Novelty, conditioning and attentional bias to sexual rewards (2015)

COMMENTS: New Cambridge University brain study. Subjects were carefully screened porn addicts. Compared with controls, they habituated faster to sexual images. That is, their brains became less activated seeing the same image…they were more quickly bored. Thus, the novelty of internet porn drives the addiction to it, creating a circular spiral of needing more novelty to overcome faster habituation. But this desire for novelty in porn addicts was NOT pre-existing. That is, the ‘chicken’ is porn use and the ‘egg’ is novelty-seeking.

Press Release. November 23, 2015

People who show compulsive sexual behaviour – sex addiction – are driven to search more for new sexual images than their peers, according to new research led by the University of Cambridge. The findings may be particularly relevant in the context of online porn, which potentially provides an almost endless source of new images.

In a study published in the Journal of Psychiatric Research, researchers also report that sex addicts are more susceptible to environment ‘cues’ linked to sexual images than to those linked to neutral images.

Sex addiction – when an individual has difficulty controlling their sexual thoughts, feelings or behaviour – is relatively common, affecting as many as one in 25 young adults. It is heavily stigmatised and can lead to a sense of shame, affecting an individual’s family and social life as well as their work. There is no formal definition of the condition to help with diagnosis.

In previous work led by Dr Valerie Voon from the Department of Psychiatry at the University of Cambridge, scientists found that three brain regions were more active in sex addicts compared with the healthy volunteers. Significantly, these regions – the ventral striatum, dorsal anterior cingulate and amygdala – were regions that are also activated in drug addicts when shown drug stimuli.

In the new study, funded by the Wellcome Trust, Dr Voon and colleagues studied the behaviour of 22 sex addicts and 40 ‘healthy’ male volunteers undergoing tasks. In the first task, individuals were shown a series of images in pairs, including naked women, clothed women and furniture. They were then shown further image pairs, including familiar and new images, and asked to choose an image to ‘win £1’ – although the participants were unaware of the odds, the probability of winning for either images was 50%.

The researchers found that sex addicts were more likely to choose the novel over the familiar choice for sexual images relative to neutral object images, whereas healthy volunteers were more likely to choose the novel choice for neutral human female images relative to neutral object images.

“We can all relate in some way to searching for novel stimuli online – it could be flitting from one news website to another, or jumping from Facebook to Amazon to YouTube and on,” explains Dr Voon. “For people who show compulsive sexual behaviour, though, this becomes a pattern of behaviour beyond their control, focused on pornographic images.”

In a second task, volunteers were shown pairs of images – an undressed woman and a neutral grey box – both of which were overlaid on different abstract patterns. They learned to associate these abstract images with the images, similar to how the dogs in Pavlov’s famous experiment learnt to associate a ringing bell with food. They were then asked to select between these abstract images and a new abstract image.

This time, the researchers showed that sex addicts were more likely to choose cues (in this case the abstract patterns) associated with sexual and monetary rewards. This supports the notion that apparently innocuous cues in an addict’s environment can ‘trigger’ them to seek out sexual images.

“Cues can be as simple as just opening up their internet browser,” explains Dr Voon. “They can trigger a chain of actions and before they know it, the addict is browsing through pornographic images. Breaking the link between these cues and the behaviour can be extremely challenging.”

The researchers carried out a further test where 20 sex addicts and 20 matched healthy volunteers underwent brain scans while being shown a series of repeated images – an undressed woman, a £1 coin or a neutral grey box.

They found that when the sex addicts viewed the same sexual image repeatedly, compared to the healthy volunteers they experienced a greater decrease of activity in the region of the brain known as the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, known to be involved in anticipating rewards and responding to new events. This is consistent with ‘habituation’, where the addict finds the same stimulus less and less rewarding – for example, a coffee drinker may get a caffeine ‘buzz’ from their first cup, but over time the more they drink coffee, the smaller the buzz becomes.

This same habituation effect occurs in healthy males who are repeatedly shown the same porn video. But when they then view a new video, the level of interest and arousal goes back to the original level. This implies that, to prevent habituation, the sex addict would need to seek out a constant supply of new images. In other words, habituation could drive the search for novel images.

“Our findings are particularly relevant in the context of online pornography,” adds Dr Voon. “It’s not clear what triggers sex addiction in the first place and it is likely that some people are more pre-disposed to the addiction than others, but the seemingly endless supply of novel sexual images available online helps feed their addiction, making it more and more difficult to escape.”

More information: Paula Banca et al. Novelty, conditioning and attentional bias to sexual rewards, Journal of Psychiatric Research (2016). DOI: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.10.017

 


THE STUDY

Paula Banca, Laurel S. Morris, Simon MitchellNeil A. Harrison, Marc N. Potenza, Valerie Voon (Dr)correspondenceemail

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.10.017

Abstract

The Internet provides a large source of novel and rewarding stimuli, particularly with respect to sexually explicit materials. Novelty-seeking and cue-conditioning are fundamental processes underlying preference and approach behaviors implicated in disorders of addiction. Here we examine these processes in individuals with compulsive sexual behaviors (CSB), hypothesizing a greater preference for sexual novelty and stimuli conditioned to sexual rewards relative to healthy volunteers. Twenty-two CSB males and forty age-matched male volunteers were tested in two separate behavioral tasks focusing on preferences for novelty and conditioned stimuli. Twenty subjects from each group were also assessed in a third conditioning and extinction task using functional magnetic resonance imaging. CSB was associated with enhanced novelty preference for sexual, as compared to control images, and a generalized preference for cues conditioned to sexual and monetary versus neutral outcomes compared to healthy volunteers. CSB individuals also had greater dorsal cingulate habituation to repeated sexual versus monetary images with the degree of habituation correlating with enhanced preference for sexual novelty. Approach behaviors to sexually conditioned cues dissociable from novelty preference were associated with an early attentional bias to sexual images. This study shows that CSB individuals have a dysfunctional enhanced preference for sexual novelty possibly mediated by greater cingulate habituation along with a generalized enhancement of conditioning to rewards. We further emphasize a dissociable role for cue-conditioning and novelty preference on the early attentional bias for sexual cues. These findings have wider relevance as the Internet provides a wide range of novel and potentially rewarding stimuli.

Keywords: novelty, cue-conditioning, sexual reward, dorsal cingulate habituation, addiction, attentional bias

Introduction

Why is online surfing so compulsively engaging to many individuals? The Internet provides a large source of novel and potentially rewarding stimuli. Novelty-seeking, attentional bias and cue-conditioning are fundamental processes that may drive unconscious preference and approach decisions in daily life. These processes may also contribute to the development and maintenance of disorders of addiction.

Novelty-seeking may be both a predictor and consequence of disorders of addiction. This trait, which is often assessed using the Zuckerman’s sensation-seeking scale, has been repeatedly found elevated in diverse behavioral and substance addictions (Belin et al., 2011, Redolat et al., 2009). A suggested explanation for this strong relationship relies on the hypothesis that exposure to novelty may activate, at least in part, the same neural machinery that mediates the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse (Bardo et al., 1996). In rodent studies, novelty preference predicts a transition towards compulsive cocaine-seeking behaviors (Belin and Deroche-Gamonet, 2012). In human studies, sensation-seeking is associated prospectively with binge drinking in adolescents (Conrod et al., 2013).

Conditioned signals or cues in our environment may also significantly influence behavior. The smell of cigarettes, places or friends associated with drug use, or the sight of money may act as conditioned cues and may enhance reactivity and trigger cravings, urges and relapse in disorders of addiction (for review see (Childress et al., 1993)). These cues are neutral stimuli that may inadvertently acquire motivational significance through the process of conditioning with repeated pairing with either drug rewards or other biologically relevant natural rewards such as food (Jansen, 1998) or sex (Pfaus et al., 2001, Toates, 2009).

