Paʻu ma le Faʻateleina o le Paʻu Talavou (2011)

J Res Adolesc. 2011 Mar 1;21(1):21-33.

puna

Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology Weill Cornell Medical College, New York, NY USA.

lē faʻatino

Adolescence is a developmental period often characterized as a time of impulsive and risky choices leading to increased incidence of unintentional injuries and violence, alcohol and drug abuse, unintended pregnancy and sexually transmitted diseases. Traditional neurobiological and cognitive explanations for such suboptimal choices and actions have failed to account for nonlinear changes in behavior observed during adolescence, relative to childhood and adulthood. This review provides a biologically plausible conceptualization of the mechanisms underlying these nonlinear changes in behavior, as an imbalance between a heightened sensitivity to motivational cues and immature cognitive control. Recent human imaging and animal studies provide a biological basis for this view, suggesting differential development of subcortical limbic systems relative to top-down control systems during adolescence relative to childhood and adulthood. This work emphasizes the importance of examining transitions into and out of adolescence and highlights emerging avenues of future research on talavou faiʻai atinae.

uputatala: Adolescence, brain, development, fMRI, risk, incentive, cognitive control, connectivity

faʻatomuaga

Adolescence is characterized as a time when we act more impulsively, fail to consider long-term consequences, and engage in riskier behavior than we do as adults (Gardner & Steinberg, 2005; Scott, 1992; Steinberg, et al., 2008). This propensity to take risks is reflected in higher incidences of accidents, suicides, unsafe sexual practices, and criminal activity (Scott, 1992). Juveniles fifteen years of age and younger act more impulsively than do older adolescents, but even sixteen- and seventeen year old youth fail to exhibit adult levels of self-control (Feld, 2008).

In the past decade, a number of cognitive and neurobiological hypotheses have been postulated for why adolescents engage in impulsive and risky acts. Traditional accounts of adolescence suggest that it is a period of development associated with progressively greater efficiency of cognitive control capacities. This efficiency in cognitive control is described as dependent on maturation of the prefrontal cortex as evidenced by imaging (Galvan, et al., 2006; Gogtay, et al., 2004; Hare, et al., 2008; Sowell, et al., 2003) and post mortem studies (Bourgeois, Goldman-Rakic, & Rakic, 1994; Huttenlocher, 1979; Rakic, 1994) showing continued structural and functional development of this region well into young adulthood.

The general pattern of improved cognitive control with maturation of the prefrontal cortex (Crone & van der Molen, 2007) suggests a linear increase in development from childhood to adulthood. If cognitive control and an immature prefrontal cortex were the basis for suboptimal choice behavior alone, then children should look remarkably similar or presumably worse than adolescents, given their less developed prefrontal cortex and cognitive abilities (Casey, Getz, & Galvan, 2008). Yet suboptimal choices and actions observed during adolescence represent an inflection in development (Windle, et al., 2008) that is unique from either childhood or adulthood, as evidenced by the National Center for Health Statistics on adolescent behavior and mortality (Eaton, et al., 2008).

This review addresses the primary question of how the brain is changing during adolescence in ways that may explain inflections in risky behavior. We outline a testable neurobiological model that emphasizes the dynamic interplay between subcortical and cortical brain regions and speculate on the emergence of these systems from an evolutionary perspective. We provide evidence from behavioral and human brain imaging studies to support this model in the framework of actions in motivational contexts (Cauffman, et al., 2010; Figner, Mackinlay, Wilkening, & Weber, 2009; Galvan, Hare, Voss, Glover, & Casey, 2007; Galvan, et al., 2006) and address why some teenagers may be at greater risk than others for making suboptimal decisions leading to poorer long-term outcomes (Galvan, et al., 2007; Hare, et al., 2008).

Fa'ata'ita'iga Neurobiological o le Tupulaga Talavou

O se faʻataʻitaʻiga saʻo o suiga o le mafaufau ma le neurobiological i le taimi o le talavou e tatau ona togafitia le talavou e avea o se vaitau tau suiga (Spear, 2000), rather than a single snapshot in time. In other words, to understand this developmental period, characterizing transitions into and out of adolescence are necessary for distinguishing distinct attributes of this period of development (Casey, Galvan, & Hare, 2005; Casey, Tottenham, Liston, & Durston, 2005). Establishing developmental trajectories for cognitive processes is essential in characterizing these transitions and constraining interpretations about changes in behavior during this period.

We have developed a testable neurobiological model of adolescent development within this framework that builds on rodent models (Brenhouse, Sonntag, & Andersen, 2008; Laviola, Adriani, Terranova, & Gerra, 1999; Spear, 2000) ma suʻesuʻega faʻataʻitaʻiga lata mai o le talavou (Ernst, et al., 2005; Galvan, et al., 2007; Galvan, et al., 2006; Hare, et al., 2008; Somerville, Hare, & Casey, in press; Van Leijenhorst, Moor, et al., 2010; Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, et al., 2010). Ata 1 depicts this model. This characterization of adolescence goes beyond exclusive association of risky behavior to the immaturity of the prefrontal cortex. Rather, the proposed neurobiological model illustrates how subcortical and cortical top-down control regions must be considered together. The cartoon illustrates different developmental trajectories for these systems, with subcortical systems such as the ventral striatum developing earlier than prefrontal control regions. According to this model, the individual is biased more by functionally mature subcortical regions during adolescence (i.e., imbalance of subcortical relative to prefrontal cortical control), compared to children, for whom these systems (i.e., subcortical and prefrontal) are both still developing, and compared to adults, for whom these systems are fully mature.

Ata 1

Neurobiological model depicting later development of top down prefrontal regions relative to subcortical regions involved in desire and fear. This imbalance in development of these systems is proposed to be at the core of risky choice behavior in adolescents ...

This perspective provides a basis for nonlinear shifts in risky behavior across development, due to earlier maturation of subcortical systems relative to less mature top down prefrontal control systems. With development and experience, the functional connectivity between these regions provides a mechanism for top down control of this circuitry (Hare, et al., 2008). Further, the model reconciles the contradiction of health statistics of risky behavior during adolescence, with the astute observation by Reyna and Farley (2006) that adolescents are quite capable of rational decisions and understand risks of behaviors in which they engage. However, in emotionally salient situations, subcortical systems will win out (accelerator) over control systems (brakes) given their maturity relative to the prefrontal control system.

This model is consistent with models of adolescent development (Ernst, Pine, & Hardin, 2006; Ernst, Romeo, & Andersen, 2009; Geier & Luna, 2009; Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, 2005; Steinberg, 2008; Steinberg, et al., 2009) that suggest differential development of subcortical and cortical regions. For example, the triadic model proposed by Ernst and colleagues (Ernst, et al., 2006) describes motivated behavior as having three distinct neural circuits (approach, avoidance and regulatory). The approach system relates to reward behaviors and is largely controlled by the ventral striatum. The avoidance system relates to avoidance behaviors and is mostly controlled by the amygdala. Lastly, the regulatory system balances the approach and avoidance systems and is largely controlled by the prefrontal cortex. Accordingly, increased risk taking behavior during adolescence is due to greater influence of the approach system and a weaker influence of the regulatory system.

Our model differs from others in that it is based on empirical evidence for brain changes not only in the transition from adolescence to adulthood, but also the transition into adolescence from childhood. Further, we do not suggest that the striatum and amygdala are specific to approach and avoidant behavior given recent studies showing valence independence of these structures (Levita, et al., 2009), but rather that they are systems important in detecting motivationally and emotionally relevant cues in the environment that can bias behavior. This sensitivity to appetitive and emotive cues during adolescence has been described across species (see Spear, 2009) and is reviewed here.

Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives on Adolescence

A question that emerges from the imbalance model of adolescent brain development is why the brain might be programmed to develop in this way. This question may be addressed by taking a step backward and considering the definition of adolescence as the transitional period between childhood and adulthood. Puberty marks the onset of adolescence with the beginnings of sexual maturation (Graber & Brooks-Gunn, 1998) ma e mafai ona fa'amatalaina e fa'ailoga olaola. O le talavou e mafai ona faamatalaina o se suiga alualu i luma i le avea ma tagata matua ma se kosi ontogenetic le mautonu taimi (Spear, 2000, p.419). A complete discussion of the effect of pubertal hormones on brain and behavior is beyond the scope of this paper; see (Forbes & Dahl, 2010; Romao, 2003) for detailed reviews on the subject.

Evolutionarily speaking, adolescence is a period of gaining independence from the protection of the family, which simultaneously may put the individual in harms way (Kelley, Schochet, & Landry, 2004). Independence-seeking behaviors are observed across mammalian species, with increases in peer-directed social interactions and intensification in novelty seeking which impact adolescents’ propensity for risky behavior (Brown, 2004; Chassin, et al., 2004; Collins & Laursen, 2004; Laviola, et al., 1999). This risky behavior may be defined as the product of a biologically driven imbalance between increased novelty and sensation seeking in conjunction with immature “self-regulatory competence” (Steinberg, 2004). Speculation would suggest that this developmental pattern is an evolutionary feature in that an individual needs to engage in high-risk behavior to leave a safe and familiar niche in order to find a mate and procreate (Spear, 2000). Thus, risk taking appears to coincide with the time in which hormones drive adolescents to seek out sexual partners. In today’s society -when adolescence may extend indefinitely- with children living with parents and having financial dependence and choosing mates later in life, this behavior may be less adaptive. Our neurobiological model suggests this occurs through differential development of subcortical and cortical systems. Empirical behavioral and imaging data are reviewed in support of this view.

Adolescent Behavioral Development

A core component of behavioral development is the ability to suppress inappropriate actions in favor of goal-directed ones, especially in the presence of compelling incentives. This ability is typically referred to as cognitive control (Casey, Galvan, et al., 2005; Casey, Giedd, & Thomas, 2000; Casey, Thomas, et al., 2000). We review classic cognitive developmental literature in the context of changes in cortically driven cognitive processes with age and provide behavioral and neuroanatomical evidence for its distinction from risky behaviors.

A number of classic developmental studies have shown that cognitive control develops throughout childhood and adolescence (Tulaga, 1972; Flavell, Beach, & Chinksy, 1966; Keating & Bobbitt, 1978; Pascual-Leone, 1970). Several theorists have argued that this development is due to increases in processing speed and efficiency (e.g., (Bjorklund, 1985, 1987; Tulaga, 1972)), but others have suggested “inhibitory” processes are the key factor (Harnishfeger & Bjorklund, 1993). According to this account, suboptimal choices in childhood are due to greater susceptibility to interference from competing sources that must be suppressed (e.g., (Brainerd & Reyna, 1993; Casey, Thomas, Davidson, Kunz, & Franzen, 2002; Dempster, 1993; Diamond, 1985; Munakata & Yerys, 2001). Thus optimal decision-making requires the control of impulses (Mischel, Shoda, & Rodriguez, 1989) and this ability matures in a linear fashion across childhood and adolescence (Eigsti, et al., 2006).

In contrast, risk taking or reward seeking behaviors, seem to peak during adolescence and then decline in adulthood (Eaton, et al., 2008; Windle, et al., 2008) and are associated with puberty maturation (Dahl, 2004; Martin, et al., 2001). O se suesuega lata mai a Steinberg et al. (2008) delineated the construct of impulse/cognitive control from sensation seeking behaviors, defined as the desire to seek out novel experiences and taking risks in order to achieve them. They tested individuals between the ages of 10 and 30 and showed that differences in sensation seeking with age followed a curvilinear pattern, with peaks in sensation seeking increasing between 10 and 15 years and declining or remaining stable thereafter. In contrast, age differences in impulsivity followed a linear pattern, with decreasing impulsivity with age.

These findings suggest distinct developmental trajectories for the two constructs. Specifically, impulsivity diminishes with age across childhood and adolescence (Casey, Galvan, et al., 2005; Casey, Thomas, et al., 2002; Galvan, et al., 2007), although there are differences in the degree to which a given individual is impulsive or not, regardless of age (Eigsti, et al., 2006). In contrast to impulse/cognitive control, sensation seeking/risk taking appears to show a curvilinear pattern, with an increase during adolescence relative to childhood and adulthood (Cauffman, et al., 2010; Figner, et al., 2009; Galvan, et al., 2007). As will be reviewed in the following sections, these findings suggest a distinct neural system for the construct of risky behavior, separate from the neural system for impulse control, with earlier development of risk taking behavior relative to protracted development of impulse control (Galvan, et al., 2007; Steinberg, et al., 2008).

Adolescent Brain Development

Recent investigations of adolescent brain development have been based on advances in neuroimaging methodologies that can be easily used with developing human populations. These methods rely on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) methods and include: structural MRI, which is used to measure the size and shape of structures; functional MRI (fMRI) which is used to measure patterns of brain activity; and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) which is used to index connectivity of white matter fiber tracts. Evidence for our developmental model of competition between cortical and subcortical regions is supported by immature structural and functional connectivity as measured by DTI and fMRI, respectively.

MRI Studies of Human Brain Development

Several studies have used structural MRI to map the anatomical course of normal brain development (see review (Casey, Tottenham, et al., 2005)). Although total brain size is approximately 90% of its adult size by age six, the gray and white matter subcomponents of the brain continue to undergo dynamic changes throughout adolescence. Data from recent longitudinal MRI studies indicate that gray matter volume has an inverted U-shape pattern, with greater regional variation than white matter (Giedd, 2004; Gogtay, et al., 2004; Sowell, et al., 2003; Sowell, Thompson, & Toga, 2004). I se tulaga lautele, o itulagi o lo'o i lalo o galuega autu, e pei o le afi ma le fa'alogo, e matua matutua; nofoaga fa'alāpotopotoga maualuluga, e fa'atasia nei galuega fa'avae, fa'amatua mulimuli ane (Gogtay, et al., 2004; Sowell, et al., 2004). Mo se faʻataʻitaʻiga, o suʻesuʻega e faʻaogaina ai le MRI o loʻo faʻaalia ai o le gau o mea efuefu cortical e tupu muamua i vaega muamua o le sensorimotor ma sili ona lata mai i le pito i luma o le pito i luma ma le pito i tua.Gogtay, et al., 2004). O lenei faʻataʻitaʻiga e ogatasi ma suʻesuʻega e le o se tagata muamua ma suʻesuʻega postmortem tagata e faʻaalia ai o le cortex muamua o se tasi o vaega mulimuli o faiʻai e matua (Bourgeois, et al., 1994; Huttenlocher, 1979) while subcortical and sensorimotor regions develop sooner. In contrast to gray matter, white matter volume increases in a roughly linear pattern, increasing throughout development well into adulthood (Gogtay, et al., 2004). These changes presumably reflect ongoing myelination of axons by oligodendrocytes enhancing neuronal conduction and communication of relevant connections.

Although less attention has been given to subcortical regions when examining structural changes, some of the largest changes in the brain across development are seen in portions of the basal ganglia such as the striatum (Sowell, Thompson, Holmes, Jernigan, & Toga, 1999), especially in males (Giedd, et al., 1996). These developmental changes in structural volume within basal ganglia and prefrontal regions suggest that cortical connections are becoming more refined consistent with neural developmental processes (e.g. dendritic arborization, cell death, synaptic pruning, myelination) that are occurring during childhood and adolescence (Huttenlocher, 1979). These processes allow for fine-tuning and strengthening of connections between prefrontal and subcortical regions with learning that may coincide with greater cognitive control (e.g., signaling of prefrontal control regions to adjust behavior) (Casey, Amso, & Davidson, 2006; Casey & Durston, 2006).

