play a role of reward—rather than cue—in most experimental setups in which sexual activity and climax experience are unavailable. As we outlined above, viewing VSS is a pleasurable experience that people are willing to work and wait for (Prévost et al., 2010), and activates the same brain reward regions as monetary gains (Sescousse et al., 2010, 2013a, 2015; Gola et al., 2015b, 2016c). Moreover neutral stimuli associated with VSS through Pavlovian conditioning acquire incentive value (Sescousse et al., 2010, 2013a, 2015; Banca et al., 2016; Gola et al., 2016c; Klucken et al., 2016). This conceptualization of VSS as rewards rather than cues calls for the re-examination—and possibly re-interpretation—of the results reported in earlier studies defining VSS as cues. Certainly it may have a strong impact on the interpretation of neuroimaging studies investigating neurobiological similarities between CSB and addiction; for instance, based on the popular Incentive Salience Theory framework, one would expect opposite ventral striatal reactivity for VSS depending on whether they are conceptualized as a cue or reward (as an example of such ambiguous interpretation see: Prause et al., 2015, 2016; ara ukoll Gola, 2016 for discussion). If in most of experimental setups, as we argue, VSS play a role of reward, then diminished (rather than increased) ventral striatal reactivity to VSS in individuals with problematic pornography use (Gola et al., 2016a) would speak in favor of the addiction hypothesis (Robinson et al., 2015). We would expect this to be accompanied by increased ventral striatum activations for CS that are predictive of VSS, as well as increased effort or shorter reaction times to gain access to these VSS. In future studies, we hope that the role played by VSS in the specific protocols that are used will receive increased attention, and that appropriate interpretations of results will be made accordingly.

Informazzjoni addizzjonali

Method of Study Selection

We searched the Pubmed database from 2000 to 2016 to identify neuroscience publications (key words: fMRI, EEG, ERP, PET, MEG or TMS) with VSS (keywords: VSS, sexual stimuli, erotic stimuli, sexual pictures, erotic pictures, sexual images, erotic images, sexual videos, erotic videos). Only full peer-reviewed publications were selected (no conference abstracts). For studies published between 2013 (year of first publication on problematic pornography use) and 2016 we categorized them into three categories depending on whether VSS were described as: (1) “cue/CS”; (2) “reward/rewarding stimuli/UCS”; and (3) otherwise.

Kwistjonijiet relatati

Here we want to highlight several issues which, if properly investigated, may provide valuable information in the debate on the interpretation of studies using VSS and help to extend the significance of future research.

One of the crucial points is to examine the difference in behavioral and neural responding when VSS are used as cues vs. rewards. It could be done by comparing two experimental conditions in which VSS play a role of reward (most of current experimental settings) or cue (settings allowing subjects to climax during or after the study).

Another interesting hypothesis is that behavior and brain activaty elicited by VSS in typical experimental settings may partly reflect inhibitory control. This inhibitory control may be removed at the end of the experiment, after which subjects may start seeking sexual encounters or initiate solitary sexual activity. For instance, an old behavioral study by Brown et al. (1976) has shown that among heterosexual males, VSS viewing in the laboratory induced masturbation in 24.5% of the subjects on the day of the experiment, while on other days only 12.5% of them engaged in masturbation. This observation suggests that for a fraction of the subjects, watching VSS in the laboratory may have been a cue eliciting sexual motivation that had to be inhibited. To examine such a possibility it would be important to control for sexual activity following experimental studies. Furthermore it raises several questions: does this subgroup differ from other participants, i.e., in terms of sexual arousability (Gola et al., 2015a)? And if so, than does it affect brain activity?

We hope that these questions will inspire investigators and will be addressed in future studies.

Kontribuzzjonijiet tal-Awtur

All authors discussed the idea. MG prepared figures. MW and MG did a review of literature. MG and GS wrote the manuscript. AM and MW commented on the manuscript.

Finanzjament

MG was supported by Opus grant from National Science Centre in Poland (2014/15/B/HS6/03792; MG) and scholarship of Ministry of Science and Higher Education of Republic of Poland (469/STYP/10/2015); MW was supported by Opus grant from National Science Centre in Poland (2014/15/B/HS6/03792; MG); GS was supported by a Veni grant from the Netherlands Research Organization (NWO, ref no. 016.155.218).