The processing of novelty and learning has been proposed to include a functional polysynaptic loop involving the hippocampus, ventral striatum, and midbrain dopaminergic region (Lisman and Grace, 2005). Detection of novelty, long-term memory encoding and learning involves dopaminergic activity that enhances hippocampal synaptic plasticity which, through glutamatergic projections to the ventral striatum, relays information to the ventral tegmental area (VTA) which then projects directly back to the hippocampus (Knight, 1996, Lisman and Grace, 2005). With repeated exposure, the hippocampus and midbrain dopaminergic responses to novelty decrease, conferring habituation when stimuli become familiar (Bunzeck and Duzel, 2006, Bunzeck et al., 2013). Converging primate and human studies also show that phasic dopaminergic activity encodes prediction error, a comparison between the actual and expected outcomes indicating an unexpected salient outcome, acting as a teaching signal underlying conditioning processes (Schultz et al., 1997). Mesolimbic dopaminergic cell bodies in the midbrain project to a network including the striatum, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and hippocampus (Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1998). The dACC is implicated in attentional responding to novel and salient events and in processing reward anticipation and prediction error (Ranganath and Rainer, 2003, Rushworth et al., 2011).

In addition to novelty-seeking and cue-conditioning influences, the tendency to preferentially process cues related to the object of addiction (attentional biases) is also an important feature that characterizes disorders of addiction (Ersche et al., 2010, van Hemel-Ruiter et al., 2013, Wiers et al., 2011). The influence of emotional stimuli on attentional processes is widely reported both in healthy and clinical samples (Yiend, 2010). Attentional biases towards substance-related stimuli have been found in substance-use disorders for alcohol, nicotine, cannabis, opiates and cocaine (Cox et al., 2006). Moreover, a direct association between highly arousing sexual pictures and attentional interference has also been found in healthy individuals, which seems to be influenced by sexuality-related attitudes and sexual motivation (Kagerer et al., 2014, Prause et al., 2008). We have previously extended these findings to individuals with compulsive sexual behaviour (CSB) using a dot-probe task (Mechelmans et al., 2014).

With increasing access to the Internet, there is growing concern regarding the potential for excessive use. A study that assessed the predictive power of several types of Internet applications (gaming, gambling, email, etc.) on the development of compulsive Internet use suggested that online sexually explicit stimuli have the highest potential for addictive/compulsive use (Meerkerk et al., 2006). Online explicit stimuli are vast and expanding, and this feature may promote escalation of use in some individuals. For instance, healthy males viewing repeatedly the same explicit film have been found to habituate to the stimulus and find the explicit stimulus as progressively less sexually arousing, less appetitive and less absorbing (Koukounas and Over, 2000). However, subsequent exposure to a novel explicit film segment increases the levels of sexual arousal and absorption to the same previous levels prior to habituation, suggesting important roles for novelty and habituation. Imaging studies have identified a specific network for the neural processing of sexual stimuli in healthy humans, comprising the hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, orbitofrontal, occipital and parietal areas (Wehrum et al., 2013, Wehrum-Osinsky et al., 2014). This neural network, which is independent of general emotional arousal, is found both in men and women although men show overall stronger activations than women, which might be indicative of a stronger sexual responsivity in men. The same neural network activates for stimuli conditioned sexual arousal, with a gender effect in the same direction (Klucken et al., 2009).

In our study, we assess novelty, attentional bias and cue-conditioning to online explicit sexual material in individuals with CSB. These processes are highly relevant to substance-use disorders and may also be relevant to CSB. Online sexually explicit stimuli have significant potential for compulsive use, and CSB is relatively common, occurring in 2 to 4% in community and college-based young adults and in psychiatric inpatients (Grant et al., 2005, Odlaug and Grant, 2010, Odlaug et al., 2013). CSB is associated with significant distress, feelings of shame and psychosocial dysfunction. Although a working group for the 11th edition of the International Classification of Diseases is currently proposing to include CSB as an impulse-control disorder (Grant et al., 2014), CSB was not included in DSM-5, albeit with some controversy (Toussaint and Pitchot, 2013), largely due to limited data. Thus, further studies are needed. Understanding the similarities and differences between CSB and other psychiatric disorders, particularly impulse-control disorders and addictions, may help with classification efforts as well as with the development of improved prevention and treatment approaches.

We have previously found that individuals with CSB demonstrate greater regional brain activation in response to explicit sexual cues in the ventral striatum, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) and amygdala, regions implicated in drug cue reactivity and craving in disorders of addiction (Voon et al., 2014). Functional connectivity of this network, and particularly the dACC, was associated with greater sexual desire or motivation to explicit stimuli. We further observed that individuals with CSB, as compared to those without, demonstrate an early attentional bias towards sexually explicit cues (Mechelmans, Irvine, 2014). This early attentional bias was proposed to reflect facilitatory mechanisms underlying the motivational effect of cues conditioned to sexual outcomes. Here, we deepen our research focus investigating the mechanisms underlying the development of enhanced attentional bias and cue reactivity in CSB by assessing both behavioral and neural responses to novelty and to cue-conditioning in response to explicit sexual stimuli.

We conducted two behavioral tasks outside of the scanner to assess choice preference for novel versus familiar sexual stimuli and choice preference for cues conditioned to Sexual, Monetary and Neutral stimuli. We hypothesized that CSB individuals relative to healthy volunteers (HVs) would have greater choice preference to novel relative to familiar images in the Sexual condition but not in the control condition. We further hypothesized that CSB subjects would have greater choice preference to the conditioned cues in the Sexual condition but not in the Monetary condition.

Participants also performed a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) conditioning and extinction task involving conditioning to Sexual, Monetary and Neutral images. Two neutral stimuli were randomly paired with different sexual images shown repeatedly during conditioning. In the outcome phase of the conditioning arm, neural habituation to the sexual images was assessed by evaluating the change in neural activity of each different sexual image over time focusing on the repeated exposure thereby dissociating the analysis of the conditioning and outcome phases. We hypothesized that CSB subjects relative to HVs would show enhanced neural activity to the Sexual versus Neutral conditioned stimuli particularly in the dACC and striatum, regions previously identified in sexual cue reactivity in CSB subjects (Voon, Mole, 2014). We further hypothesized that CSB subjects compared to HVs would show greater neural habituation to Sexual compared to Neutral stimuli.

Method

Recruitment

The recruitment has been extensively described elsewhere (Voon, Mole, 2014). CSB subjects were recruited via Internet-based advertisements and therapist referrals. HVs were recruited from community-based advertisements in East Anglia. CSB subjects were interviewed by a psychiatrist to confirm they fulfilled diagnostic criteria for CSB (proposed diagnostic criteria for Hypersexual Disorder; criteria for sexual addiction) (Carnes et al., 2001, Kafka, 2010, Reid et al., 2012), focusing on compulsive use of online sexually explicit material.

All CSB subjects and comparably aged HVs were male and heterosexual given the nature of the cues. HVs were matched in a 2:1 ratio with CSB subjects to increase statistical power. Exclusionary criteria included being under 18 years of age, history of substance-use disorders, current regular user of illicit substances (including cannabis), and having a serious psychiatric disorder, including current moderate-severe major depression (Beck Depression Inventory > 20) or obsessive-compulsive disorder, or history of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia (Mini International Neuropsychiatric Inventory) (Sheehan et al., 1998). Other compulsive or behavioral addictions were exclusions, which were assessed by a psychiatrist, including problematic use of online gaming or social media, pathological gambling or compulsive shopping and binge-eating disorder.

Subjects completed the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale (Whiteside and Lynam, 2001), Beck Depression Inventory (Beck et al., 1961), State Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger et al., 1983) and the Alcohol-Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (Saunders et al., 1993). The National Adult Reading Test (Nelson, 1982) was used to obtain an index of IQ.

Two CSB subjects were taking antidepressants and had comorbid generalized anxiety disorder and social phobia: social phobia (N=1) and a childhood history of ADHD (N=1).

Written informed consent was obtained and the study was approved by the University of Cambridge Research Ethics Committee. Subjects were paid for their participation.

Behavioral tasks

Twenty-two CSB subjects and 40 comparably aged male volunteers were tested in a novelty-preference task and two conditioning-preference tasks reported here, and an attentional bias task (dot-probe task) reported elsewhere (Mechelmans, Irvine, 2014). The tasks were carried out after the fMRI experiment, in a counterbalanced order.

