Non-substance addictive behaviors in the context of DSM-5 (2014)

Marc N. Potenza

DOI: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.004

Keywords – Gambling; Internet use; Video-gaming; Eating/Food; Sex; Shopping

The term addiction holds significance for patients, clinicians, researchers, policy makers and many other groups of people. Thus, how the term addiction is used (and to whom it may apply) has been debated. The term “addiction” originated in Roman times and initially was not linked to substance use (Maddux & Desmond, 2000). However, over time, the term addiction became increasingly linked to substance use such that around the time of DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987), the committee working on substance-related disorders believed that addiction was defined by compulsive drug use (O’Brien, Volkow, & Li, 2006). Nonetheless, the term was largely omitted from the DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), in part given the charged nature of the term (perceived as pejorative or stigmatizing) and associated complexities (O’Brien et al., 2006). However, in DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), the category of “Substance-related and Addictive Disorders” replaces the “Substance-related Disorders” category found in DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). This difference represents an important change in multiple ways, as described below.

The DSM-5 process involved multiple research workgroups that convened prior to the operation of the committees. Two research workgroups, one focusing on substance-related/addictive disorders and the other on obsessive–compulsive-spectrum disorders, considered how pathological gambling might be considered from a classification perspective, with manuscripts emanating from each workgroup (Petry, 2006, Potenza et al., 2009 and Potenza, 2006). An important aspect of the DSM process involved using extant data to direct decisions on how best to define and classify conditions. Since the publication of DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), a considerable amount of research had been conducted into pathological gambling, substance-use disorders, and other potentially or theoretically related conditions. On the basis of research demonstrating clinical, phenomenological, genetic, neurobiological and other similarities between gambling and substance-use disorders, a decision was made to group pathological gambling (now gambling disorder) with substance-related disorders in DSM-5.

Although gambling disorder is presently the only condition in the subsection of “Non-substance-related disorders” in the category of “Substance-related and Addictive Disorders”, other conditions were considered. Notably, Internet gaming disorder has been included in the DSM-5 as a condition requiring further study (Petry & O’Brien, 2013). The inclusion of specific diagnostic criteria for this disorder should help advance clinical and research efforts into its prevalence and impact, and thus its inclusion in DSM-5 represents a significant advance. However, Internet gaming may represent just one facet of problematic use of the Internet and the potential impact of other Internet-related behaviors (e.g., social networking, shopping, pornography viewing, gambling) warrants consideration (Yau, Crowley, Mayes, & Potenza, 2012). Additionally, problematic use of non-Internet-based forms of technology (e.g., non-Internet video-gaming, television viewing) warrants consideration as being potentially addictive (Sussman and Moran, 2013 and Yau et al., 2012). Consistent with the idea that problems with video-gaming might not exist solely within problems with Internet use, studies have found differences relating to risky/problematic Internet use versus risky/problematic video-gaming in samples of adolescents (Desai et al., 2010 and Liu et al., 2011) and adults (Yau, Potenza, & White, 2013). However, the associations between negative measures of health and functioning and these problematic uses of technology in these studies, as well as the proportions of individuals acknowledging risky or problematic levels of these behaviors, highlight the importance of additional research of these behaviors and the need for increased awareness of and treatments and prevention strategies for these behaviors.

Although not included in the DSM-5, several other non-substance or behavioral addictions were considered. Specifically, the topics of addictions relating to sex, exercise and shopping were discussed but not included as it was concluded that (p. 481 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013)), “at this time there is insufficient peer-reviewed evidence to establish the diagnostic criteria and course descriptions needed to identify these behaviors as mental health disorders.” Nonetheless, as many individuals seek help for these conditions, it is important to gather information on these behaviors and their clinical correlates in order to continue to improve public health initiatives.

During this millennium, significant advances have already been made in the area of non-substance addictions, although much more progress is needed. At the millennial onset, Constance Holden questioned whether behavioral addictions existed, and at the close of its first decade, she communicated on the proposal to group gambling with substance-use disorders in DSM-5 (Holden, 2001 and Holden, 2010). Debate on how best to define which disorders constitute addictions remains. For example, considerable debate exists regarding the extent to which food may be addictive and whether food addiction might be an important entity (Avena et al., 2012, Ziauddeen et al., 2012a and Ziauddeen et al., 2012b). Regardless of the outcome of the debate, it seems premature to dismiss a food-addiction entity, particularly given potential implications for prevention, treatment and policy (Gearhardt, Grilo, DiLeone, Brownell, & Potenza, 2011). Given the current obesity epidemic, an improved understanding of how a food-addiction model might relate to obesity or other eating-related conditions may help lead to improved prevention, treatment and policy initiatives (Potenza, in press). As neurobiological similarities represented one important consideration in the decision to classify together gambling and substance-use disorders in DSM-5, it is important to note that similar neurobiological features have been reported between gambling, substance-use and eating disorders. For example, relatively diminished ventral striatal activation during the anticipatory phase of reward processing has been observed in pathological gambling, alcohol dependence, tobacco smoking and binge-eating disorder (Balodis et al., 2012, Balodis et al., 2013, Beck et al., 2009, Choi et al., 2012, Peters et al., 2011 and Wrase et al., 2007), the last of which shows particularly strong similarities with and high rates of food addiction (Gearhardt, White, & Potenza, 2011). The possibility that diminished ventral striatal activity during anticipatory phases of reward processing might represent an important biomarker for addictive processes warrants further examination.

