Nucleus accumbens dopamine/glutamate interaction switches mode to generate desire versus dread: D1 alone for appetitive eating but D1 and D2 together for fear (2011)

J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC Mar 7, 2012.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC3174486

NIHMSID: NIHMS323168

The publisher’s final edited version of this article is available free at J Neurosci

See other articles in PMC that cite the published article.

Go to:

Abstract

The medial shell of nucleus accumbens (NAc) and its mesolimbic dopamine inputs mediate forms of fearful as well as of incentive motivation. For example, either appetitive and/or actively fearful behaviors are generated in a keyboard pattern by localized glutamate disruptions in NAc (via microinjection of AMPA receptor antagonist DNQX) at different anatomical locations along a rostrocaudal gradient within medial shell of rats. Rostral glutamate disruptions produce intense increases in eating, but more caudally placed disruptions produce increasingly fearful behaviors: distress vocalizations and escape attempts to human touch, and a spontaneous and directed antipredator response called defensive treading/burying. Local endogenous dopamine is required for either intense motivation to be generated by AMPA disruptions. Here we report that only endogenous local signaling at D1 dopamine receptors is needed for rostral generation of excessive eating, potentially implicating a direct output pathway contribution. By contrast, fear generation at caudal sites requires both D1 and D2 signaling simultaneously, potentially implicating an indirect output pathway contribution. Finally, when motivation valence generated by AMPA disruptions at intermediate sites was flipped by manipulating environmental ambience, from mostly appetitive in a comfortable home environment to mostly fearful in a stressful environment, the roles of local D1 versus D2 signaling in dopamine/glutamate interaction at microinjection sites also switched dynamically to match the motivation valence generated at the moment. Thus, NAc D1 and D2 receptors, and their associated neuronal circuits, play different and dynamic roles in enabling desire and dread to be generated by localized NAc glutamate disruptions in medial shell.

Introduction

Intense aberrant motivation is an important feature of psychopathological disorders, ranging from intense appetitive motivation in addiction and binge eating to more fearful paranoia in schizophrenia and anxiety disorders (Barch, 2005; Kalivas and Volkow, 2005; Howes and Kapur, 2009; Woodward et al., 2011). Both appetitive and fearful motivations involve interactions between dopamine and glutamate in overlapping mesocorticolimbic circuits that converge on nucleus accumbens (NAc) (Kelley et al., 2005; Faure et al., 2008; Meredith et al., 2008; Carlezon and Thomas, 2009; Kalivas et al., 2009; Humphries and Prescott, 2010).

NAc and dopamine-related circuits are best known for roles in appetitive motivation (Schultz, 2007; Wise, 2008), but are also implicated in some forms of aversive motivation related to fear, stress, disgust and pain (Levita et al., 2002; Salamone et al., 2005; Ventura et al., 2007; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2009; Zubieta and Stohler, 2009; Cabib and Puglisi-Allegra, 2011). Within medial shell of NAc, neuroanatomical coding plays an important role in determining appetitive versus fearful valence of intense motivations generated by glutamate disruptions.

Local AMPA blockade (e.g., by DNQX microinjection) produces intense eating and/or fearful reactions in an anatomical keyboard pattern along a rostrocaudal gradient (Reynolds and Berridge, 2001, 2003; Faure et al., 2008; Reynolds and Berridge, 2008). At rostral sites in medial shell, purely positive/appetitive behavior, such as intense eating, is produced by local glutamate disruptions (Maldonado-Irizarry et al., 1995; Kelley and Swanson, 1997). By contrast, as locations move caudally, disruptions generate progressively more fearful behaviors, including reactive distress vocalizations and escape dashes in response to touch, and spontaneous actively fearful behaviors such as an anti-predator response of defensive treading/burying, in which rodents use rapid forepaw movements to toss sand or bedding at a threatening stimulus (e.g., rattlesnake) (Coss and Owings, 1978; Treit et al., 1981; Reynolds and Berridge, 2001, 2003; Faure et al., 2008; Reynolds and Berridge, 2008). At intermediate sites in NAc shell, glutamate disruptions generate a mixture of both behaviors, and the dominant valence can be flexibly flipped between positive and negative by changing environmental ambience between familiar and stressful (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008).