Novelty preference

Subjects were familiarized to three categories of stimuli (Sexual images, Neutral human images and Neutral object images) and then performed a choice-discrimination testing phase, choosing between novel versus familiar stimuli matched within each category (Figure 1A). In the familiarization phase, six images were shown to the participant: 2 images of undressed women (Sexual condition), 2 images of dressed women (Control1) and 2 images of a piece of furniture (Control2) (2 images per condition). The 6 images were randomly presented in pairs to the participants, in a total of 48 trials (16 trials each condition). The duration of each trial was 5 sec. To ensure engagement with the task, subjects were instructed to carefully study the images because they would be asked questions during the familiarization phase. Simple questions about the images were randomly posed during the task in the inter-trial interval (e.g., to indicate which woman had her arms crossed using the right or left arrow: ‘Arms crossed’). Each question was relevant to the previously viewed pair of images, hence ensuring that subjects maintained attention to each pair of images.

Thumbnail image of Figure 1. Opens large image

Figure 1

Novelty and conditioning behavioral measures. A. Novelty preference: task and outcomes. Subjects were familiarized with sexual images and two non-sexual control images followed by a choice-discrimination task involving choosing between a familiar or matched novel choice randomly (p=0.50) associated with winning. The graph shows the proportion of novelty choices across trials in subjects with compulsive sexual behavior (CSB) and healthy volunteers (HV). B. Conditioning: task and outcomes. The sexual conditioning task is shown. During conditioning, two black-and-white visual patterns (CS+Sex and CS-) were followed by sexual or neutral images respectively. During choice discrimination testing, subjects chose between CS+Sex and CS- paired with novel visual-pattern stimuli (A and B). The CS+Sex and CS- stimuli were associated with greater probabilities of winning. The graphs show the proportion of conditioned stimuli choices across trials of CSB and HV for Sexual outcomes (left) and Monetary outcomes (right). *Group-by-Valence interaction: p<0.05.

In the testing phase, subjects viewed three image-pairs consisting of a familiarized image and a novel image matched for each experimental condition. Six images were used: 3 familiar, selected from the previous familiarization phase (one for each of the three conditions) and 3 new images (one novel for each condition). The image-pair was shown for 2.5 seconds followed by a 1-second feedback (win £1 or win nothing). A total of 60 trials (20 trials each condition) were presented. The probability of winning for any of the images was random at p=0.50. The subject was instructed to choose one of the stimuli from the pair with the goal to make as much money as possible and told that they would receive a proportion of their earnings. They were instructed that the first trial would be a guess but that one of the stimuli would be associated with a greater likelihood of winning. The primary outcome measure was the proportion of novel choices across trials for each condition. Since the learning contingencies used here were purely random (p=0.50), the outcome measure exclusively indicates a stimuli preference. After the study, subjects were asked to rate attractiveness of the female subjects on a scale of 1 to 10 following testing. Task duration was 8 minutes (4 min for the training and 3.5 min for the testing phase).

Conditioning preference

Subjects were tested on two conditioning preference tasks in a counterbalanced order, both consisting of a conditioning phase and a testing phase (Figure 1B). Both tasks had the same design but one focused on sexual and the other on monetary conditioning.

In one training phase, two visual patterns (CS+Sex, CS-), presented for 2 seconds, were conditioned to an image of an undressed female or a neutral grey box (1-second outcome), respectively. This was followed by an inter-trial interval of 0.5 to 1 second. Sixty trials were presented in total (30 CS+ and 30 CS). To ensure task engagement, subjects were instructed to keep track of the number of times they saw a red square around the outcome image, and they reported this number at the end of the training phase.

The training phase was followed by a testing phase in which the CS+Sex and CS- stimuli were each paired with a novel visual-pattern stimulus (e.g. Image A or Image B respectively). Subjects were asked to choose one of the stimuli from the stimulus pair (e.g. CS+Sex or Image A; CS- or Image B; duration 2.5 seconds), which was followed by feedback of win £1 or win nothing (duration 1 second). The CS+Sex and CS- had a greater probability of winning (p=0.70 win £1/p=0.30 win nothing) relative to the novel paired stimulus (p=0.70 win nothing/p=0.30 win £1). Subjects were tested for 40 trials in total (20 trials per condition) and were told the goal was to make as much money as possible and that they would receive a proportion of their earnings. They were instructed that the first trial would be a guess but that one of the stimuli would be associated with a greater likelihood of winning.

In the second training and testing task, a similar task design was used paired with monetary outcomes: a different set of visual patterns was conditioned (CS+Money, CS-) to the image of £1 or a neutral grey box. Subjects were told they would win a proportion of the money they viewed. A similar testing phase followed.

As the CS+ and CS- stimuli were associated with higher probabilities of winning, we assessed novelty choice preference of the first trial to assess initial approach behaviors and the proportion of times the CS+ and CS- stimuli were selected across all trials to assess the influence of choice preference of the cue on instrumental learning. Each task lasted approximately 7 minutes (4 min for the training and 2.5 min for the testing phases).

Imaging task

Twenty CSB subjects and 20 matched HVs were scanned performing a conditioning and extinction task (Figure 3A ). In the conditioning phase, six images (colored patterns) were used as conditioned stimuli (CS+) paired with the unconditioned stimulus (US) image of an undressed female (CS+sex), £1 (CS+money) or a neutral grey box (CS-). Two CS+ were paired per outcome. Five different images of undressed females were used for the sexual outcomes and repeated 8 times over the course of conditioning. The CS+ duration was 2000 msec; at 1500 msec, the US was overlaid for 500 msec and followed by a response block with a central fixation point, which ranged from 500 to 2500 msec. To maintain attention to the task, subjects pressed the left button for the money outcome, the right button for the person outcome, and either button for the neutral outcome during the fixation period. Subjects viewed a total of 120 trials (20 per CS+ or 40 per condition) in the conditioning phase. The conditions were randomly presented. In the extinction phase, each CS+ was shown for 2000 msec without the US for a total of 90 trials (15 per CS+ or 30 per condition) followed by a fixation point (500 to 2500 msec). Thus, at 1500 msec, subjects would expect an outcome, which was unexpectedly omitted. Prior to the study, subjects were trained outside of the scanner on 20 trials of a similar design with different CS+ and images of females, money and neutral items to practice the motor response during the response block. During the practice, subjects viewed images of dressed females but were told that in the scanner, they might see explicit stimuli. All tasks were programmed using E-prime professional v2.0 software.

Thumbnail image of Figure 2. Opens large image

Figure 2

Relationship between choice preferences and attentional bias across groups. The left graph shows early attentional bias scores for sexual versus neutral stimuli (higher scores indicated greater bias towards sexual versus neutral stimuli) in subjects who preferred the CS+Sex as compared to CS- as the first choice across both groups. *p<0.05. The right graph shows early attentional bias scores for sexual versus neutral stimuli in subjects who preferred the novel sexual stimulus as compared to the familiar stimulus.

Thumbnail image of Figure 3. Opens large image

Figure 3

Conditioning imaging task and habituation. A. Imaging task. During conditioning, subjects viewed six colored patterns followed by a Sexual, Monetary or Neutral image. The extinction phase followed, during which the conditioned stimulus was shown without the unconditioned stimulus. B. Habituation. Habituation of dorsal anterior cingulate (dACC) activity in compulsive sexual behavior (CSB) subjects versus healthy volunteers (HV) to repeated Sexual versus Neutral images. The image shows the comparison of the first and last half of the trials. C. Slope and intercept of dACC habituation. The graphs show the slope or degree of habituation (left graph) of beta values of the dACC in CSB and HV individuals and the intercept or initial activity of CSB versus HV (right graph) of Sexual – Neutral (Sex) and Monetary – Neutral (Money) images. *Valence and Group-by-Valence effects p<0.05; **Valence effect p<0.05.