As with the concept of food addiction, considerable debate exists regarding whether other behaviors (e.g., excessive/compulsive sex (Kor, Fogel, Reid, & Potenza, 2013); see also http://healthland.time.com/2013/07/23/my-name-is-john-and-i-am-a-sex-addict-or-maybe-not/?iid=hl-main-lead) might represent addictions. Interestingly, many of these behaviors (excessive/compulsive gambling, sex, eating, and shopping) have been associated with Parkinson’s disease and its treatment (Weintraub et al., 2010), further suggesting a common biological pathway across these conditions. Thus, while the reclassification of gambling disorder in DSM-5 represents an important development, it is anticipated that additional changes may occur in the future as more knowledge is attained. Gathering information to provide empirical support for such changes is critical in this process, and the resulting understanding should provide a foundation for generating an improved public health through better policy, prevention and treatment approaches.

Funding and disclosure

This research was funded in part by NIH grants from NIDA (P20 DA027844), NIAAA (RL1 AA017539), the Connecticut State Department of Mental Health and Addictions Services, the Connecticut Mental Health Center, an unrestricted gift from Mohegan Sun and a Center of Excellence in Gambling Research from the National Center for Responsible Gaming. The funding agencies did not provide input or comment on the content of the manuscript, and the content of the manuscript reflects the contributions and thoughts of the author and not necessarily the views of the funding agencies. Dr. Potenza has consulted for Lundbeck and Ironwood pharmaceuticals; has had financial interests in Somaxon pharmaceuticals; received research support from Mohegan Sun Casino, Psyadon Pharmaceuticals, the National Center for Responsible Gambling, the National Institutes of Health (NIH), Veterans Administration; has participated in surveys, mailings, or telephone consultations related to drug addiction, impulse-control disorders, or other health topics; has consulted for gambling, legal and governmental entities on issues related to addictions or impulse-control disorders; has provided clinical care in the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services Problem Gambling Services Program; has performed grant reviews for the NIH and other agencies; has guest edited journal sections; has given academic lectures in grand rounds, Continuing Medical Education events, and other clinical or scientific venues; and has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts.

References

American Psychiatric Association

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(3rd — Revised ed.)American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC (1987)

Petry, 2006

N.M. Petry

Should the scope of addictive behaviors be broadened to include pathological gambling?

Addiction, 101 (s1) (2006), pp. 152–160

|

 | 

Peters et al., 2011

J. Peters, U. Bromberg, S. Schneider, S. Brassen, M. Menz, T. Banaschewski, et al.

Lower ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation in adolescent smokers

American Journal of Psychiatry, 168 (2011), pp. 540–549

|

 | 

O’Brien et al., 2006

C.P. O’Brien, N. Volkow, T.K. Li

What’s in a word? Addiction versus dependence in DSM-V

American Journal of Psychiatry, 163 (2006), pp. 764–765

|

 | 

Maddux and Desmond, 2000

J.F. Maddux, D.P. Desmond

Addiction or dependence?

Addiction, 95 (2000), pp. 661–665

|

 | 

Liu et al., 2011

T.C. Liu, R.A. Desai, S. Krishnan-Sarin, D.A. Cavallo, M.N. Potenza

Problematic Internet use and health in adolescents: Data from a high school survey in Connecticut

Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 72 (2011), pp. 836–845

|

 | 

Kor et al., 2013

A. Kor, Y. Fogel, R. Reid, M.N. Potenza

Should hypersexual disorder be classified as an addiction?

Sexual Addiction and Compulsivity, 20 (2013), pp. 27–47

 

Holden, 2010

C. Holden

Behavioral addictions debut in proposed DSM-V

Science, 327 (2010), p. 935

|

 | 

Holden, 2001

C. Holden

‘Behavioral’ addictions: Do they exist?