We previously reported that endogenous dopamine activity was required locally for glutamate disruptions in NAc shell to generate feeding or fear (Faure et al., 2008). What remains unknown are the relative roles of D1-like versus D2-like dopamine receptors and their associated direct versus indirect output circuits in DNQX-generated motivations. Here we addressed these roles, and found that only D1 receptor stimulation, potentially involving the direct pathway to ventral tegmentum, was needed for glutamatergic disruptions to generate appetitive eating at rostral sites. In contrast, endogenous activity at both D1 and D2 receptors, potentially recruiting a stronger role of the indirect pathway to ventral pallidum and lateral hypothalamus, was needed for DNQX to generate fearful behavior at caudal sites. Further, we found that motivational valence trumped rostrocaudal location at flexible intermediate sites, which switched reversibly between an appetitive mode that required only D1 neurotransmission and a fearful mode that required simultaneous D1 and D2 neurotransmission.

Methods

Subjects

Male Sprague-Dawley rats (total n = 87; feeding and fear test groups, n = 51; Fos plume groups, n = 36), weighing 300 – 400 grams at surgery, were housed at ~21°C on a reverse 12:12 light:dark cycle. All rats had ad libitum access to both food and water. All of the following experimental procedures were approved by the University Committee on the Use and Care of Animals at the University of Michigan.

Cranial cannulation surgery

Rats were anesthetized with intraperitoneal injections of ketamine hydrochloride (80 mg/kg) and xylazine (5 mg/kg), and treated with atropine (0.05 mg/kg) to prevent respiratory distress, and then placed in a stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf Instruments). The incisor bar was set at 5.0 mm above intra-aural zero, angling cannula trajectory so as to avoid penetrating the lateral ventricles. Under surgical anesthesia, rats (n=87) received bilateral implantation of permanent cranial cannulae (14 mm, 23 gauge stainless-steel) aimed at staggered points throughout the rostrocaudal extent of medial shell of NAc. Cannulae were bilaterally inserted at coordinates between anteroposterior (AP) +2.4 to +3.1, mediolateral (ML) +/−.9 to 1.0 mm, and dorsoventral (DV) −5.6 to 5.7 mm from bregma. Cannulae were anchored to the skull using surgical screws and dental acrylic. Stainless steel obturators (28 gauge) were inserted in cannulae to avoid occlusion. After surgery, each rat received subcutaneous injection of chloramphenical sodium succinate (60 mg/kg) to prevent infection and carprofen (5 mg/kg) for pain relief. Rats received carprofen again 24 hrs later, and were allowed to recover for at least 7 days before testing began.

Drugs and intracerebral microinjections

Localized glutamate disruptions in medial shell were induced before behavioral tests by bilateral microinjections of DNQX, an AMPA/kainate receptor glutamate antagonist (6,7-dinotroquinoxaline-2,3(1H,4H)-dione; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) at a dose of 450 ng/0.5 μl per side. Either DNQX or vehicle (0.5 μl per side) was microinjected alone, or in combination with a) the selective D1 antagonist SCH23390 (R(+) – 7-chloro-8-hydroxy-3-methyl1-phenyl-2,3,4,5,-tetrahydro-1H-3-benzazepine, Sigma) at a dose of 3 μg/0.5 μl per side; or b) the selective D2 antagonist raclopride (3,5-dichloro-N-{[(2S)-1-ethylpyrrolidin-2-yl]methyl}-2-hydroxy-6-methoxybenzamide) at a dose of 5 μg/0.5 μl per side, or c) both SCH23390 and raclopride. Drug doses were chosen based on Faure et al. (2008) and Reynolds and Berridge (2003). All drugs were dissolved in a vehicle of 50% DMSO mixed with 50% 0.15 M saline, and microinjected at a volume of 0.5 μl per side. The pH was normalized to 7.0 to 7.4 using HCl for both drug and vehicle microinjections. On test days, solutions were brought to room temperature (~21°C), inspected to confirm the absence of precipitation, and bilaterally infused at a speed of 0.3 μl/minute by syringe pump via PE-20 tubing through stainless-steel injectors (16 mm, 29 gauge) extending 2 mm beyond the guide cannulae to reach NAc targets. Injectors were left in place for 1 minute following microinjection to allow drug diffusion, after which obturators were replaced and rats were immediately placed in the testing chamber.

Glutamate/dopamine interaction group

Each rat tested for motivated behavior (n=23) received the following 5 drug microinjections on different days, spaced 48 hours apart, in counter-balanced order: 1) vehicle alone, 2) DNQX alone (in order to elicit motivated behavior), 3) a mixture of DNQX plus SCH23390 (D1 blockade), 4) DNQX plus raclopride (D2 blockade), and 5) DNQX plus both SCH23390 and raclopride (combined dopamine blockade) (Faure et al., 2008).