It is unclear exactly how structural changes relate to behavior changes. A few studies have shown indirect associations between MRI-based volumetric change and cognitive function using neuropsychological measures (e.g., (Casey, Castellanos, et al., 1997; Sowell, et al., 2003)). Specifically, associations have been reported between MRI-based prefrontal cortical and basal ganglia regional volumes and measures of cognitive control (i.e., ability to override an inappropriate choice/action in favor of another (Casey, Castellanos, et al., 1997) (Casey, Trainor, et al., 1997)). These findings suggest that cognitive changes are reflected in structural changes in the brain and underscore the importance of subcortical (striatum) as well as cortical (e.g., prefrontal cortex) development.

DTI Studies of Human Brain Development

The MRI-based morphometry studies reviewed suggest that cortical connections are being fine-tuned with the elimination of an overabundance of synapses and strengthening of relevant connections with development and experience. Recent advances in MRI technology like DTI provide a tool for examining the developmental modulation of specific white matter tracts and their relation to behavior. In one study, development of cognitive control was positively correlated with prefrontal-parietal fiber tracts (Nagy, Westerberg, & Klingberg, 2004) consistent with functional neuroimaging studies showing differential recruitment of these regions in children relative to (Klingberg, Forssberg, & Westerberg, 2002).

Faʻaaogaina se auala tutusa, Liston ma paaga (2006) examined the strength of white matter tracts in frontostriatal circuits, which continue to develop across childhood into adulthood. The frontostriatal fiber tracts were defined by connecting two regions of interest in the striatum and ventral prefrontal cortex identified in an fMRI study using the same task (Durston, Thomas, Worden, Yang, & Casey, 2002; Epstein, et al., 2007). Across these developmental DTI studies, fiber tract measures across the entire brain were correlated with development. However, there was specificity in which particular fiber tracts were associated with cognitive control (Casey, et al., 2007; Liston, et al., 2006) po'o le tomai fa'apitoa (Nagy, et al., 2004). Specifically, frontostriatal connection strength positively predicted impulse control capacity, as measured by performance on a go/nogo task (Casey, et al., 2007; Liston, et al., 2006). These findings underscore the importance of examining not only regional structural changes, but also circuitry related changes when making claims about age-dependent maturation of neural substrates of cognitive development.

Functional MRI Studies of Behavioral and Brain Development

Although structural changes as measured by MRI and DTI have been associated with behavioral changes during development, a more direct approach for examining structure-function associations is to measure changes in the brain and behavior simultaneously, as with fMRI. The ability to measure functional changes in the developing brain with MRI has significant potential for the field of developmental science. In the context of the current article, fMRI provides a means for constraining interpretations of adolescent decision-making. As stated previously, the development of the prefrontal cortex is believed to play an important role in the maturation of higher cognitive abilities such as decision-making and goal oriented choice behavior (Casey, Tottenham, & Fossella, 2002; Casey, Trainor, et al., 1997). Many paradigms have been used, together with fMRI, to assess the neurobiological basis of these abilities. These paradigms include go/nogo, (participants must respond to one stimulus but suppress responses to a second stimulus) flanker (participants choose the directionality of a target surrounded by symbols that are either compatible or incompatible with the target), stop signal (participants respond as fast as possible to a stimulus but must suppress this response when they receive a stop signal such as an auditory tone), and antisaccade tasks (participants must inhibit reflexive eye movements to gaze in the opposite direction of a target) (Bunge, Dudukovic, Thomason, Vaidya, & Gabrieli, 2002; Casey, Giedd, et al., 2000; Casey, Trainor, et al., 1997; Durston, et al., 2003; Luna, et al., 2001). Collectively, these studies show that children recruit distinct but often larger, more diffuse prefrontal regions when performing these tasks than do adults. The pattern of activity within brain regions central to task performance (i.e., that correlate with cognitive performance) become more focal or fine-tuned with age; while regions not correlated with task performance diminish in activity with age. This pattern has been observed across both cross-sectional (Brown, et al., 2005) ma su'esu'ega umi (Durston, et al., 2006) and across a variety of paradigms.

Although neuroimaging studies cannot definitively characterize the mechanism of such developmental changes (e.g. dendritic arborization, synaptic pruning) the findings reflect development within, and refinement of, projections to and from activated brain regions with maturation. Further, the findings suggest that these neuroanatomical changes occur over a protracted period of time (Brown, et al., 2005; Bunge, et al., 2002; Casey, Thomas, et al., 2002; Casey, Trainor, et al., 1997; Crone, Donohue, Honomichl, Wendelken, & Bunge, 2006; Luna, et al., 2001; Moses, et al., 2002; Schlaggar, et al., 2002; Tamm, Menon, & Reiss, 2002; Thomas, et al., 2004; Turkeltaub, Gareau, Fugalaau, Zeffiro, & Etena, 2003).

How can this methodology inform us about whether adolescent decisions are indeed impulsive or are risky? Impulse control as measured by tasks such as the go/nogo task show a linear pattern of development across childhood and adolescence as described above. However, recent neuroimaging studies have begun to examine reward-related processing relevant to risk-taking in adolescents (Bjork, et al., 2004; Ernst, et al., 2005; Galvan, et al., 2005; May, et al., 2004; Van Leijenhorst, Moor, et al., 2010). These studies have focused primarily on the region of the ventral striatum, a region implicated in learning and predicting reward outcomes.

Sensitivity to Appetitive cues in Adolescence

Our neurobiological model suggests that the combination of heightened responsiveness to motivational cues and immaturity in behavioral control may bias adolescents to seek immediate, rather than long-term gains. Tracking subcortical (e.g., ventral striatum) and cortical (e.g., prefrontal) development across childhood through adulthood provides constraints on whether changes reported in adolescence are specific to this period of development, or reflect maturation that is steadily occurring in a somewhat linear pattern from childhood to adulthood.

Several groups have shown that adolescents show heightened activation of the ventral striatum in anticipation and/or receipt of rewards compared to adults (Ernst, et al., 2005; Galvan, et al., 2006; Geier, Terwilliger, Teslovich, Velanova, & Luna, 2009; Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, et al., 2010), coupled with less activation in the prefrontal cortex relative to adults. In one of the first studies to examine this response across the full range of childhood to adulthood Galvan and her colleagues examined behavioral and neural responses to reward manipulations in 6 to 29 year olds. They focused on brain circuitry implicated in reward related learning and behavior in animal studies (Hikosaka & Watanabe, 2000; Pecina, Cagniard, Berridge, Aldridge, & Zhuang, 2003; Schultz, 2006), adult human imaging studies (e.g. (Knutson, Adams, Fong, & Hommer, 2001; O’Doherty, Kringelbach, Rolls, Hornak, & Andrews, 2001; Zald, et al., 2004)) and in studies of addiction (Hyman & Malenka, 2001; Volkow & Li, 2004). Fa'avae i fa'ata'ita'iga rodent (Laviola, et al., 1999; Spear, 2000) ma faʻataʻitaʻiga faʻataʻitaʻiga muamua (Ernst, et al., 2005), they hypothesized that relative to children and adults, adolescents would show exaggerated activation of the ventral striatum in concert with less mature recruitment of top down prefrontal control regions. Their results supported this hypothesis showing that the spatial extent of brain activity in adolescents in the ventral striatum to reward, was similar to that observed in adults, whereas the extent of activity in prefrontal regions was more similar to children. The extent of activity between these two regions was associated with elevated magnitude of activity in the ventral striatum in adolescents relative to children and adults assumed to result from the imbalance in corticosubcortial development (see Ata 2). Recent work showing delayed functional connectivity between prefrontal and subcortical regions in adolescence relative to adults provides a mechanism for the lack of top down control of regions related to processing motivational cues (Hare, et al., 2008).