Dikjarazzjoni ta 'Kunflitt ta' Interess

L-awturi jiddikjaraw li r-riċerka saret fin-nuqqas ta 'kwalunkwe relazzjoni kummerċjali jew finanzjarja li tista' tinftiehem bħala kunflitt ta 'interess potenzjali.

The reviewers RS and TK declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Referenzi

Abler, B., Kumpfmüller, D., Grön, G., Walter, M., Stingl, J., and Seeringer, A. (2013). Neural correlates of erotic stimulation under different levels of female sexual hormones. PLoS Wieħed 8: e54447. doi: 10.1371 / journal.pone.0054447

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Amezcua-Gutiérrez, C., Ruiz-Díaz, M., Hernández-González, M., Guevara, M. A., Å gmo, A., and Sanz-Martin, A. (2016). Effect of sexual arousal on cortical coupling during performance of the tower of hanoi task in young men. J. Sess Res. 1–11. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2015.1130211 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Arnow, BA, Desmond, JE, Banner, LL, Glover, GH, Solomon, A., Polan, ML, et al. (2002). Attivazzjoni tal-moħħ u tqanqil sesswali f'irġiel b'saħħithom u eterosesswali. Moħħ 125, 1014 – 1023. doi: 10.1093 / moħħ / awf108

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Banca, P., Morris, L. S., Mitchell, S., Harrison, N. A., Potenza, M. N., and Voon, V. (2016). Novelty, conditioning and attentional bias to sexual rewards. J. Psikjatru. Riż. 72, 91 – 101. doi: 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2015.10.017

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C. (2012). From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of reward motivation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1124 – 1143. Doi: 10.1111 / j.1460-9568.2012.07990.x

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Borg, C., de Jong, P. J., and Georgiadis, J. R. (2014). Subcortical BOLD responses during visual sexual stimulation vary as a function of implicit porn associations in women. Soc. Cogn. Affettwa. Newrosci. 9, 158–166. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss117

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M. M., Niv, Y., and Barto, A. C. (2009). Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: a reinforcement learning perspective. Konjizzjoni 113, 262–280. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.011

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Brand, M., Snagowski, J., Laier, C., and Maderwald, S. (2016). Ventral striatum activity when watching preferred pornographic pictures is correlated with symptoms of Internet pornography addiction. Neuroimage 129, 224 – 232. doi: 10.1016 / j.neuroimage.2016.01.033

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Brown, M., Amoroso, D. M., and Ware, E. E. (1976). Behavioral effects of viewing pornography. J. Soc. Psychol. 98, 235 – 245. doi: 10.1080 / 00224545.1976.9923394

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Cassidy, C. M., Brodeur, M. B., Lepage, M., and Malla, A. (2014). Do reward-processing deficits in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders promote cannabis use? An investigation of physiological response to natural rewards and drug cues. J. Psikjatrija Neurosci. 39, 339–347. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130207

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Chivers, M. L., and Bailey, J. M. (2005). A sex difference in features that elicit genital response. Biol. Psychol. 70, 115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.12.002

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Chung, W. S., Lim, S. M., Yoo, J. H., and Yoon, H. (2013). Gender difference in brain activation to audio-visual sexual stimulation; do women and men experience the same level of arousal in response to the same video clip? Int. J. Impot. Riż. 25, 138–142. doi: 10.1038/ijir.2012.47

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Cooper, A. (1998). Sexuality and the internet: surfing into the new millennium. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 1, 187–193. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.187

Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Costumero, V., Barrós-Loscertales, A., Bustamante, J. C., Fuentes, P., Rosell-Negre, P., Ventura-Campos, N., et al. (2015a). A new window to understanding individual differences in reward sensitivity from attentional networks. Moħħ Struct. Funct. 220, 1807–1821. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0760-6

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Costumero, V., Bustamante, J. C., Rosell-Negre, P., Fuentes, P., Llopis, J. J., Ávila, C., et al. (2015b). Reduced activity in functional networks during reward processing is modulated by abstinence in cocaine addicts. Addict. Biol. doi: 10.1111/adb.12329 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Costumero, V., Barrós-Loscertales, A., Bustamante, J. C., Ventura-Campos, N., Fuentes, P., Rosell-Negre, P., et al. (2013). Reward sensitivity is associated with brain activity during erotic stimulus processing. PLoS Wieħed 8: e66940. doi: 10.1371 / journal.pone.0066940