View Large Image | Download PowerPoint Slide

Statistical analysis of behavioural data

Subject characteristics were analysed using independent t-tests or Chi Square. Data was inspected for outliers (>3 SD from group mean) and tested for normality of distribution (Shapiro Wilks test). Choice preference averaged across all trials for both novelty and conditioning tasks were assessed using mixed measures ANOVA with the between-subjects factor of Group (CSB, HV) and within-subject factor of Valence (Sexual, Control1, Control2; CS+, CS-). Choices for the first trial were also analyzed using Chi-Square tests. P<0.05 was considered significant.

Neuroimaging

Imaging data acquisition

Participants were scanned in a 3T Siemens Magnetom TimTrio scanner, at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, University of Cambridge, with a 32-channel head coil. Anatomical images were obtained using a T1-weighted structural image using an MPRAGE sequence (TR=2300 ms; TE=2.98 ms; FOV 240 x 256 x 176 mm, voxel size 1x1x1 mm). fMRI data were acquired using blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast whole-brain echo-planar imaging (EPI) with the following parameters: 39 interleaved axial slices per volume, TR 2.32 s, TA 2.26, TE 33 ms, 3mm slice thickness.

Data analyses were conducted using Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM 8) (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). Pre-processing consisted of slice-time correction, spatial realignment, coregistration with the subjects’ T1-weighted structural images, normalization, and spatial smoothing (full-width at half-maximum of 8 mm). The first 4 volumes of each session were discarded to allow for T1-equilibration effects.

Imaging data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using a general-linear-model (GLM) modeling the conditioning and extinction phases for both conditioned stimuli and outcomes separately for all 3 categories. Realignment parameters were included to correct for motion artifact. The time of onset of the outcome omission in the extinction phase used was 1500 msec after the onset of the stimulus (or the time at which the outcome would have been expected in the conditioning phase) with a 500 msec duration.

For each condition, the conditioned stimuli (CS+Sex, CS+Money, CS-) were averaged across trials separately for the conditioning and extinction phase, and also for the outcome in the extinction phase. The two different stimuli were averaged within the same condition. In the second level analysis, we used a full factorial analysis (repeated measures ANOVA) comparing Group, Valence and Interactions for averaged trials. The different phases of the imaging task and description of the analyses is further illustrated in Figure 4.

Thumbnail image of Figure 4. Opens large image

Figure 4

Illustration of conditioning, habituation and extinction This figure illustrates the phases of the imaging task in which the conditioned stimuli are paired with outcomes (CS+sex shown here; CS+money conditioned to monetary outcomes and CS- conditioned to neutral outcomes were randomly interspersed and are not shown) and extinction phase in which only the conditioned stimuli are shown without the outcome. Two different CS+ for each outcome type or CS- were conditioned over 20 trials per stimulus. Five different sexual images (shown here with differing colours of the female stick image) were randomly paired with the two different CS+sex and were each shown 8 times. For the habituation analysis, the change in time of these repeated outcomes was analysed.

For the habituation analysis of the outcomes in the conditioning phase, we created regressors for the first and last half of the Sexual and Neutral outcomes in the first level analysis. Subjects were shown 5 different Sexual images 8 times across both CS+Sex trials. Thus for Sexual images, the first half corresponded with the first 4 Sexual image exposures for each of the 5 different images and the last half, the last 4 Sexual image exposures for each of the 5 different images. In the second level analysis, using a full factorial analysis, we compared activity in the first and last half of the Sexual versus Neutral outcomes using a between-subject factor of Group, and within-subject factors of Valence and Time. For all above analyses, whole brain cluster corrected FWE p<0.05 was considered significant.

As we identified an interaction between Group x Valence x Time in the dACC, we then used an SPM Toolbox, MarsBaR (MARSeille Boite A Region d’Interet), to extract the beta values on a trial-by-trial basis for each individual using the dACC central coordinate and radius of 5 mm. In the first level analysis, we created regressors to evaluate change on a trial-by-trial basis. For example, 8 regressors were created for the sexual outcome consisting of different sexual outcomes shown 8 times. We calculated the slope and intercept points of each of the three outcomes for each individual. The slope and intercept points were then separately entered into a mixed-measures ANOVA comparing Group as a between-subject factor and Valence as a within-subject factor. P<0.05 was considered significant.

Similarly, a psychophysiological-interaction analysis was conducted with the same dACC region-of-interest (ROI) seed comparing early versus late exposure to the sexual outcomes. In all analyses, activations above family-wise error (FWE) whole-brain corrected p<0.05 and 5 contiguous voxels were considered significant. We further conducted region of interest analyses focusing on a priori regions using WFU PickAtlas small volume correction (SVC) FWE-corrected with a Bonferroni correction for multiple ROI comparisons (p<0.0125).

Results

The characteristics of the CSB and HVs are reported in Table 1.

Table 1Subject characteristics.
CSBHVT/Chi squareP
Number2240
Age25.14 (4.68)25.20 (6.62)0.0370.970
Abstinence (days)32 (28.41)
EducationHigh school22400.0001.000
Current Univ.6130.1820.777
College degree350.0391.000
Univ. undergrad9140.2120.784
Masters degree634.4720.057
IQ110.49 (5.83)111.29 (8.39)0.3970.692
Relationship statusSingle10160.1730.790
Curr. relationship7160.4070.591
Married580.0641.000
OccupationStudent7150.2000.784
Part-time work321.4280.337
Full-time work12210.0241.000
Unemployed021.1370.535
MedicationsAntidepressants2
Current Smoking statusSmokers01
Body mass index24.91 (3.64)23.19 (4.38)1.5660.122
Binge EatingBES6.91 (6.46)5.72 (6.17)0.7150.478
Alcohol useAUDIT7.13 (4.11)6.29 (3.41)0.8620.392
DepressionBDI11.03 (9.81)5.38 (4.89)3.0390.004
AnxietySSAI44.59 (13.19)36.15 (13.29)2.3700.021
STAI49.54 (13.91)38.23 (14.57)2.9710.004
Obsessive compulsiveOCI-R19.23 (17.38)12.29 (11.72)1.8720.067
ImpulsivityUPPS-P150.83 (17.95)130.26 (23.49)3.569<0.001

Abbreviations: CSB = subjects with compulsive sexual behavior; HV = healthy volunteers; BES = Binge Eating Scale; AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; SSAI/STAI = Speilberger State and Trait Anxiety Inventory; OCI-R = Obsessive Compulsive Inventory; UPPS-P = UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale

Behavioral results

Novelty preference

For the averaged choice preference across 20 trials, there was a trend towards a Valence effect (F(1,59)=2.89, p=0.065) and a Group-by-Valence interaction (F(2,59)=3.46, p=0.035) and no Group effect (F(1,60)=1.47, p=0.230) (Figure 1A). Given the interaction effect, we conducted post-hoc analyses, which showed that CSB subjects had greater novelty preference for Sexual versus Control2 (p=0.039) whereas HV had greater novelty preference for Control1 versus Control2 (p=0.024).

For the choice preference for the first trial, although CSB subjects were less likely to choose the novel compared to familiar neutral stimulus (percent of first choice novel: Sexual, Control 1, Control 2: HV: 51.6%, 58.1%, 38.7%; CSB: 50.0%, 44.4%, 22.2%) there were no significant group differences (Sexual, Control1, Control2: Chi-square=0.012, 0.357, 0.235 p=0.541, 0.266, 0.193).

In summary, CSB subjects were more likely to choose the novel over the familiar choice for Sexual images relative to Neutral object images whereas HVs were more likely to choose the novel choice for Neutral human female images relative to Neutral object images.

Conditioning preference

Sexual conditioning task

For the averaged choice preference over 20 trials, there was a Valence effect (F(1,60)=5.413, p=0.024) and a Group-by-Valence effect (F(1,60)=4.566, p=0.037) in which CSB subjects were more likely to select the CS+Sex versus CS- compared to HVs (Figure 1B). There was no Group effect (F(1,60)=0.047, p=0.830). As there was an interaction effect, we conducted further post-hoc analyses: CSB subjects were more likely to select the CS+Sex versus the CS- (p=0.005) but not HVs (p=0.873). For the choice preference for the first trial, there were no differences between groups (percent of first choice CS+Sex: HV: 64.5%, CSB: 72.2%; Chi-square=0.308, p=0.410).