Ata 2

Activity in the ventral striatum to anticipated reward as a function of age, for each individual subject, showing enhanced activity between roughly 13 to 18 years (Adapted from Galvan et al., 2006; 2007).

These findings are consistent in part with rodent models (Laviola, Macri, Morley-Fletcher, & Adriani, 2003) ma suʻesuʻega ata muamua (Ernst, et al., 2005; Van Leijenhorst, Moor, et al., 2010) showing enhanced ventral striatal activity to rewards and anticipation of rewards during adolescence. Relative to children and adults, adolescents showed an exaggerated ventral striatal response to reward. However, both children and adolescents showed a less mature response in prefrontal control regions than adults. These findings suggest different developmental trajectories for these regions may underlie the enhancement in ventral striatal activity, relative to children or adults, which may in turn relate to the increased risky decisions observed during this period of development (Figner, et al., 2009). It is relevant to note that while several laboratories (Ernst, et al., 2005; Galvan, et al., 2006; Geier, et al., 2009; Somerville, et al., in press; Van Leijenhorst, Moor, et al., 2010) have shown this heightened response in the ventral striatum in adolescents, one laboratory has failed to observe this response (Bjork, et al., 2004; Bjork, Smith, Chen, & Hommer, 2010) Future studies will be needed to clarify the specific conditions under which this pattern of brain activity is or is not observed.

Differential recruitment of prefrontal and subcortical regions have been reported across a number of developmental fMRI studies (Casey, Thomas, et al., 2002; Geier, et al., 2009; Luna, et al., 2001; Monk, et al., 2003; Thomas, et al., 2004; Van Leijenhorst, Zanolie, et al., 2010). E masani lava o nei suʻesuʻega ua faʻamatalaina i tulaga o vaega e le o matua i luma nai lo le le paleni i le va o le atinaʻe faʻaitulagi muamua ma le subcortical. Tuuina atu faʻamaoniga o itulagi muamua i le taʻitaʻia o gaioiga talafeagai i tulaga eseese (Miller & Cohen, 2001), immature prefrontal activity might hinder appropriate estimation of future outcomes and appraisal of risky choices, and might thus be less influential on reward valuation than the ventral striatum. This pattern is consistent with previous research showing elevated subcortical, relative to cortical activity when decisions are biased by immediate over long-term gains (McClure, Laibson, Loewenstein, & Cohen, 2004). During adolescence, relative to childhood or adulthood, immature prefrontal cortex engagement may not provide sufficient top-down control of robustly activated reward processing regions (e.g., ventral striatum), resulting in less influence of prefrontal systems relative to the ventral striatum in reward valuation.

While differential recruitment of cortical and subcortical regions has been robustly reported across development, only a few studies have addressed how cognitive control and reward systems interact. A recent study by (Geier, et al., 2009) examined this interaction using a version of an antisaccade task during fMRI in adolescents and adults. Their findings showed that on trials for which money was at stake, performance was enhanced, with the greatest enhancement (faster and more accurate responses) observed in adolescents. This performance was paralleled by exaggerated activation in the ventral striatum in adolescents following a cue that the next trial would be rewarded while they were preparing for and subsequently executing the antisaccade. Adolescents also showed elevated prefrontal activity in regions important for controlling eye movements. These findings suggest a reward-related upregulation in these control regions.

The Geier study provides an example of how appetitive cues can facilitate cognitive performance in adolescents, but high risk behavior in adolescence in every day life suggest that appetitive cues may impair cognitive decisions. To test this hypothesis, Somerville and colleagues (Somerville, et al., in press) tested children, adolescents and adults while performing a go/nogo task on which they had to suppress a response to an appetitive social cue. She showed that adolescents had greater difficulty resisting appetitive social cues compared to children and adults, as evidenced by more false alarms to these cue than neutral ones. This behavioral performance was paralleled by enhanced activity in the ventral striatum. In contrast, activation in the prefrontal cortex was associated with overall accuracy and showed a linear decrease in activity with improvement in performance and age. A functional connectivity analyses identified the dorsal striatum as a key convergence point for cortical and subcortical signals. Collectively, these studies suggest that differences in adolescent behavior from adults depend on the context of the behavior. In appetitively charged situations, subcortical systems involved in detection of appetitive cues will win out (accelerator) over cortical control systems (brakes), given differential regional development. However, in situations in which appetitive or emotive cues are not present, cortical control systems are not compromised, leading to more optimal performance in adolescents.

Adolescence and Individual Differences

Individuals vary in their ability to control impulses and in risk taking, a phenomenon that has been recognized in psychology for some time (Benthin, Slovic, & Severson, 1993). Therefore, some adolescents will be more likely to engage in risky behaviors, and be more prone to poorer outcomes. Thus, examining individual variability may help to identify potential bio-behavioral markers to identify individuals who might be at greater risk for poor outcomes during adolescence.

A classic example of individual differences reported in these abilities in the social, cognitive and developmental psychology literature is that of delay of gratification (Mischel, et al., 1989). Delay of gratification is typically assessed in 3 to 4 year old children. The child is asked whether they would prefer a small reward (one marshmallow) now or a large reward (two marshmallows) later. The child is then told that the experimenter will leave the room in order to prepare for upcoming activities and explains to the child that if she remains in her seat and does not eat a marshmallow during that time, then she will receive the large reward of both marshmallows. If the child does not or cannot wait, she should ring a bell to summons the experimenter and thereby receive the smaller reward. Once it is clear the child understands the task, she is seated at the table with the two rewards and the bell. Distractions in the room are minimized, with no toys, books or pictures. The experimenter returns after 15 minutes or after the child has rung the bell, eaten the rewards, or shown any signs of distress. Using this paradigm, Mischel showed that children typically behave in one of two ways on this task: 1) they ring the bell almost immediately in order to have the marshmallow, which means they only get one; 2) they wait and optimize their gains, and receive both marshmallows. This observation suggests that some individuals are better than others in their ability to control impulses in the face of highly salient incentives and this bias can be detected in early childhood (Mischel, et al., 1989) and appear to remain throughout adolescence and young adulthood (Eigsti, et al., 2006).

What might explain individual differences in optimal choice behavior? Some theorists have postulated that dopaminergic mesolimbic circuitry, implicated in reward processing, underlies risky behavior (Blum, et al., 2000). Developmental studies provide neurochemical evidence indicates that the balance in the adolescent brain between cortical and subcortical dopamine systems begins to shift toward greater cortical dopamine levels during adolescence (Brenhouse, et al., 2008; Spear, 2000). Similarly, there is a delayed time course of dopaminergic enervation of the nonhuman primate prefrontal cortex through adolescence into adulthood suggesting that functional maturity is not reached until adulthood (Rosenberg & Lewis, 1995). Individual differences in this circuitry, such as allelic variants in dopamine-related genes, resulting in too little or too much dopamine in subcortical regions, might relate to the propensity of some to engage in risky behavior more than others (O'Doherty, 2004).