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Demos, K. E., Heatherton, T. F., and Kelley, W. M. (2012). Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. J. Neurosci. 32, 5549–5552. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5958-11.2012

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Ferretti, A., Caulo, M., Del Gratta, C., Di Matteo, R., Merla, A., Montorsi, F., et al. (2005). Dinamika ta 'tqanqil sesswali maskili: komponenti distinti ta' attivazzjoni tal-moħħ żvelati minn fMRI. Neuroimage 26, 1086 – 1096. doi: 10.1016 / j.neuroimage.2005.03.025

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Flagel, S. B., Clark, J. J., Robinson, T. E., Mayo, L., Czuj, A., Willuhn, I., et al. (2011). A selective role for dopamine in stimulus-reward learning. natura 469, 53–57. doi: 10.1038/nature09588

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Flaisch, T., Imhof, M., Schmälzle, R., Wentz, K. U., Ibach, B., and Schupp, H. T. (2015). Implicit and explicit attention to pictures and words: an fMRI-study of concurrent emotional stimulus processing. Quddiem. Psychol. 6: 1861. doi: 10.3389 / fpsyg.2015.01861

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Georgiadis, JR, u Kringelbach, ML (2012). Iċ-ċiklu ta 'rispons sesswali tal-bniedem: evidenza ta' immaġni tal-moħħ li tgħaqqad is-sess ma 'pjaċiri oħra Prog. Neurobiol. 98, 49 – 81. doi: 10.1016 / j.pneurobio.2012.05.004

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Gola, M. (2016). Decreased LPP for sexual images in problematic pornography users may be consistent with addiction models. Everything depends on the model. (Commentary on Prause, Steele, Staley, Sabatinelli, and Hajcak, 2015). Biol. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.05.003 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Gola, M., Kowalewska, E., Wierzba, M., Wordecha, M., and Marchewka, A. (2015a). Polish adaptation of the sexual arousability inventory SAI-PL and validation for males. Psychiatria 12, 245-254.

Gola, M., Miyakoshi, M., and Sescousse, G. (2015b). Sex, impulsivity and anxiety: interplay between ventral striatum and amygdala reactivity in sexual behaviors. J. Neurosci. 35, 15227–15229. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3273-15.2015

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Gola, M., Lewczuk, K., and Skorko, M. (2016a). What matters: quantity or quality of pornography use? Psychological and behavioral factors of seeking treatment for problematic pornography use. J. Sess. Med. 13, 815–824. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2016.02.169

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Gola, M., Skorko, M., Kowalewska, E., Kołodziej, A., Sikora, M., Wodyk, M., et al. (2016b). Polish adaptation of sexual addiction screening test—revised. Pol. Psychiatry 41, 1–21. doi: 10.12740/PP/OnlineFirst/61414

Test sħiħ CrossRef

Gola, M., Wordecha, M., Sescousse, G., Lew-Starowicz, M., Kossowski, B., Wypych, M., et al. (2016c). Can pornography be addictive? An fMRI study of men seeking treatment for problematic pornography use. bioRxiv 057083. doi: 10.1101/057083

Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Gola, M., and Potenza, M. N. (2016). Paroxetine treatment of problematic pornography use: a case series. J. Behav. Addict. 1–4. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.046 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Graf, H., Abler, B., Hartmann, A., Metzger, C. D., and Walter, M. (2013). Modulation of attention network activation under antidepressant agents in healthy subjects. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 16, 1219–1230. doi: 10.1017/s1461145712001368

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Habermeyer, B., Esposito, F., Händel, N., Lemoine, P., Klarhöfer, M., Mager, R., et al. (2013). Immediate processing of erotic stimuli in paedophilia and controls: a case control study. Psikjatrija BMC 13:88. doi: 10.1186/1471-244x-13-88

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Hernández-González, M., Amezcua Gutiérrez, C., Martin, A. S., Sánchez, K. R., and Guevara, M. A. (2013). Sexual arousal decreases the functional synchronization between cortical areas in young men. J. Sess Maritali Ther. 39, 264–279. doi: 10.1080/0092623x.2012.665815

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Jacob, G. A., Arntz, A., Domes, G., Reiss, N., and Siep, N. (2011). Positive erotic picture stimuli for emotion research in heterosexual females. Psikjatrija Res. 190, 348–351. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.05.044