Monetary conditioning task

For the averaged choice preference over 20 trials, there was no significant effect of Valence (F(1,60)=1.450, p=0.235) or Group (F(1,60)=1.165, p=0.287). There was a Group-by-Valence effect (F(1,60)=4.761, p=0.035) (Figure 1B). For the choice preference for the first trial, there were no differences between groups (percent of first choice CS+Money: HV: 48.4%, CSB: 66.7%; Chi-square=1.538 p=0.173).

CSB subjects (attractiveness score 8.35, SD 1.49) had similar ratings of attractiveness of all female images relative to HVs (8.13, SD 1.45; t=0.566, p=0.573).

Thus, CSB subjects had greater preference for stimuli conditioned to either sexual images or money.

Relationship between choice preferences and attentional bias

We further investigated if there was any relationship between our previously published findings of enhanced attentional bias to sexual images (Mechelmans, Irvine, 2014) and the current findings of initial choice preference for novelty or for CS+Sex. Using independent t-tests we assessed early attentional bias for the sexual versus neutral images comparing choice preference for subjects who chose the CS- versus CS+Sex and separately Familiar versus Novel stimuli. Across both groups, subjects who chose the CS+Sex as compared to those who chose the CS- had enhanced attentional bias for the sexual versus neutral stimuli (t=-2.05, p=0.044). In contrast, there was no statistically significant difference between subjects who chose the Novel as compared to the Familiar and attentional bias scores for the sexual compared to neutral stimuli (t=0.751, p=0.458) (Figure 2).

Thus, our previously reported findings of early attentional bias may be related to conditioning preferences for sexual stimuli rather than novelty preferences for sexual stimuli.

Imaging results

Conditioning: cue

We first assessed averaged cue-conditioning across all trials. There was no Group effect. There was a Valence effect in which exposure to conditioned stimuli to Money (CS+Mon) and Sex (CS+Sex) as compared to Neutral (CS-) stimuli was associated with greater activity in the occipital cortex (all the following p-values report whole brain cluster corrected FWE p<0.05: peak cluster in Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates: X Y Z in mm: -6 -88 -6, Cluster size=3948, whole brain FWE p<0.0001), left primary motor cortex (X Y Z = -34 -24 52, Cluster size=5518, whole brain FWE p<0.0001) and bilateral putamen (left: X Y Z = -24 -2 4, Cluster size=338, whole brain FWE p<0.0001; right:X Y Z = 24 4 2, Cluster size=448, FWE p<0.0001), and thalamus (X Y Z = -0 -22 0, Cluster size=797, p<0.0001) activity. There was no Group-by-Valence interaction.

Extinction: cue

We then assessed the Extinction phase of the conditioned stimuli. There was a Valence effect in which CS+Sex and CS+Mon versus CS- exposure was associated with greater occipital cortex activity (X Y Z = -10 -94 2, Cluster size=2172, whole brain FWE p<0.0001). There were no Group or Interaction effects.

Acquisition: outcome

To examine effects of habituation to sexual novelty, we first investigated if any regions had a greater decrease in activity to Sexual outcomes in CSB subjects compared to HVs by comparing the Group x Valence x Time interaction of the first and last half of the Sexual imagery versus Neutral outcome phase. CSB subjects had greater decrease in dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) activity over time (X Y Z = 0 18 36, Cluster size=391, whole brain FWE p=0.02) and right inferior temporal cortex (X Y Z = 54 -36 -4, Cluster size =184, whole brain FWE p=0.04) to Sexual versus Neutral outcomes compared to HVs (Figure 3B).

We then extracted the trial-by-trial beta values focusing on the dACC for Sexual, Monetary and Neutral outcomes. We compared the slopes (i.e., degree of habituation) and intercept points (i.e., activity to initial exposure) comparing Sexual – Neutral and Monetary – Neutral outcomes (Figures 3C). For the slope, there was a main effect of Valence (F(1,36)=6.310, p=0.017) and a Group-by-Valence interaction (F(1,36)=6.288, p=0.017). As there was an interaction effect, we conducted post-hoc analyses: there was a steeper decrease in dACC slope to Sexual outcomes in CSB compared to HVs (F=4.159, p=0.049) with no differences to Monetary outcomes (F=0.552, p=0.463). There was no main effect of Group (F(1,36)=2.135, p=0.153). For the intercept value, there was a main effect of Valence (F(1,36)=11.527, p=0.002) but no main effect of Group (F(1,36)=0.913, p=0.346) or interaction effect (F(1,36)=2.067, p=0.159). There were no correlations between the conditioning and outcome phases.

Extinction: outcome

We assessed the omission of outcome during the Extinction phase across all trials. Here we had a very specific prediction that ventral striatal activity was decreased during outcome omission to previously rewarding outcomes consistent with a negative prediction error. There was an effect of Valence in which lower right ventral striatal activity was observed to the lack of Sexual and Monetary outcomes compared to Neutral outcomes (X Y Z 2 8 -10, Z=3.59, SVC FWE corrected p=0.036) (Figure 5A). There were no Group or interaction effects. There were no significant differences between Sexual and Monetary outcomes.

Thumbnail image of Figure 5. Opens large image

Figure 5

Extinction and functional connectivity. A. Omission of outcome during extinction. Decreased right ventral striatal activity in both groups for unexpected omission of Sexual and Monetary outcomes versus Neutral outcomes during extinction (Valence effect: p<0.05). B. Functional connectivity with repeated exposure. Psychophysiological interaction of individuals with compulsive sexual behaviours (CSB) and healthy volunteers (HV) individuals comparing early versus late exposure of sexual outcomes with a dorsal cingulate seed showing functional connectivity with right ventral striatum (left) and bilateral hippocampus (right). *p<0.05; **p<0.005.

 

Functional connectivity of dorsal cingulate

Functional connectivity using psychophysiological interaction of the dACC contrasting early versus late exposure (the first 2 trials versus the last 2 trials) of the Sexual outcomes was also assessed. There was greater functional connectivity in the HVs compared to CSB subjects in the early compared to late trials between the dACC and right ventral striatum (X Y Z = 18 20 -8 mm, Z=3.11, SVC FWE-corrected p=0.027) and bilateral hippocampus (right: X Y Z = 32 -34 -8, Z=3.68, SVC FWE-corrected p=0.003; left: X Y Z = -26 -38 04, Z=3.65 SVC FWE-corrected p=0.003) (Figure 5B). Thus CSB subjects had greater functional connectivity between these regions late in exposure whereas healthy volunteers had greater functional connectivity early in exposure.

Relationship between behavioral and imaging results

We investigated if there was a relationship between dACC habituation (slope) of the Sexual outcome with novelty preference for Sex – Control2 using Pearson correlation. Across subjects, the novelty preference for Sexual versus Control2 images was negatively correlated with the slope for sexual images (r=-0.404, p=0.037). Thus, greater sexual novelty preference was correlated with a more negative slope or greater dACC habituation.

Discussion

We show that CSB subjects had greater choice preference for novel sexual images and for cues conditioned to both sexual and monetary stimuli compared to healthy volunteers. CSB subjects also had greater habituation of dACC activity to repeated sexual versus monetary images. Across all subjects, the degree of dACC habituation to sexual stimuli was associated with greater novelty preference for sexual images. This study builds on our previous findings of enhanced attentional bias (Mechelmans, Irvine, 2014) and cue reactivity (Voon, Mole, 2014) towards explicit sexual cues in CSB implicating a dACC-(ventral striatal)-amygdalar network. Here, we show that early attentional bias to sexual cues assessed using a dot-probe task was associated with greater approach behaviors towards cues conditioned to sexual images but not novelty preference. Thus, the findings indicate that possible mechanisms underlying early attentional bias to sexual cues observed in CSB subjects are closely aligned with cue-conditioning and enhanced approach behaviors towards sexual conditioned cues. Although novelty preference to sexual stimuli is also enhanced in CSB subjects, this behavior is unrelated to the observation of early attentional bias. This observation contrasts with a previous study in healthy volunteers, which shows a relation between attentional bias towards sexual stimuli and sexual sensation-seeking (Kagerer, Wehrum, 2014). This may be explained by a greater influence of cue-conditioning in individuals with pathology.