The ventral striatum has been shown to increase in activity immediately prior to making risky choices on monetary-risk paradigms (Kuhnen & Knutson, 2005; Matthews, Simmons, Lane, & Paulus, 2004; Montague & Berns, 2002) and as described previously, adolescents show exaggerated striatal activity to rewarding outcomes relative to children or adults (Ernst, et al., 2005; Galvan, et al., 2006). I le tuufaatasiga, o nei faʻamatalaga o loʻo fautua mai ai e mafai e tupulaga talavou ona sili atu ona faʻalavelave i filifiliga lamatia o se vaega (Figner, et al., 2009; Gardner & Steinberg, 2005), but some adolescents will be more prone than others to engage in risky behaviors, putting them at potentially greater risk for negative outcomes.

To explore individual differences in risk taking behavior, Galvan ma ana paʻaga (2007) examined the association between activity in reward-related neural circuitry in response to a large monetary reward with personality trait measures of risk taking and impulsivity in adolescence. Functional magnetic resonance imaging and anonymous self-report rating scales of risky behavior, risk perception and impulsivity were acquired in individuals between the ages of 7 and 29 years. There was a positive association between ventral striatal activity and the likelihood of engaging in risky behavior across development. This activity varied as a function of individuals’ ratings of anticipated positive or negative consequences of such behavior. The individuals who perceived risky behaviors as leading to dire consequences, activated the ventral striatum less to reward. This negative association was driven by the child participants, whereas a positive association was seen in the adults who rated the consequences of such behavior as positive.

In addition to linking risk taking to reward circuitry, Galvan showed no association between activity of this circuitry and ratings of impulsivity (Galvan, et al., 2007). Instead she showed that impulsivity was negatively correlated with age. This finding is consistent with a recent report Steinberg (2008) showing differential development of sensation seeking and impulsivity, with sensation seeking increasing during adolescence relative to childhood and adulthood, but impulsivity followed a linear pattern of decreasing with age. These findings suggest that during adolescence, some individuals may be more prone to engage in risky behaviors due to developmental changes in concert with variability in a given individual’s predisposition to engage in risky behavior, rather than to simple changes in impulsivity. Further, these individual and developmental differences may help explain vulnerability in some individuals to risk-taking associated with substance use, and ultimately, addiction.

iʻuga

Human imaging studies show structural and functional changes in corticosubcortical circuitry (for review, (Casey, Tottenham, et al., 2005; Giedd, et al., 1999; Giedd, et al., 1996; Jernigan, et al., 1991; Sowell, et al., 1999)) that parallel increases in cognitive control and self-regulation (Casey, Trainor, et al., 1997; Luna & Sweeney, 2004; Luna, et al., 2001; Rubia, et al., 2000; Steinberg, 2004; Steinberg, et al., 2008). These changes show a shift in activation of prefrontal regions from diffuse to more focal recruitment over time (Brown, et al., 2005; Bunge, et al., 2002; Casey, Trainor, et al., 1997; Durston & Casey, 2006; Moses, et al., 2002) ma le maualuga o le faʻafaigaluegaina o vaega pito i lalo i le taimi o le talavou (Casey, Thomas, et al., 2002; Durston & Casey, 2006; Luna, et al., 2001). Although neuroimaging studies cannot definitively characterize the mechanism of such developmental changes, these changes in volume and structure may reflect development within, and refinement of, projections to and from these brain regions during maturation suggestive of fine-tuning of the system with development (Hare, et al., 2008; Liston, et al., 2006).

Taken together, the findings synthesized here indicate that increased risk taking behavior in adolescence is associated with different developmental trajectories of subcortical motivational and cortical control regions. However, this is not to say that adolescents are incapable of making rational decisions. Rather, in emotionally charged situations, the more mature limbic system may win over the prefrontal control system in guiding actions.

Although adolescence has been distinguished as a period characterized by reward seeking and risk taking behaviors (Gardner & Steinberg, 2005; Spear, 2000) individual differences in neural responses to reward, predispose some adolescents to take more risks than others, putting them at greater risk for poor outcomes such as addiction, substance abuse and mortality. These findings provide crucial groundwork by synthesizing the various findings related to impulsivity and risk taking in adolescence and in understanding individual differences and developmental markers for propensities for suboptimal choices leading to negative consequences.

tautinoga

This work was supported in part by NIDA R01 DA018879, NIMH P50 MH62196, NSF 06-509, and NSF 0720932 to BJC, the Mortimer D. Sackler family, the Dewitt-Wallace fund, and by the Weill Cornell Medical College Citigroup Biomedical Imaging Center and Imaging Core.