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Kim, G. W., and Jeong, G. W. (2013). A comparative study of brain activation patterns associated with sexual arousal between males and females using 3.0-T functional magnetic resonance imaging. Sess. Saħħa 11, 11–16. doi: 10.1071/SH13127

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Kim, G. W., and Jeong, G. W. (2014). Neural mechanisms underlying sexual arousal in connection with sexual hormone levels: a comparative study of the postoperative male-to-female transsexuals and premenopausal and menopausal women. Neuroreport 25, 693–700. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000000159

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Kim, T. H., Kim, G. W., Kim, S. K., and Jeong, G. W. (2016). Brain activation-based sexual orientation in female-to-male transsexuals. Int. J. Impot. Riż. 28, 31–38. doi: 10.1038/ijir.2015.29

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Klucken, T., Kruse, O., Wehrum-Osinsky, S., Hennig, J., Schweckendiek, J., and Stark, R. (2015). Impact of COMT Val158Met-polymorphism on appetitive conditioning and amygdala/prefrontal effective connectivity. Hum. Moħħ Mapp. 36, 1093 – 1101. doi: 10.1002 / hbm.22688

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Klucken, T., Wehrum, S., Schweckendiek, J., Merz, CJ, Hennig, J., Vaitl, D., et al. (2013). Il-polimorfiżmu 5-HTTLPR huwa assoċjat ma ’reazzjonijiet emodinamiċi mibdula waqt l-ikkondizzjonar ta’ l-aptit. Hum. Moħħ Mapp. 34, 2549 – 2560. doi: 10.1002 / hbm.22085

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Klucken, T., Wehrum-Osinsky, S., Schweckendiek, J., Kruse, O., and Stark, R. (2016). Altered appetitive conditioning and neural connectivity in subjects with compulsive sexual behavior. J. Sess. Med. 13, 627–636. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2016.01.013

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Knott, V., Impey, D., Fisher, D., Delpero, E., and Fedoroff, P. (2016). Pedophilic brain potential responses to adult erotic stimuli. Moħħ Riż. 1632, 127–140. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.12.004

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., and Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 21: RC159.

PubMed Astratt | Google Scholar

Kraus, S. W., Voon, V., and Potenza, M. N. (2016a). Neurobiology of compulsive sexual behavior: emerging science. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 385–386. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.300

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Kraus, S. W., Voon, V., and Potenza, M. N. (2016b). Should compulsive sexual behavior be considered an addiction? Dipendenza doi: 10.1111/add.13297 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Kühn, S., u Gallinat, J. (2014). Struttura tal-moħħ u konnettività funzjonali assoċjata mal-konsum tal-pornografija: il-moħħ fuq il-porn. JAMA Psikjatrija 71, 827 – 834. doi: 10.1001 / jamapsychiatry.2014.93

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Lee, S. W., Jeong, B. S., Choi, J., and Kim, J. W. (2015). Sex differences in interactions between nucleus accumbens and visual cortex by explicit visual erotic stimuli: an fMRI study. Int. J. Impot. Riż. 27, 161–166. doi: 10.1038/ijir.2015.8

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Sescousse, G., and Dreher, J. C. (2014). Endogenous cortisol levels are associated with an imbalanced striatal sensitivity to monetary versus non-monetary cues in pathological gamblers. Quddiem. Behav. Neurosci. 8:83. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00083

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Liu, X., Hairston, J., Schrier, M., and Fan, J. (2011). Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1219 – 1236. doi: 10.1016 / j.neubiorev.2010.12.012

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Love, T., Laier, C., Brand, M., Hatch, L., and Hajela, R. (2015). Neuroscience of internet pornography addiction: a review and update. Behav. Sci. (Basel) 5, 388–433. doi: 10.3390/bs5030388

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Mascaro, J. S., Hackett, P. D., and Rilling, J. K. (2014). Differential neural responses to child and sexual stimuli in human fathers and non-fathers and their hormonal correlates. Psikoneuroendokrinoloġija 46, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.04.014

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Minnix, J. A., Versace, F., Robinson, J. D., Lam, C. Y., Engelmann, J. M., Cui, Y., et al. (2013). The late positive potential (LPP) in response to varying types of emotional and cigarette stimuli in smokers: a content comparison. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 89, 18 – 25. doi: 10.1016 / j.ijpsycho.2013.04.019