Preference for stimuli conditioned to sexual or monetary rewards

This enhanced preference for conditioned stimuli across both forms of reward (sexual and monetary rewards) suggests either that CSB subjects have greater reward sensitivity or generalization and transfer of the effects of conditioning between similar stimuli (Mazur, 2002). This phenomenon is in line with behavioral cross-sensitization observed in rodent studies between stimulants and incentive properties of natural rewards, such as sex proposed to involve dopaminergic mechanisms (Fiorino and Phillips, 1999, Frohmader et al., 2011). Applying such investigative approaches to individuals with other non-substance addictions like gambling disorder is warranted as initial studies have suggested differential neural activation patterns to monetary and sexual rewards in this population (Sescousse et al., 2013).

Although we have used the term habituation to explain the decrease in activity to repeated sexual stimuli, as this is assessed in the context of cue-conditioning during which cues are paired with the outcomes, one relevant process may be the effect of associative learning underlying the cue-conditioning in which dopaminergic activity to the unexpected reward shifts towards the cue with conditioning and thus decreases over time such that activity as the reward outcome becomes expected will decrease over time (Schultz, 1998). However, as (i) we randomized the 5 sexual images repeated 8 times across the two stimuli conditioned to sexual rewards; (ii) we did not observe any relationship between the decrease in dACC activity to repeated sexual stimuli with conditioning preference but did observe a relationship with sexual novelty preference, (iii) there were no group differences in imaging outcomes to the conditioned cues and no evidence of enhanced conditioning specific to sexual rewards, and (iv) CSB subjects had a preference for both stimuli conditioned to sexual and monetary rewards, we have suggested the process may be consistent with a habituation effect.

We further show that the unexpected lack of sexual or monetary reward is associated with lower right ventral-striatal activity across all subjects. Converging primate and human studies suggest that phasic dopamine encodes a prediction error with a positive prediction error to unexpected reward and a negative prediction error to unexpected lack of reward (Pessiglione et al., 2006, Schultz, 1998). This decrease in ventral-striatal activity to the unexpected lack of sexual or monetary rewards may be consistent with a negative prediction error, suggesting similar mechanisms underlying secondary and primary rewards, both of which may elicit conditioned preferences.

Preference for novel sexual stimuli and dorsal cingulate habituation

Novelty-seeking and sensation-seeking are associated with disorders of addiction across a range of substances including tobacco, alcohol and drug use (Djamshidian et al., 2011, Kreek et al., 2005, Wills et al., 1994). Preclinical studies demonstrate a role for novelty preference as a risk factor for drug-seeking behaviors (Beckmann et al., 2011, Belin, Berson, 2011), and similarly, higher sensation-seeking is a predictor of subsequent binge drinking in adolescents but not of eating disorders (Conrod, O’Leary-Barrett, 2013). Likewise, in Parkinson’s patients who develop impulse control behaviors on dopamine agonists, novelty seeking is associated with externalizing rewards such as pathological gambling and compulsive shopping but not natural rewards such as binge eating or CSB (Voon et al., 2011). In our current study, there were no differences in sensation-seeking scores between CSB subjects and HVs, suggesting a role for novelty preference specific to the reward but not generalized novelty- or sensation-seeking. Our findings may be particularly relevant in the context of online explicit stimuli, which potentially provides an endless source of novelty, and may indeed differ from drug addiction in which ongoing novelty may be less of an issue.

We further show that CSB subjects had more rapid habituation of the dACC to repeated sexual images relative to monetary images. This finding may reflect repeated exposure to explicit online stimuli, similar to the observation of decreased putaminal activity to excessive use of online explicit materials in healthy male volunteers (Kuhn and Gallinat, 2014). Across all subjects, novelty preference to repeated sexual images was predicted by a greater habituation of dACC activity to sexual outcomes. We have recently shown enhanced dACC activity in CSB subjects to explicit videos (Voon, Mole, 2014), and the dACC has been implicated in both drug-cue reactivity and craving (Kuhn and Gallinat, 2011). In this previous study, the videos were sexually explicit and may have acted as conditioned cues and were shown infrequently, and hence they may have been less likely to be associated with habituation. Habituation was also not specifically assessed. The dACC receives extensive projections from midbrain dopaminergic neurons and is well-localized with multiple cortical connections to influence action selection. The dACC plays a role in detecting and planning appropriate behavioral responses to salient events during continuous behavioral adaptation (Sheth et al., 2012). Alternatively, the dACC is also implicated in reward-motivated behaviors, particularly prediction about future rewards and reward-prediction errors (Bush et al., 2002, Rushworth and Behrens, 2008). Thus, the role of the dACC may be related to effects of salience or unexpected reward.

The assessment of novelty involves the comparison of incoming information with stored memory mediated by a polysynaptic hippocampal-(ventral striatal)-(ventral tegmental area) loop suggested to combine information on novelty, salience and goals (Lisman and Grace, 2005). Our observation of enhanced dACC-(ventral striatal)-hippocampal connectivity in CSB subjects with repeated exposure to sexual outcomes despite a decrease in dACC activity may represent a network involved in the aberrant encoding of hippocampal-dependent memory to repeated sexual imagery.

The study has important strengths. This is the first investigation into the neural underpinnings of novelty and cue-conditioning processes in CSB, with the investigation permitting insights into specific aspects of the behavioral and neural correlates of these processes. We show experimentally what is observed clinically that CSB is characterized by novelty-seeking, conditioning and habituation to sexual stimuli in males. However, some limitations should also be acknowledged. First, the study involved solely young heterosexual men. Although this feature can be seen as a strength by limiting heterogeneity, it can also be a limitation with respect to generalization to women, other age groups and individuals with other sexual orientations. Second, CSB participants were generally more anxious, depressed, and impulsive and showed a trend for more obsessive-compulsive features. Although we did not find a direct effect of these variables in our results, we cannot exclude the possibility that they may have influenced the findings. Third, there were no significant differences in the imaging analyses of conditioning, extinction cues, extinction outcome. Our imaging findings support the behavioral processes of sexual novelty but we did not observe imaging findings to support the findings of conditioning preferences. Larger samples, more explicit images, or facilitating consolidation with subsequent testing represent important considerations for future studies that may generate different results. Fourth, this study used imagery that could be perceived as erotic rather than sexually explicit. Further studies using sexually explicit materials may differentiate between conditioning effects to monetary and sexually explicit stimuli.

We highlight the role of enhanced preference for sexual novelty and a generalized enhancement of conditioning to rewards in CSB subjects involving dACC habituation. These findings extend our recent observations that CSB subjects have greater sexual cue reactivity in a network involving the dACC, ventral striatum and amygdala (Voon, Mole, 2014) and enhanced attentional bias to sexually explicit cues (Mechelmans, Irvine, 2014). We emphasize a role for cue-conditioning dissociable from novelty preference underlying this observation of enhanced early attentional bias for sexual cues. These findings have potential wider relevance as the Internet provides a vast source of novel and potentially rewarding stimuli, particularly with respect to sexually explicit material. Future studies should examine the extent to which the current findings might relate to clinically relevant measures related to CSB, both cross-sectionally and prospectively. These findings suggest a role for targeting dissociable cognitive processes in the therapeutic management of CSB.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: VV. Performed the experiments: PB, SM and VV. Analyzed the data: PB, LSM, SM, VV. Wrote the paper: PB, NAH, MNP and VV.

Role of Funding Source

PB is supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (individual fellowship: SFRH/BD/33889/2009). Dr. Voon is a Wellcome Trust Intermediate Fellow and the study was funded by the Wellcome Trust (WT093705/Z/10/Z). Channel 4 was involved in assisting with recruitment by placing ethics-approved advertisements for the study on Internet sites. The advertisements provided contact details of the study researchers for interested participants.

Conflicts of interest

The material is original research, has not been previously published and has not been submitted for publication elsewhere. Authors PB, LM, SM, NH, MNP and VV declare no competing financial interests.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the participants who took part in the study and the staff at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre. We also acknowledge Channel 4 for helping with recruitment and the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology and the Wellcome Trust for funding.