mau faasino

  • Benthin A, Slovic P, Severson H. O se suʻesuʻega faʻapitoa o le mafaufau o le talavou. Tala o le Autalavou. 1993;16: 153-168. [PubMed]
  • Bjork JM, Knutson B, Fong GW, Caggiano DM, Bennett SM, Hommer DW. Faʻaauau-na mafua ai le faʻamalosia o faiʻai ile tupulaga talavou: tutusa ma eseesega mai talavou matutua. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24(8): 1793-1802. [PubMed]
  • Bjork JM, Smith AR, Chen G, Hommer DW. Adolescents, adults and rewards: comparing motivational neurocircuitry recruitment using fMRI. PLoS se tasi. 2010;5(7): e11440. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Bjorklund DF. The role of conceptual knowledge in the development of organization in children’s memory. In: Brainerd CJ, Pressley M, editors. Fa'agasologa fa'avae ile atina'eina o manatuaga: Alualu i luma ile su'esu'ega o le atina'eina ole mafaufau. Springer-Verlag; New York: 1985. pp. 103–142.
  • Bjorklund DF. How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children’s memory: An interpretive review. Atinaeina o Atinae. 1987;7: 993-130.
  • Blum K, Braverman ER, Holder JM, Lubar JF, Monastra VJ, Miller D, et al. Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors. J Fualaau faasaina. 2000;32(Suppl, i-iv):1–112.
  • Bourgeois JP, Goldman-Rakic ​​PS, Rakic ​​P. Synaptogenesis i le pito i luma o le kheshkeys. Cerebral Cortex. 1994;4: 78-96. [PubMed]
  • Brainerd CJ, Reyna VF. O le tuto'atasi o le mafaufau ma le fa'alavelave o le mafaufau i le atina'eina o le mafaufau. Toe Iloiloina o le Mafaufau. 1993;100: 42-67. [PubMed]
  • Brenhouse HC, Sonntag KC, Andersen SL. Faʻaaliga o le nusipepa D1 dopamine o loʻo faʻaogaina i luga o laina uʻamea o le gasegase muamua: sootaga i le faʻaleleia atili o le faʻaosofia o le fualaau o fualaau oona i le talavou. J Neurosci. 2008;28(10): 2375-2382. [PubMed]
  • Brown BB. Adolescents’ Relationships with Peers. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg LD, editors. Tusitaulima o mafaufauga o talavou. John Wiley & Sons, Inc; Hoboken: 2004. pp. 363–394.
  • Brown TT, Lugar HM, Coalson RS, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. Suiga fa'atupuina ile fa'alapotopotoga ole ga'o ole tagata mo le fa'atupuina o upu. Cerebral Cortex. 2005;15: 275-290. [PubMed]
  • Bunge SA, Dudukovic NM, Thomason ME, Vaidya CJ, Gabrieli JD. O le sao o le lobe i luma o le matua i le puleaina o le mafaufau i tamaiti: faʻamaoniga mai le fMRI. Neuron. 2002;33(2): 301-311. [PubMed]
  • Case R. Validation of a neo-Piagetian mental capacity construct. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1972;14: 287-302.
  • Casey BJ, Amso D, Davidson MC. Learning about learning and development with neuroimaging. In: Johnsons M, Munakata Y, editors. Attention and Performance XXI: Processes of Change in Brain and Cognitive Development. MIT; Cambridge, MA: 2006.
  • Casey BJ, Castellanos FX, Giedd JN, Marsh WL, Hamburger SD, Schubert AB, et al. Fa'aa'afiaga ole ta'amilosaga sa'o i luma ile fa'alavelave tali ma le fa'aletonu o le mafaufau/fa'aletonu. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(3): 374-383. [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Durston S. From behavior to cognition to the brain and back: what have we learned from functional imaging studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(6): 957-960. [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Epstein JN, Buhle J, Liston C, Davidson MC, Tonev ST, et al. Faʻafesoʻotaʻi Frontostriatal ma lana matafaioi i le puleaina o le mafaufau i matua-tamaiti dyads ma le ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(11): 1729-1736. [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Galvan A, Hare TA. Suiga i le faʻatulagaina o le cerebral i le taimi o le atinaʻeina o le mafaufau. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15(2): 239-244. [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Getz S, Galvan A. O le faigata matua. Dev Faʻaaliga 2008;28(1): 62-77. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Thomas KM. Atinaʻega o le faiai o le gaosiga ma le gaioiga ma lona fesoʻotaʻiga i le atinaʻeina o le mafaufau. Biol Psychol. 2000;54(1-3): 241-257. [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Kunz K, Franzen PL. Fa'amavaega striatal ma hippocampal galuega fa'atupuina fa'atasi ma se galuega feso'ota'iga fa'aosofia-tali. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(19): 8647-8652. [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Thomas KM, Welsh TF, Badgaiyan RD, Eccard CH, Jennings JR, et al. Fa'amavaega o fete'ena'iga tali, filifiliga fa'alogo, ma fa'amoemoe fa'atasi ma fa'ata'ita'iga fa'amaneta. Taualumaga a le National Academy of Sciences o le Iunaite Setete o Amerika. 2000;97(15): 8728-8733. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Fossella J. Falemaʻi, faʻataʻitaʻiga, manuʻa, ma faʻasologa faʻasolosolo agai i se faʻataʻitaʻiga o le pulea o le mafaufau. Dev Psychobiol. 2002;40(3): 237-254. [PubMed]
  • Casey BJ, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Faʻataʻitaʻiina o faiʻai atiae: o le a se mea ua tatou aʻoaʻoina e uiga i le atinaʻeina o mafaufauga? Trends in Scientific Science. 2005;9(3): 104-110.
  • Casey BJ, Faiaoga RJ, Orendi JL, Schubert AB, Nystrom LE, Giedd JN, et al. O se suʻesuʻega MRI faʻatinoina o le faʻagaioiga muamua i le taimi o le faʻatinoina o se galuega e le alu. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 1997;9: 835-847.
  • Cauffman E, Shulman EP, Steinberg L, Claus E, Banich MT, Graham S, et al. Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. Dev Psychol. 2010;46(1): 193-207. [PubMed]
  • Chassin L, Hussong A, Barrera M, Jr., Molina BSG, Trim R, Ritter J. Adolescent Substance Use. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg LD, editors. Tusitaulima o mafaufauga o talavou. John Wiley & Sons, Inc; Hoboken: 2004. pp. 665–696.
  • Collins AW, Laursen B. Parent-Adolescent Relationships and Influences. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg LD, editors. Tusitaulima o mafaufauga o talavou. John Wiley & Sons, Inc; Hoboken: 2004. pp. 331–362.
  • Crone EA, Donohue SE, Honomichl R, Wendelken C, Bunge SA. Brain regions mediating flexible rule use during development. J Neurosci. 2006;26(43): 11239-11247. [PubMed]
  • Crone EA, van der Molen MW. Development of decision making in school-aged children and adolescents: evidence from heart rate and skin conductance analysis. Tamaiti Dev. 2007;78(4): 1288-1301. [PubMed]
  • Dahl R. Atinaʻe faiʻai talavou: O se vaitaimi o faʻafitauli ma avanoa. Annals o le New Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021: 1-22. [PubMed]
  • Dempster FN. Tete'e i le fa'alavelave: Suiga fa'atupuina i se faiga fa'avae fa'agaioia. I totonu: Howe ML, Pasnak R, faatonu. Autu fa'atupu i le atina'eina o le mafaufau. Vol. 1. Springer; New York: 1993. pp. 3–27.
  • Diamond A. Atina'e o le tomai e fa'aoga ai le manatua e ta'ita'ia ai gaioiga, e pei ona fa'ailoa mai e pepe meamea ile AB. Atinaʻeina o Tama. 1985;56: 868-883. [PubMed]
  • Durston S, Casey BJ. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development: the authors’ reply. Dev Sci. 2006;9(1): 18-20. [PubMed]
  • Durston S, Davidson MC, Thomas KM, Worden MS, Tottenham N, Martinez A, et al. Faʻataʻitaʻiga faʻataʻitaʻiga o feteʻenaʻiga ma faʻatauvaʻa tali faʻaaogaina vave faʻafefiloi-faʻataʻitaʻiga faʻataʻitaʻiga fMRI. Neuroimage. 2003;20(4): 2135-2141. [PubMed]
  • Durston S, Davidson MC, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Spicer J, Fossella JA, et al. O se fesuiaiga mai le faasalalauina atu i gaoioiga faʻasolosolo faʻasalalau ma le atinaʻeina. Dev Sci. 2006;9(1): 1-8. [PubMed]
  • Durston S, Thomas KM, Worden MS, Yang Y, Casey BJ. Le aʻafiaga o faʻamatalaga muamua i luga o le faʻasaina: o se suʻesuʻega fMRI e faʻatatau i mea na tupu. Neuroimage. 2002;16(2): 449-453. [PubMed]