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Oei, N. Y., Both, S., van Heemst, D., and van der Grond, J. (2014). Acute stress-induced cortisol elevations mediate reward system activity during subconscious processing of sexual stimuli. Psikoneuroendokrinoloġija 39, 111–120. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.10.005

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Politis, M., Loane, C., Wu, K., O’Sullivan, S. S., Woodhead, Z., Kiferle, L., et al. (2013). Neural response to visual sexual cues in dopamine treatment-linked hypersexuality in Parkinson’s disease. Moħħ 136, 400–411. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws326

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Prause, N., Staley, C., and Roberts, V. (2014). Frontal alpha asymmetry and sexually motivated states. Psikofiżjoloġija 51, 226–235. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12173

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Prause, N., Steele, V. R., Staley, C., Sabatinelli, D., and Hajcak, G. (2015). Modulation of late positive potentials by sexual images in problem users and controls inconsistent with “porn addiction”. Biol. Psychol. 109, 192–199. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.06.005

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Prause, N., Steele, V. R., Staley, C., Sabatinelli, D., and Hajcak, G. (2016). Prause et al. (2015) the latest falsification of addiction predictions. Biol. Psychol. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.05.007 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Prévost, C., Pessiglione, M., Météreau, E., Cléry-Melin, M. L., and Dreher, J. C. (2010). Separate valuation subsystems for delay and effort decision costs. J. Neurosci. 30, 14080–14090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2752-10.2010

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Redouté, J., Stoléru, S., Grégoire, M., Costes, N., Cinotti, L., Lavenne, F., et al. (2000). Brain processing of visual sexual stimuli in human males. Hum. Moħħ Mapp. 11, 162–177. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200011)11:3<162::AID-HBM30>3.0.CO;2-A

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Robinson, TE, u Berridge, KC (1993). Il-bażi newrali tal-effetti tax-xenqa tad-droga: teorija ta 'inċentiv-sensibilizzazzjoni tad-dipendenza. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247–291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-p

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Robinson, M. J. F., Fischer, A. M., Ahuja, A., Lesser, E. N., Maniates, H. (2015). “Roles of “wanting” and “liking” in motivating behavior: gambling, food and drug addictions”, in Behavioral Neuroscience of Motivation, Vol. 27: Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, eds E. H. Simpson and P. D. Balsam (Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 105–136. Available online at: http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/7854_2015_387

Google Scholar

Rupp, H. A., and Wallen, K. (2009). Sex-specific content preferences for visual sexual stimuli. Arch. Sess. Behav. 38, 417–426. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9402-5

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Sabatinelli, D., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., Costa, V. D., and Versace, F. (2007). Pleasure rather than salience activates human nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurofiżjol. 98, 1374–1379. doi: 10.1152/jn.00230.2007

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Schöne, B., Schomberg, J., Gruber, T., and Quirin, M. (2016). Event-related frontal alpha asymmetries: electrophysiological correlates of approach motivation. Exp. Moħħ Riż. 234, 559–567. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4483-6

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Seok, J. W., and Sohn, J. H. (2015). Neural substrates of sexual desire in individuals with problematic hypersexual behavior. Quddiem. Behav. Neurosci. 9:321. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00321

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Sescousse, G., Barbalat, G., Domenech, P., and Dreher, J. C. (2013a). Imbalance in the sensitivity to different types of rewards in pathological gambling. Moħħ 136, 2527–2538. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt126

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Sescousse, G., Caldú, X., Segura, B., and Dreher, J. C. (2013b). Processing of primary and secondary rewards: a quantitative meta-analysis and review of human functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 681 – 696. doi: 10.1016 / j.neubiorev.2013.02.002

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Sescousse, G., Li, Y., and Dreher, J. C. (2015). A common currency for the computation of motivational values in the human striatum. Soc. Cogn. Affettwa. Newrosci. 10, 467–473. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu074

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Sescousse, G., Redouté, J., and Dreher, J. C. (2010). The architecture of reward value coding in the human orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 13095–13104. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3501-10.2010

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Stark, R., Schienle, A., Girod, C., Walter, B., Kirsch, P., Blecker, C., et al. (2005). Erotic and disgust-inducing pictures–differences in the hemodynamic responses of the brain. Biol. Psychol. 70, 19–29. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.014