Independent dopamine blockade group

A separate group of rats (n=18) was tested for motivated behavior after receiving microinjections of dopamine antagonists alone (without DNQX), or DNQX alone, or vehicle to ensure that dopamine antagonists in NAc shell did not prevent DNQX from generating motivations by simply eliminating motoric capacity or normal motivated behavior. Use of different groups ensured that the number of microinjections any rat received was limited to 5 or 6. This dopamine antagonist group received the following 5 drug conditions: 1) vehicle, 2) SCH23390 alone, 3) raclopride alone, 4) SCH23390 plus raclopride, and 5) DNQX alone (as a positive contrast to confirm that motivated behaviors could be generated at high intensities in these rats). All drug conditions were administered in counterbalanced order within each group and tests were spaced at least 48 hours apart.

Environmental shift group

A separate environment-shift group (n=10) was used to assess whether changing the environmental ambience flexibly altered the mode of dopamine-glutamate interactions at a particular site within the intermediate two-thirds of medial shell that is capable of generating both appetitive and fearful motivations (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008). Rats in this group had microinjection cannulae aimed at intermediate rostral-caudal sites. Each rat was tested on different days in two environments: comfortable and familiar “Home” versus overstimulating and “Stressful” (described below) in counterbalanced order. Rats were tested in each environment three times, also in counterbalanced in order, after microinjections of either: 1) vehicle, 2) DNQX, or 3) DNQX plus raclopride. Thus each rat received 6 test conditions; all separated at least 48 hrs apart in balanced order.

Behavioral tests of spontaneous motivated behaviors

Following 3 days of handling, all rats tested for motivated behavior (n = 51) were habituated to the testing procedure and apparatus on 4 days for 1 hour each. On the 4th day of habituation, rats received mock microinjections of vehicle prior to entering the test chamber, in order to habituate them to the microinjection procedure. On each test day, rats received one of the drug conditions described previously and were placed immediately in the transparent testing chamber (23 × 20 × 45 cm) which contained pre-weighed food (~20g rat chow) and ad libitum water, to allow the expression of appetitive behavior. The chamber also contained granular cob bedding spread on the floor ~3 cm deep to allow the expression of defensive treading behavior. Behavior in the chamber was videorecorded for 60 minutes, to be scored later offline for analysis. At the end of each session, rats were removed by the experimenter’s gloved hand using a standardized slow-approach hand motion in order to quantify any fearful distress calls, escape attempts or defensive bites elicited by human touch. Following a ~5 second approach towards the testing cage, the experimenter slowly reached towards the rat, taking ~2 seconds. Upon contact, the experimenter lightly brushed the side of the rat with gloved fingertips, taking ~1 sec, before lifting the rat from the chamber in a gentle movement that lasted ~2 sec. The observer recorded any attempts by the rat to escape when touched, as well as bites and audible distress vocalizations.

All behavioral tests for the above groups (n = 41) were conducted in a “Standard” lab environment (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008), following a brief transport from the Home room. The Standard environment was intended to be similar to most behavioral neuroscience laboratories in lighting, sounds, and odors, and to be of relatively neutral ambience (in between positive Home and negative Stressful of the next experiment). This Standard environment consisted of a conventional laboratory testing room (daylight illumination conditions of white fluorescent light intensity 550–650 lux, ambient noise sound intensity 65 – 70 decibels) as described previously (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008).

Rats in the environmental shift group were tested in 2 environments of opposite extreme valence: 1) the “Home” environment, which consisted of normal dim red lighting (5–10 lux) and quiet levels of ambient noise (65–70 decibels, primarily rat noise and static noise from ventilation systems), as well as familiar odors and sights of the rat’s own home-room; versus 2) the “Stressful” high-intensity sensory-stimulation environment, which was conducted in the standard laboratory except that additional incandescent lamps were directed at the test chamber (1000–1300 lux within the cage) and loud, unpredictable sound was presented continuously throughout the test (raucous rock music from the continuous full-album soundtrack of “Raw Power” by Iggy & The Stooges [1973; Iggy Pop reissue 1997]; 80–86 decibels). In preference tests, rats have been shown to prefer the Home environment over the Standard and to prefer the Standard lab environment over the Stressful (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008).