  • Eaton LK, Kann L, Kinchen S, Shanklin S, Ross J, Hawkins J, et al. Talosaga a le Autalavou i Amioga Faʻataʻitaʻi - Iunaite Setete, 2007, vaʻaiga puʻupuʻu. Lipoti Faʻavaomalo ma le Soifuaga Faʻavaomalo. 2008;57(SS04): 1-131. [PubMed]
  • Eigsti IM, Zayas V, Mischel W, Shoda Y, Ayduk O, Tamalani MB, et al. Faʻailoaina le pulea o le mafaufau mai le aoga faataitai i le tuai o le talavou ma le talavou matutua. Psycho Sci. 2006;17(6): 478-484. [PubMed]
  • Epstein JN, Casey BJ, Tonev ST, Davidson MC, Reiss AL, Garrett A, et al. ADHD- and medication-related brain activation effects in concordantly affected parent-child dyads with ADHD. J Child Child Psychiatry. 2007;48(9): 899-913. [PubMed]
  • Ernst M, Nelson EE, Jazbec S, McClure EB, Monk CS, Leibenluft E, et al. Amygdala ma le nucleus faʻapupula i tali i le mauaina ma le faʻaaogaina o tupe maua i tagata matutua ma tagata talavou. Neuroimage. 2005;25(4): 1279-1291. [PubMed]
  • Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic faʻataʻitaʻiga o le neurobiology o amioga faʻamalosia i le talavou. Mafaufauga Psychol. 2006;36(3): 299-312. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Ernst M, Romeo RD, Andersen SL. Neurobiology o le atinaʻeina o amioga faʻaosofia i le talavou: o se faʻamalama i totonu o se faʻataʻitaʻiga o neural system. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;93(3): 199-211. [PubMed]
  • Feld BC. A slower form of death: Implications of Roper v. Simmons for juveniles sentenced to life without parol. Notre Dame Journal of Law, Ethics, & Public Policy. 2008;22: 9-65.
  • Figner B, Mackinlay RJ, Wilkening F, Weber EU. Faiga faʻapitoa ma le faia o filifiliga i se filifiliga lamatia: eseesega o tausaga e ono lamatia ai le ave i le Columbia Card Task. J Exp Psychol Aoao Mem Cogn. 2009;35(3): 709-730. [PubMed]
  • Flavell JH, Beach DR, Chinksy JM. Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Atinaʻeina o Tama. 1966;37: 283-299. [PubMed]
  • Forbes EE, Dahl RE. Atinaʻe ma amioga faʻapitoa: faʻamalosia o le hormonal o agafesootai ma faʻamalosi. Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1): 66-72. [PubMed]
  • Galvan A, Hare T, Voss H, Glover G, Casey BJ. Faʻalavelave faʻafuaseʻi ma le faiʻai faigata: o ai o loʻo lamatia? Dev Sci. 2007;10(2): F8-F14. [PubMed]
  • Galvan A, Hare TA, Davidson M, Spicer J, Glover G, Casey BJ. O le matafaioi a le matagaluega a le frontostriatal i totonu o aʻoaʻoga faʻavae i luga o taui i totonu o tagata. J Neurosci. 2005;25(38): 8650-8656. [PubMed]
  • Galvan A, Hare TA, Parra CE, Penn J, Voss H, Glover G, et al. O le atinaʻe muamua o le faʻalavelave e fesoʻotaʻi ma le cortex orbitofrontal atonu e ono aʻafia ai amioga e ono aʻafia ai i le taulealea. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26(25): 6885-6892. [PubMed]
  • Gardner M, Steinberg L. Aafiaga a tupulaga i le faia o tulaga lamatia, mea e fiafia i ai, ma le faia o filifiliga lamatia i le talavou ma le matua: o se suʻesuʻega faʻataʻitaʻiga. Dev Psychol. 2005;41(4): 625-635. [PubMed]
  • Geier CF, Luna B. The maturation of incentive processing and cognitive control. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;93(3): 212-221. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Geier CF, Terwilliger R, Teslovich T, Velanova K, Luna B. Faamoemoe i le Faʻasalaga o Taui ma Ona Aafiaga i le Faʻatonuina o le Faʻatonuina i Tupulaga Talavou. Cereb Cortex. 2009
  • Giedd JN. Fa'ata'otoga fa'amaneta resonance o le fai'ai talavou. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021: 77-85. [PubMed]
  • Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, et al. Atinaʻe o le faiʻai i le taimi o tamaiti ma talavou: o se suʻesuʻega MRI umi. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10): 861-863. [PubMed]
  • Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, Rajapakse JC, Casey BJ, Kaysen D, et al. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4-18. Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6: 551-560. [PubMed]
  • Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis AC, et al. Fa'afanua malosi o le atina'eina o le tino i le taimi o le tamaitiiti e o'o atu i le matua. Taualumaga a le National Academy of Sciences, ISA. 2004;101(21): 8174-8179.
  • Graber JA, Brooks-Gunn J. Puberty. I totonu: Blechman EA, Brownell KD, faatonu. Behavioral medicine and women: A comprehensive handbook. Guilford Press; New York, NY: 1998. pp. 51–58.
  • Hare TA, Tottenham N, Galvan A, Voss HU, Glover GH, Casey BJ. Faʻavae o meaola o le gaioiga faʻalagona ma tulafono faatonutonu i le talavou i le taimi o se galuega go-nogo faʻalagona. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(10): 927-934. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Harnishfeger KK, Bjorklund DF. The ontogeny of inhibition mechanisms: A renewed approach to cognitive development. In: Howe ML, Pasnek R, editors. Autu fa'atupu i le atina'eina o le mafaufau. Vol. 1. Springer-Verlag; New York: 1993.
  • Hikosaka K, Watanabe M. Faʻatuai le gaioiga o le faʻaogaina o le orbital ma le lateral prefrontal neurons o le manuki e fesuisuiaʻi ma taui eseese. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(3): 263-271. [PubMed]
  • Huttenlocher PR. Synaptic density i le pito i luma o le tagata - faʻalautelega suiga ma aʻafiaga o le matua. Suesueina o Brain. 1979;163: 195-205. [PubMed]
  • Hyman SE, Malenka RC. Addiction and the brain: the neurobiology of compulsion and its persistence. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2(10): 695-703. [PubMed]
  • Jernigan TL, Zisook S, Heaton RK, Moranville JT, Hesselink JR, Braff DL. Fa'ailoga fa'amaneta fa'amaneta i le nuclei lenticular ma le cerebral cortex i le schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48(10): 881-890. [PubMed]
  • Keating DP, Bobbitt BL. Eseesega o tagata taʻitoʻatasi ma atinaʻe i vaega o le gaioiga o le mafaufau o le mafaufau. Atinaʻeina o Tama. 1978;49: 155-167.
  • Kelley AE, Schochet T, Landry CF. Risk taking and novelty seeking in adolescence: introduction to part I. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021: 27-32. [PubMed]
  • Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Faʻateleina gaioiga faiʻai i luma ma parietal cortex faʻavaeina le atinaʻeina o le malosi o le mafaufau galue visuospatial i le taimi o le tamaitiiti. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(1): 1-10. [PubMed]
  • Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D. O le faʻamoemoe o le faʻaopoopoga o le tau tupe e filifilia ma le filifilia e le gata i lea. J Neurosci. 2001;21(16): RC159. [PubMed]
  • Kuhnen CM, Knutson B. Le faavae neural o tulaga lamatia tau tupe. Neuron. 2005;47(5): 763-770. [PubMed]
  • Laviola G, Adriani W, Terranova ML, Gerra G. Psychobiological tulaga lamatia mo le tulaga lamatia i psychostimulants i tama talavou ma meaola manu. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23(7): 993-1010. [PubMed]
  • Laviola G, Macri S, Morley-Fletcher S, Adriani W. O amioga e ono lamatia ai le gaioio talavou: suesuega o le psychobiology ma le vave epigenetic influence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003;27(1-2): 19-31. [PubMed]
  • Levita L, Hare TA, Voss HU, Glover G, Ballon DJ, Casey BJ. O le itu lua o le nucleus accumbens. Neuroimage. 2009;44(3): 1178-1187. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Liston C, Watts R, Tottenham N, Davidson MC, Niogi S, Ulug AM, et al. Frontostriatal microstructure faʻaleleia le faʻaleleia lelei o le puleaina o le mafaufau. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16(4): 553-560. [PubMed]
  • Luna B, Sweeney JA. Le tulaʻi mai o le galulue faʻatasi o le faiʻai: suʻesuʻega FMRI o le atinaʻeina o le faʻalavelave tali. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021: 296-309. [PubMed]