Science, 294 (2001), pp. 980–982

|

 | 

Gearhardt, White and Potenza, 2011

A.N. Gearhardt, M.A. White, M.N. Potenza

Binge eating disorder and food addiction

Current Drug Alcohol Rev, 4 (2011), pp. 201–207

 | 

Gearhardt, Grilo, DiLeone, Brownell and Potenza, 2011

A.N. Gearhardt, C.M. Grilo, R.J. DiLeone, K.D. Brownell, M.N. Potenza

Can food be addictive? Public health and policy implications

Addiction, 106 (2011), pp. 1208–1212

|

 | 

Desai et al., 2010

R.A. Desai, S. Krishnan-Sarin, D.A. Cavallo, M.N. Potenza

Video-gaming among high school students: Health correlates, gender differences and problematic gaming

Pediatrics, 126 (2010), pp. e1414–e1424

|

 | 

Choi et al., 2012

J.-S. Choi, Y.-C. Shin, W.H. Jung, J.H. Jang, D.-H. Kang, C.-H. Choi, et al.

Altered brain activity during reward anticipation in pathological gambling and obsessive–compulsive disorder

PLoS One, 7 (9) (2012), p. e45938

Beck et al., 2009

A. Beck, F. Schlagenhauf, T. Wustenberg, J. Hein, T. Kienast, T. Kahnt, et al.

Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics

Biological Psychiatry, 66 (2009), pp. 734–742

|

|

 | 

Balodis et al., 2013

I.M. Balodis, H. Kober, P.D. Worhunsky, M.A. White, M.C. Stevens, G.D. Pearlson, et al.

Monetary reward processing in obese individuals with and without binge eating disorder

Biological Psychiatry, 73 (2013), pp. 877–886

|

|

 | 

Balodis et al., 2012

I.M. Balodis, H. Kober, P.D. Worhunsky, M.C. Stevens, G.D. Pearlson, M.N. Potenza

Diminished fronto-striatal activity during processing of monetary rewards and losses in pathological gambling

Biological Psychiatry, 71 (2012), pp. 749–757

|

|

 | 

Avena et al., 2012

N.M. Avena, A.N. Gearhardt, M.S. Gold, G.J. Wang, M.N. Potenza

Tossing the baby out with the bathwater after a brief rinse? The potential downside of dismissing food addiction based on limited data

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13 (7) (2012), p. 514

|

 | 

American Psychiatric Association, 2013

American Psychiatric Association

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(5th ed.)American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC (2013)

 

American Psychiatric Association, 2000

American Psychiatric Association

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Fourth edition—test revision

American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC (2000)

 

American Psychiatric Association, 1994

American Psychiatric Association

Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(4th ed.)American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC (1994)

Petry and O’Brien, 2013

N.M. Petry, C.P. O’Brien

Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5

Addiction, 108 (2013), pp. 1186–1187

|

 | 

 

Ziauddeen et al., 2012b

H. Ziauddeen, I.S. Farooqi, P.C. Fletcher

Obesity and the brain: How convincing is the addiction model?

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13 (2012), pp. 279–286

 | 

Ziauddeen et al., 2012a

H. Ziauddeen, I.S. Farooqi, P.C. Fletcher

Food addiction: Is there a baby in the bathwater?

Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13 (2012), p. 514

|

 | 

Yau et al., 2013

Y.H.C. Yau, M.N. Potenza, M.A. White

Problematic Internet use, mental health and impulse control in an online survey of adults

Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2 (2) (2013), pp. 72–81

|

 | 

Yau et al., 2012

Y.H.C. Yau, M.J. Crowley, L.C. Mayes, M.N. Potenza

Are internet use and video-game playing addictive behaviors? Biological, clinical and public health implications for youths and adults

Minerva Psichiatrica, 53 (2012), pp. 153–170

 | 

Wrase et al., 2007

J. Wrase, F. Schlagenhauf, T. Kienast, T. Wustenberg, F. Bermpohl, T. Kahnt, et al.

Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics

Neuroimage, 35 (2) (2007), pp. 787–794

|

|

 | 

Weintraub et al., 2010

D. Weintraub, J. Koester, M.N. Potenza, A.D. Siderowf, M.A. Stacy, V. Voon, et al.

Impulse control disorders in Parkinson’s disease: A cross-sectional study of 3,090 patients

Archives of Neurology, 67 (2010), pp. 589–595

 | 

Sussman and Moran, 2013

S. Sussman, M.B. Moran

Hidden addiction: Television

Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2 (3) (2013), pp. 125–132

|

 | 

Potenza et al., 2009

M.N. Potenza, L.M. Koran, S. Pallanti

The relationship between impulse control disorders and obsessive–compulsive disorder: A current understanding and future research directions

Psychiatry Research, 170 (2009), pp. 22–31

|

|

 | 

Potenza, in press

M.N. Potenza

Obesity, food and addiction: Emerging neuroscience and clinical and public health implications

Neuropsychopharmacology (2013) (in press)

 

Potenza, 2006

M.N. Potenza

Should addictive disorders include non-substance-related conditions?

Addiction, 101 (s1) (2006), pp. 142–151

|

 |