Behavioral coding

The incidence of elicited fearful distress vocalizations, escape dashes, and bite attempts directed at the experimenter’s hand were scored when the rat was gently picked up at end of the test session (Reynolds and Berridge, 2003), after which total grams of chow pellets consumed were recorded. Behaviors emitted spontaneously and videotaped during the 1-hr test were subsequently scored by experimenters blind to treatment for the total cumulative duration (seconds) for each of the following: eating behavior (involving both appetitive approach and voluntary initiation of ingestion plus consummatory chewing and swallowing of food), drinking behaviors (licking from water spout), and fearful defensive treading/burying behavior (defined as active spraying or pushing of bedding with rapid alternating thrusts of the forepaws, spatially directed generally towards the brightly lit front or corners of the cage). Additionally, the number of bouts of appetitive behaviors such as food carrying and food sniffs, as well as less-valenced behaviors such as rearing, cage crosses, and grooming behavior were also recorded.

Histology

Following behavioral testing, rats were deeply anesthetized with an overdose of sodium pentobarbital. Rats in which Fos plumes were measured were perfused and brains treated as described previously (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008). These included rats behaviorally tested in the environmental shift group (n=10; which therefore received a 7th final drug or vehicle microinjection and behavioral test 90 minutes prior to perfusion) and a separate dedicated Fos group (n = 36; which were histologically assessed after just a single drug or vehicle microinjection into locations staggered throughout medial shell, administered under conditions identical to the first day of testing for behavioral rats). The purpose of the dedicated Fos group was to assess maximal local impact radius, and avoid danger of under-estimating plume size due to progressive necrosis/gliosis over a series of microinjections that might shrink a final plume. If shrinkage occurred in the behaviorally tested group, that in turn could give rise to overly precise estimates of localization of function in brain maps. This potential distortion of impact estimates by plume shrinkage was prevented in the dedicated group that received only one microinjection.

All rats used for Fos analysis were anesthetized and transcardially perfused 90 minutes after their final or sole bilateral microinjection of vehicle (n=10), DNQX alone (n=13), DNQX plus SCH23390 (n=6), DNQX plus raclopride (n=10), DNQX plus raclopride and SCH23390 (n=3) or no solution (normal, n=3). Brain slices were processed for Fos-like immunoreactivity using NDS, goat anti-cfos (Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA) and donkey anti-goat Alexa Fluor 488 (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA) (Faure et al., 2008; Reynolds and Berridge, 2008). Sections were mounted, air-dried and coverslipped with ProLong Gold antifade reagent (Invitrogen). Zones where the expression of fluorescent Fos was elevated in neurons surrounding microinjection sites (“Fos plumes”) were assessed via microscope as described previously (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008).

Other brains were removed and fixed in 10% paraformaldehyde for 1–2 days and in 25% sucrose solution (0.1 M NaPB) for 3 days. For assessment of microinjection site locations in behaviorally tested rats, brains were sliced at 60 microns on a freezing microtome, mounted, air-dried and stained with cresyl violet for verification of microinjection sites. Bilateral microinjection sites for each rats were placed on coronal slices from a rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2007), which were used to extrapolate the position of each site on one sagittal slice. Mapping in the sagittal view allows for the presentation on the same map of the entire rostrocaudal and dorsoventral extents of NAc medial shell. Functional effects on appetitive and fearful behaviors were mapped using color-coding to express the intensity of changes in motivated behaviors for individual behaviorally-tested rats. Symbols were sized to match the maximal diameter of Fos plumes measured as described below. Sites were classified as rostral shell if their NAc placements were located +1.4 to +2.6 mm ahead of bregma, and as caudal shell if their placements were located +0.4 to +1.4 mm ahead of bregma.

Statistical analysis

The effects of DNQX on parametric behaviors were assessed using a three-factor mixed within- and between-subject ANOVA (drug × group [glutamate/dopamine interaction versus independent dopamine blockade] × anatomical level [rostral versus caudal]) to verify elicitation of eating and defensive behavior along a rostrocaudal gradient. The effects of antagonism at D1-and D2-like receptors on DNQX-induced behavior was assessed using an additional two-factor mixed within- and between subject ANOVA to compare with behavior on DNQX-alone (D1 antagonism × D2 antagonism). The effects of environmental modulation were assessed using a two-factor within-subject ANOVA (environment × drug). When significant effects were found, rats were split by anatomical location and additional analysis was done using a one-way ANOVA and pairwise comparisons using Sidak corrections for multiple comparisons. For nominal data, differences between drug conditions were assessed using McNemar’s repeated- measures test.