  • Luna B, Thulborn KR, Munoz DP, Merriam EP, Garver KE, Minshew NJ, et al. O le fa'amatuaina o galuega fai'ai fa'asalalau lautele e fa'aitiitia ai le atina'eina o le mafaufau. Neuroimage. 2001;13(5): 786-793. [PubMed]
  • Martin CA, Logan TK, Portis C, Leukefeld CG, Lynam D, Staton M, et al. The association of testosterone with nicotine use in young adult females. Addict Behav. 2001;26(2): 279-283. [PubMed]
  • Matthews SC, Simmons AN, Lane SD, Paulus MP. O le fa'agaoioia filifilia o le nucleus accumbens i le taimi o le faia o fa'ai'uga fa'aletonu. Neuroreport. 2004;15(13): 2123-2127. [PubMed]
  • Me JC, Delgado MR, Dahl RE, Stenger VA, Ryan ND, Fiez JA, et al. Faʻailoga faʻapitoa faʻataʻitaʻi faʻamaonia ata o vaʻaiga o faiʻai i taui i fanau ma tamaiti. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;55(4): 359-366. [PubMed]
  • McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. O faiga neural tu'u'ese'ese e fa'atauaina vave ma fa'atuai taui tau tupe. Saienisi. 2004;306(5695): 503-507. [PubMed]
  • Miller EK, Cohen JD. O se talitonuga faʻavae o galuega faʻapitoa muamua. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24: 167-202. [PubMed]
  • Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez MI. Fa'atuai o le fa'amalieina o tamaiti. Saienisi. 1989;244(4907): 933-938. [PubMed]
  • Monk CS, McClure EB, Nelson EE, Zarahn E, Punaoa RM, Leibenluff E, et al. Tupulaga laʻitiiti i le gaioiga o le mafaufau i le mafaufau e faʻaalia i foliga faʻalagona. Neuroimage. 2003;20: 420-428. [PubMed]
  • Montague PR, Berns GS. Neural economics ma mea olaola o le tau. Neuron. 2002;36(2): 265-284. [PubMed]
  • Mose P, Roe K, Buxton RB, Wong EC, Frank LR, Stiles J. Functional MRI o le lalolagi atoa ma le gaioiga faʻapitonuʻu i tamaiti. Neuroimage. 2002;16(2): 415-424. [PubMed]
  • Munakata Y, Yerys BE. All together now: when dissociations between knowledge and action disappear. Psycho Sci. 2001;12(4): 335-337. [PubMed]
  • Nagy Z, Westerberg H, Klingberg T. O le maturation o mea papaʻe e fesoʻotaʻi ma le atinaʻeina o galuega faʻapitoa i le taimi o le tamaitiiti. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(7): 1227-1233. [PubMed]
  • Nelson EE, Leibenluft E, McClure EB, Pine DS. Le faʻasalalauga faʻaagafesootai o le talavou: o se vaʻaiga o le neuroscience i le faagasologa ma lona sootaga i le psychopathology. Mafaufauga Psychol. 2005;35(2): 163-174. [PubMed]
  • O'Doherty JP. Taui faʻailoga ma aʻoaʻoga faʻatatau i le faiʻai o le tagata: malamalamaaga mai neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14(6): 769-776. [PubMed]
  • O’Doherty JP, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4(1): 95-102. [PubMed]
  • Pascual-Leone JA. O se fa'ata'ita'iga fa'a-matematika mo le suiga i la'asaga o atina'e a Piaget. Acta Psychologica. 1970;32: 301-345.
  • Pecina S, Cagniard B, Berridge KC, Aldridge JW, Zhuang X. Hyperdopaminergic mutant isumu e maualuga atu le "manaʻo" ae le "fiafia" mo taui suamalie. J Neurosci. 2003;23(28): 9395-9402. [PubMed]
  • Rakic P. e. a. Synaptic development of the cerebral cortex: implications for learning, memory and mental illness. Prog. Brain Res. 1994;102: 227-243. [PubMed]
  • Romeo RD. Faʻaauau: o se vaitaimi o faʻalapotopotoga ma faʻamalosiaga o hormones i luga o le atinaʻe o neurobehavioural. J Neuroendocrinol. 2003;15(12): 1185-1192. [PubMed]
  • Rosenberg DR, Lewis DA. Faʻailogaina o le faʻailogaina o le inanervation o le manuki i le pito i luma ma le afi: o le tyrosine hydroxylase immunohistochnical analysis. J Comp Neurol. 1995;358(3): 383-400. [PubMed]
  • Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer M, Williams SC, Simmons A, et al. Functional frontalisation with age: mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories with fMRI. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24(1): 13-19. [PubMed]
  • Schlaggar BL, Brown TT, Lugar HM, Visscher KM, Miezin FM, Petersen SE. Faʻasologa o neuroanatomical functional i le va o tagata matutua ma tamaiti aʻoga i le gaosiga o upu taʻitasi. Saienisi. 2002;296(5572): 1476-1479. [PubMed]
  • Schultz W. Faʻasologa o taleni ma le neurophysiology o le taui. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006;57: 87-115. [PubMed]
  • Scott ES. Judgment and Reasoning in Adolescent Decisionmaking. Villanova Law Review. 1992;37: 1607-1669. [PubMed]
  • Somerville LH, Hare TA, Casey BJ. Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. i le lomitusi.
  • Somerville LH, Jones RM, Casey BJ. A time of change: behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1): 124-133. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Susuga SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW. Faʻasologaina o suiga faʻasolosolo i le salafa o le olaga o le tagata. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(3): 309-315. [PubMed]
  • Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Jernigan TL, Toga AW. In vivo faʻamaoniga mo le matua o le faiʻai pe a maeʻa le talavou i luma ma pito i tua. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10): 859-861. [PubMed]
  • Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Toga AW. Fa'afanua suiga i le tino o le tagata i le gasologa o le olaga. Neuroscientist. 2004;10(4): 372-392. [PubMed]
  • Spear LP. O le mafaufau o le tagata matua ma ona uiga e uiga i le matua. Neuroscience ma Biobehavioral Reviews. 2000;24(4): 417-463. [PubMed]
  • Spear LP. Le Neuroscience Faʻasalalau o le Tauleʻaleʻa. W.W. Norton & Company; New York: 2009.
  • Steinberg L. Risk i le laʻititi: o a suiga, ma aisea? Ann NY Acad Sci. 2004;1021: 51-58. [PubMed]
  • Steinberg L. O se vaaiga faʻafesoʻotaʻi faʻale-agafesootai e uiga i le lamatiaga o le tauleʻaleʻa. Atinaeina o Atinae. 2008;28: 78-106. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Steinberg L, Albert D, Cauffman E, Banich M, Graham S, Woolard J. Age eseese i le sailia o lagona ma le le mautonu e pei ona faasinoupuina e le amio ma lipoti faaletagata lava ia: faamaoniga mo se faiga faataitai lua. Dev Psychol. 2008;44(6): 1764-1778. [PubMed]
  • Steinberg L, Graham S, O’Brien L, Woolard J, Cauffman E, Banich M. Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Tamaiti Dev. 2009;80(1): 28-44. [PubMed]
  • Tamm L, Menon V, Reiss AL. O le matua o le gaioiga o le faiʻai e fesoʻotaʻi ma le faʻalavelave tali. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(10): 1231-1238. [PubMed]
  • Thomas KM, Hunt RH, Vizueta N, Sommer T, Durston S, Yang Y, et al. Evidence of developmental differences in implicit sequence learning: an fMRI study of children and adults. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(8): 1339-1351. [PubMed]
  • Turkeltaub PE, Gareau L, Fugalaau DL, Zeffiro TA, Eden GF. Atina'e o faiga neural mo le faitau. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6(7): 767-773. [PubMed]
  • Van Leijenhorst L, Moor BG, Op de Macks ZA, Rombouts SA, Westenberg PM, Crone EA. Fa'atupu fa'ai'uga fa'aletonu a le tupulaga: Neurocognitive atina'e o taui ma fa'atonu vaega. Neuroimage. 2010
  • Van Leijenhorst L, Zanolie K, Van Meel CS, Westenberg PM, Rombouts SA, Crone EA. O le a le mea e faaosofia ai le talavou? Fa'aitu'u fai'ai fa'atalanoaina fa'aa'oa'oga taui i tupulaga talavou. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(1): 61-69. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Li TK. Drug addiction: the neurobiology of behaviour gone awry. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(12): 963-970. [PubMed]
  • Windle M, Spear LP, Fuligni AJ, Angold A, Brown JD, Pine D, et al. Transitions into underage and problem drinking: developmental processes and mechanisms between 10 and 15 years of age. Pediatrics. 2008;121: S273-S289. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Zald DH, Boileau I, El-Dearedy W, Gunn R, McGlone F, Dichter GS, et al. Dopamine tuʻuina atu i le striatum tagata i taimi o galuega tau tupe. J Neurosci. 2004;24(17): 4105-4112. [PubMed]