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Steele, V. R., Staley, C., Fong, T., and Prause, N. (2013). Sexual desire, not hypersexuality, is related to neurophysiological responses elicited by sexual images. Socioaffett. Neurosci. Psychol. 3:20770. doi: 10.3402/snp.v3i0.20770

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Stoléru, S., Fonteille, V., Cornélis, C., Joyal, C., u Moulier, V. (2012). Studji funzjonali newroimiminali ta 'tqanqil u orga sesswali f'irġiel u nisa b'saħħithom: reviżjoni u meta-analiżi. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 1481 – 1509. doi: 10.1016 / j.neubiorev.2012.03.006

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Stoléru, S., Grégoire, M., Gérard, D., Decety, J., Lafarge, E., Cinotti, L., et al. (1999). Neuroanatomical correlates of visually evoked sexual arousal in human males. Arch. Sess. Behav. 28, 1–21. doi: 10.1023/A:1018733420467

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Sutton, R., and Barto, A. (1998). Reinforcement learning: an introduction. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. 9, 1054–1054. doi: 10.1109/TNN.1998.712192

Test sħiħ CrossRef

Sylva, D., Safron, A., Rosenthal, A. M., Reber, P. J., Parrish, T. B., and Bailey, J. M. (2013). Neural correlates of sexual arousal in heterosexual and homosexual women and men. Horm. Imġiba. 64, 673–684. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.08.003

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Mole, TB, Banca, P., Porter, L., Morris, L., Mitchell, S., et al. (2014). Korrelati newrali ta 'reattività ta' cue sesswali f'individwi bi u mingħajr imgieba sesswali kompulsivi. PLoS Wieħed 9: e102419. doi: 10.1371 / journal.pone.0102419

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Walter, M., Bermpohl, F., Mouras, H., Schiltz, K., Tempelmann, C., Rotte, M., et al. (2008). Distinguishing specific sexual and general emotional effects in fMRI-subcortical and cortical arousal during erotic picture viewing. Neuroimage 40, 1482 – 1494. doi: 10.1016 / j.neuroimage.2008.01.040

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Wehrum, S., Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Walter, B., Hermann, A., Vaitl, D., et al. (2013). Gender commonalities and differences in the neural processing of visual sexual stimuli. J. Sess. Med. 10, 1328–1342. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12096

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Wehrum-Osinsky, S., Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Walter, B., Hermann, A., and Stark, R. (2014). At the second glance: stability of neural responses toward visual sexual stimuli. J. Sess. Med. 11, 2720–2737. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12653

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Wetherill, R. R., Childress, A. R., Jagannathan, K., Bender, J., Young, K. A., Suh, J. J., et al. (2014). Neural responses to subliminally presented cannabis and other emotionally evocative cues in cannabis-dependent individuals. Psikofarmakoloġija (Berl) 231, 1397–1407. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3342-z

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Wierzba, M., Riegel, M., Pucz, A., Leśniewska, Z., Dragan, W., Gola, M., et al. (2015). Erotic subset for the nencki affective picture system (NAPS ERO): cross-sexual comparison study. Quddiem. Psychol. 6: 1336. doi: 10.3389 / fpsyg.2015.01336

PubMed Astratt | Test sħiħ CrossRef | Google Scholar

Keywords: visual sexual stimuli, neuroimaging, compulsive sexual behaviors, behavioral addictions, incentive salience, reinforcement learning, sexual behavior

Citation: Gola M, Wordecha M, Marchewka A and Sescousse G (2016) Visual Sexual Stimuli—Cue or Reward? A Perspective for Interpreting Brain Imaging Findings on Human Sexual Behaviors. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:402. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00402

Rċevut: 27 April 2016; Aċċettat: 26 Lulju 2016;
Published: 15 August 2016.

Editjat minn:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, USA

Riveduti mill:

Rudolf Stark, Università ta’ Giessen, il-Ġermanja
Tim Klucken, Università ta’ Giessen, il-Ġermanja
Janniko Georgiadis, Ċentru Mediku ta 'l-Università ta' Groningen, l-Olanda
Shane W. Kraus, Department of Veterans Affairs and University of Massachusetts, USA

Copyright © 2016 Gola, Wordecha, Marchewka and Sescousse. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Liċenzja tal-Attribuzzjoni tal-Commons Creative (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

* Korrispondenza: Mateusz Gola, [protett bl-email]