References

  1. Bardo, M.T., Donohew, R.L., and Harrington, N.G. Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behav Brain Res. 1996; 77: 23–43
  2. Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1961; 4: 561–571
  3. View in Article 
  4. | CrossRef
  5. | PubMed
  6. View in Article 
  7. | CrossRef
  8. | PubMed
  9. | Scopus (32)
  10. View in Article 
  11. | CrossRef
  12. | PubMed
  13. | Scopus (68)
  14. View in Article 
  15. | CrossRef
  16. | PubMed
  17. | Scopus (7)
  18. View in Article 
  19. | Abstract
  20. | Full Text
  21. | Full Text PDF
  22. | PubMed
  23. | Scopus (158)
  24. View in Article 
  25. | PubMed
  26. View in Article 
  27. | CrossRef
  28. | PubMed
  29. | Scopus (537)
  30. Beckmann, J.S., Marusich, J.A., Gipson, C.D., and Bardo, M.T. Novelty seeking, incentive salience and acquisition of cocaine self-administration in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2011; 216: 159–165
  31. View in Article 
  32. | PubMed
  33. View in Article 
  34. | CrossRef
  35. | PubMed
  36. | Scopus (40)
  37. View in Article 
  38. | CrossRef
  39. | PubMed
  40. | Scopus (184)
  41. View in Article 
  42. | CrossRef
  43. | PubMed
  44. | Scopus (22)
  45. View in Article 
  46. | CrossRef
  47. | PubMed
  48. | Scopus (56)
  49. View in Article 
  50. | PubMed
  51. View in Article 
  52. | CrossRef
  53. | PubMed
  54. | Scopus (7)
  55. View in Article 
  56. | CrossRef
  57. | PubMed
  58. | Scopus (5)
  59. View in Article 
  60. | CrossRef
  61. | PubMed
  62. | Scopus (176)
  63. View in Article 
  64. | CrossRef
  65. | PubMed
  66. | Scopus (141)
  67. View in Article 
  68. | CrossRef
  69. | PubMed
  70. | Scopus (186)
  71. View in Article 
  72. | CrossRef
  73. | PubMed
  74. View in Article 
  75. | CrossRef
  76. | PubMed
  77. | Scopus (44)
  78. View in Article 
  79. | CrossRef
  80. | PubMed
  81. | Scopus (533)
  82. View in Article 
  83. | CrossRef
  84. | PubMed
  85. | Scopus (17)
  86. View in Article 
  87. | CrossRef
  88. | PubMed
  89. | Scopus (447)
  90. View in Article 
  91. | CrossRef
  92. | PubMed
  93. | Scopus (63)
  94. View in Article 
  95. View in Article 
  96. | Abstract
  97. | Full Text
  98. | Full Text PDF
  99. | PubMed
  100. | Scopus (708)
  101. Belin, D., Berson, N., Balado, E., Piazza, P.V., and Deroche-Gamonet, V. High-novelty-preference rats are predisposed to compulsive cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011; 36: 569–579
  102. View in Article 
  103. | CrossRef
  104. | PubMed
  105. | Scopus (2)
  106. View in Article 
  107. | CrossRef
  108. | PubMed
  109. | Scopus (94)
  110. Belin, D. and Deroche-Gamonet, V. Responses to novelty and vulnerability to cocaine addiction: contribution of a multi-symptomatic animal model. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. 2012; 2
  111. View in Article 
  112. | PubMed
  113. View in Article 
  114. | PubMed
  115. View in Article 
  116. | CrossRef
  117. | PubMed
  118. | Scopus (535)
  119. View in Article 
  120. | CrossRef
  121. | PubMed
  122. | Scopus (180)
  123. View in Article 
  124. | CrossRef
  125. | PubMed
  126. | Scopus (43)
  127. View in Article 
  128. | CrossRef
  129. | PubMed
  130. | Scopus (323)
  131. View in Article 
  132. | CrossRef
  133. | PubMed
  134. | Scopus (23)
  135. View in Article 
  136. | Abstract
  137. | Full Text
  138. | Full Text PDF
  139. | PubMed
  140. | Scopus (40)
  141. View in Article 
  142. | CrossRef
  143. | PubMed
  144. | Scopus (330)
  145. View in Article 
  146. | Abstract
  147. | Full Text
  148. | Full Text PDF
  149. | PubMed
  150. | Scopus (241)
  151. View in Article 
  152. | CrossRef
  153. | PubMed
  154. View in Article 
  155. | PubMed
  156. View in Article 
  157. | CrossRef
  158. | PubMed
  159. | Scopus (3155)
  160. View in Article 
  161. | CrossRef
  162. | PubMed
  163. | Scopus (23)
  164. View in Article 
  165. | PubMed
  166. View in Article 
  167. | CrossRef
  168. | PubMed
  169. | Scopus (91)
  170. Bunzeck, N. and Duzel, E. Absolute coding of stimulus novelty in the human substantia nigra/VTA. Neuron. 2006; 51: 369–379
  171. View in Article 
  172. | CrossRef
  173. | PubMed
  174. | Scopus (49)
  175. View in Article 
  176. | PubMed
  177. View in Article 
  178. | CrossRef
  179. | PubMed
  180. | Scopus (8)
  181. View in Article 
  182. | CrossRef
  183. | PubMed
  184. | Scopus (5)
  185. View in Article 
  186. | CrossRef
  187. | PubMed
  188. | Scopus (119)
  189. View in Article 
  190. | Abstract
  191. | Full Text
  192. | Full Text PDF
  193. | PubMed
  194. | Scopus (8)
  195. View in Article 
  196. | Abstract
  197. | Full Text
  198. | Full Text PDF
  199. | PubMed
  200. View in Article 
  201. | CrossRef
  202. | Scopus (984)
  203. View in Article 
  204. | CrossRef
  205. | PubMed
  206. | Scopus (164)
  207. View in Article 
  208. | CrossRef
  209. | PubMed
  210. | Scopus (255)
  211. View in Article 
  212. | CrossRef
  213. | PubMed
  214. | Scopus (316)
  215. View in Article 
  216. | CrossRef
  217. | Scopus (155)
  218. Bunzeck, N., Guitart-Masip, M., Dolan, R.J., and Duzel, E. Pharmacological Dissociation of Novelty Responses in the Human Brain. Cereb Cortex. 2013;
  219. Bush, G., Vogt, B.A., Holmes, J., Dale, A.M., Greve, D., Jenike, M.A. et al. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002; 99: 523–528
  220. Carnes P, Delmonico DL, Griffin E. In the Shadows of the Net: Breaking Free from Compulsive Online Sexual Behaviour. 2nd ed. Center City, Minnesota: Hazelden 2001.
  221. Childress, A.R., Hole, A.V., Ehrman, R.N., Robbins, S.J., McLellan, A.T., and O’Brien, C.P. Cue reactivity and cue reactivity interventions in drug dependence. NIDA research monograph. 1993; 137: 73–95
  222. Conrod, P.J., O’Leary-Barrett, M., Newton, N., Topper, L., Castellanos-Ryan, N., Mackie, C. et al. Effectiveness of a selective, personality-targeted prevention program for adolescent alcohol use and misuse: a cluster randomized controlled trial. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013; 70: 334–342
  223. Cox, W.M., Fadardi, J.S., and Pothos, E.M. The addiction-stroop test: Theoretical considerations and procedural recommendations. Psychological bulletin. 2006; 132: 443–476
  224. Djamshidian, A., O’Sullivan, S.S., Wittmann, B.C., Lees, A.J., and Averbeck, B.B. Novelty seeking behaviour in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2011; 49: 2483–2488
  225. Ersche, K.D., Bullmore, E.T., Craig, K.J., Shabbir, S.S., Abbott, S., Muller, U. et al. Influence of compulsivity of drug abuse on dopaminergic modulation of attentional bias in stimulant dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010; 67: 632–644
  226. Fiorino, D.F. and Phillips, A.G. Facilitation of sexual behavior and enhanced dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens of male rats after D-amphetamine-induced behavioral sensitization. J Neurosci. 1999; 19: 456–463
  227. Frohmader, K.S., Lehman, M.N., Laviolette, S.R., and Coolen, L.M. Concurrent exposure to methamphetamine and sexual behavior enhances subsequent drug reward and causes compulsive sexual behavior in male rats. J Neurosci. 2011; 31: 16473–16482
  228. Grant, J.E., Atmaca, M., Fineberg, N.A., Fontenelle, L.F., Matsunaga, H., Janardhan Reddy, Y.C. et al. Impulse control disorders and “behavioural addictions” in the ICD-11. World Psychiatry. 2014; 13: 125–127
  229. Grant, J.E., Levine, L., Kim, D., and Potenza, M.N. Impulse control disorders in adult psychiatric inpatients. Am J Psychiatry. 2005; 162: 2184–2188
  230. Jansen, A. A learning model of binge eating: cue reactivity and cue exposure. Behav Res Ther. 1998; 36: 257–272
  231. Kafka, M.P. Hypersexual disorder: a proposed diagnosis for DSM-V. Archives of sexual behavior. 2010; 39: 377–400
  232. Kagerer, S., Wehrum, S., Klucken, T., Walter, B., Vaitl, D., and Stark, R. Sex attracts: investigating individual differences in attentional bias to sexual stimuli. PloS one. 2014; 9: e107795
  233. Klucken, T., Schweckendiek, J., Merz, C.J., Tabbert, K., Walter BAtSGSoNRTVSS, Kagerer, S. et al. Neural activations of the acquisition of conditioned sexual arousal: effects of contingency awareness and sex. J Sex Med. 2009; 6: 3071–3085
  234. Knight, R. Contribution of human hippocampal region to novelty detection. Nature. 1996; 383: 256–259
  235. Koukounas, E. and Over, R. Changes in the magnitude of the eyeblink startle response during habituation of sexual arousal. Behav Res Ther. 2000; 38: 573–584
  236. Kreek, M.J., Nielsen, D.A., Butelman, E.R., and LaForge, K.S. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8: 1450–1457
  237. Kuhn, S. and Gallinat, J. Common biology of craving across legal and illegal drugs – a quantitative meta-analysis of cue-reactivity brain response. Eur J Neurosci. 2011; 33: 1318–1326
  238. Kuhn, S. and Gallinat, J. Brain Structure and Functional Connectivity Associated With Pornography Consumption: The Brain on Porn. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;
  239. Lisman, J.E. and Grace, A.A. The hippocampal-VTA loop: controlling the entry of information into long-term memory. Neuron. 2005; 46: 703–713
  240. Mazur JE. Learning and behavior. 5th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 2002.
  241. Mechelmans, D.J., Irvine, M., Banca, P., Porter, L., Mitchell, S., Mole, T.B. et al. Enhanced attentional bias towards sexually explicit cues in individuals with and without compulsive sexual behaviours. PloS one. 2014; 9: e105476
  242. Meerkerk, G.J., Van Den Eijnden, R.J., and Garretsen, H.F. Predicting compulsive Internet use: it’s all about sex!. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2006; 9: 95–103
  243. Nelson HE. National Adult Reading Test (NART): Test Manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson; 1982.
  244. Odlaug, B.L. and Grant, J.E. Impulse-control disorders in a college sample: results from the self-administered Minnesota Impulse Disorders Interview (MIDI). Primary care companion to the Journal of clinical psychiatry. 2010; 12
  245. Odlaug, B.L., Lust, K., Schreiber, L.R., Christenson, G., Derbyshire, K., Harvanko, A. et al. Compulsive sexual behavior in young adults. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2013; 25: 193–200
  246. Pessiglione, M., Seymour, B., Flandin, G., Dolan, R.J., and Frith, C.D. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006; 442: 1042–1045
  247. Pfaus, J.G., Kippin, T.E., and Centeno, S. Conditioning and sexual behavior: a review. Hormones and behavior. 2001; 40: 291–321
  248. Prause, N., Janssen, E., and Hetrick, W.P. Attention and emotional responses to sexual stimuli and their relationship to sexual desire. Archives of sexual behavior. 2008; 37: 934–949
  249. Ranganath, C. and Rainer, G. Neural mechanisms for detecting and remembering novel events. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2003; 4: 193–202
  250. Redolat, R., Perez-Martinez, A., Carrasco, M.C., and Mesa, P. Individual differences in novelty-seeking and behavioral responses to nicotine: a review of animal studies. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. 2009; 2: 230–242
  251. Reid, R.C., Carpenter, B.N., Hook, J.N., Garos, S., Manning, J.C., Gilliland, R. et al. Report of findings in a DSM-5 field trial for hypersexual disorder. J Sex Med. 2012; 9: 2868–2877
  252. Rushworth, M.F. and Behrens, T.E. Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2008; 11: 389–397
  253. Rushworth, M.F., Noonan, M.P., Boorman, E.D., Walton, M.E., and Behrens, T.E. Frontal cortex and reward-guided learning and decision-making. Neuron. 2011; 70: 1054–1069
  254. Saunders, J.B., Aasland, O.G., Babor, T.F., de la Fuente, J.R., and M, G. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on Early Detection of Persons with Harmful Alcohol Consumption–II. Addiction. 1993; 88: 791–804
  255. Schultz, W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998; 80: 1–27
  256. Schultz, W., Dayan, P., and Montague, P.R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997; 275: 1593–1599
  257. Sescousse, G., Barbalat, G., Domenech, P., and Dreher, J.C. Imbalance in the sensitivity to different types of rewards in pathological gambling. Brain. 2013; 136: 2527–2538
  258. Sheehan, D.V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K.H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E. et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998; 59: 22–33 (quiz 4–57)
  259. Sheth, S.A., Mian, M.K., Patel, S.R., Asaad, W.F., Williams, Z.M., Dougherty, D.D. et al. Human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex neurons mediate ongoing behavioural adaptation. Nature. 2012; 488: 218–221
  260. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, P.R. V, Jacobs GA. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.; 1983.
  261. Toates, F. An integrative theoretical framework for understanding sexual motivation, arousal, and behavior. J Sex Res. 2009; 46: 168–193
  262. Toussaint, I. and Pitchot, W. Hypersexual disorder will not be included in the DSM V: a contextual analysis. Rev Med Liege. 2013; 68: 348–353
  263. van Hemel-Ruiter, M.E., de Jong, P.J., Oldehinkel, A.J., and Ostafin, B.D. Reward-related attentional biases and adolescent substance use: the TRAILS study. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013; 27: 142–150
  264. Voon, V., Mole, T.B., Banca, P., Porter, L., Morris, L., Mitchell, S. et al. Neural correlates of sexual cue reactivity in individuals with and without compulsive sexual behaviours. PloS one. 2014; 9: e102419
  265. Voon, V., Sohr, M., Lang, A.E., Potenza, M.N., Siderowf, A.D., Whetteckey, J. et al. Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a multicenter case–control study. Ann Neurol. 2011; 69: 986–996
  266. Wehrum, S., Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Walter, B., Hermann, A., Vaitl, D. et al. Gender commonalities and differences in the neural processing of visual sexual stimuli. J Sex Med. 2013; 10: 1328–1342
  267. Wehrum-Osinsky, S., Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Walter, B., Hermann, A., and Stark, R. At the second glance: stability of neural responses toward visual sexual stimuli. J Sex Med. 2014; 11: 2720–2737
  268. Whiteside, S.P. and Lynam, D.R. The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001; 30: 669–689
  269. Wiers, R.W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E.S., and Lindenmeyer, J. Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological science. 2011; 22: 490–497
  270. Williams, S.M. and Goldman-Rakic, P.S. Widespread origin of the primate mesofrontal dopamine system. Cereb Cortex. 1998; 8: 321–345
  271. Wills, T.A., Vaccaro, D., and McNamara, G. Novelty seeking, risk taking, and related constructs as predictors of adolescent substance use: an application of Cloninger’s theory. J Subst Abuse. 1994; 6: 1–20
  272. Yiend, J. The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion. 2010; 24: 3–47