Results

Local AMPA receptor blockade in medial shell elicits eating and defensive treading behavior in a rostrocaudal gradient

Localized glutamate disruptions in medial shell induced by microinjections of DNQX, an AMPA/kainate receptor glutamate antagonist, stimulated intense appetitive and/or fearful behaviors depending on placement along a rostrocaudal gradient as expected (Figure 1a). At rostral sites in medial shell, NAc glutamate disruptions generated robust elevations nearly 5-times over vehicle levels in amounts of eating behavior and food consumed during the 1-hr test (cumulative duration of eating: drug × site interaction, F(1,32) = 10.0, p = .003; food intake measured in grams consumed: drug × site interaction, F(1,32) = 14.5, p = .001, Figures 2a–b, ​,3a).3a). Conversely, at caudal sites in medial shell, DNQX microinjections did not elevate food intake (and in some caudal rats actually suppressed eating and food intake below control vehicle levels; Figure 2a–b), but instead generated profound elevations in the incidence of fearful distress vocalizations (Figures 2d, ​,3c;3c; 73% of rats after DNQX microinjection vs 0% after vehicle, McNemar’s test, p = .001) and of fearful escape attempts to human touch (Figures 2e, ​,3c;3c; 40% of rats after DNQX vs 0% after vehicle, McNemar’s test, p = .031). Likewise, caudal DNQX microinjections generated nearly 10-fold increases in the spontaneous emission of defensive treading-burying behavior over vehicle control levels (Figures 2c, ​,3b;3b; drug × site interaction in cumulative duration of treading, F(1,32) = 6.9, p = .013, Figure 1a). Defensive treading typically was not diffuse or random, but rather was directionally focused on a particular target: usually towards the transparent front of the cage (beyond which objects and people in the room could be seen) and towards light-reflecting front corners of the transparent plastic chamber.

Figure 1 

Summary maps of behavior and Fos plume analysis
Figure 2 

Motivated behavior summary graphs
Figure 3 

Effects of D1 and D2 antagonism on DNQX-induced eating and defensive fearful behaviors

D1 dopamine receptor transmission alone needed for DNQX to generate appetitive behaviors at rostral sites

A novel finding here was that endogenous local dopamine stimulation was needed only at D1-like (D1, D5) receptors around the microinjection site in rostral shell for the generation of intense appetitive behavior by DNQX microinjections. Rostral D2-like receptors (D2, D3, D4) appeared essentially irrelevant to glutamate-related amplification of eating behavior and food intake (Figures 13). That is, when the dopamine D1-antagonist, SCH23390, was added to the rostral DNQX microinjection, the D1 blockade abolished the ability of DNQX to increase time spent eating or food intake, leaving eating behavior and intake at control levels seen after vehicle microinjections (Figures 2a–b and ​and3a,3a, eating: SCH23390, F(1,7) = 13.3, p = .008; Figure 2b, grams intake: SCH23390, F(1,7) = 11.1, p = .010).

By contrast, combining the D2-like antagonist raclopride with DNQX microinjection for rostral sites failed to prevent or even impair the DNQX-enhancement of eating (cumulative duration; Figures 2a–b and ​and3a,3a, raclopride, F(1,8) < 1, p = .743) or food intake (grams consumed; Figure 2b, raclopride, F(1,8) < 1, p = .517). Quite the opposite, at least at caudal shell sites, adding the D2 antagonist allowed caudal DNQX to further increase time spent eating to even higher levels that were 245% above vehicle, or 156% above eating levels produced by DNQX alone (Figures 2a, ​,3a;3a; DNQX stimulation of eating at caudal sites was usually low due to the rostrocaudal gradient: average of 566 sec +/− 101 sec on DNQX plus raclopride versus 362 sec on DNQX alone and 230 sec on vehicle; raclopride × DNQX, F(1,10) = 6.0, p = 0.035). A slight caveat to this additional enhancement is that adding the D2 antagonist did not actually boost the physical amount of food consumed for this group, even though it nearly doubled the proportion of time during the trial in which rats ate (Figure 2b, raclopride, F(1,11) < 1, p = .930; however, we note that raclopride did boost stimulation of food consumption as well as of eating behavior for caudal DNQX microinjections in a separate experiment tested below (in tests conducted in a more stressful environment).

As expected, combining both the D1 antagonist and the D2 antagonist together with DNQX completely prevented DNQX from enhancing eating (similar to D1 antagonist above), and kept levels of intake equivalent to vehicle baseline levels (Figure 2a–b; versus vehicle: grams intake, F(1,7) < 1, p = .973; eating, F(1,7) = 1.1, p = .322). However, the D1–D2 mixture of antagonists was no more effective than adding just the D1 antagonist alone to DNQX, which also completely prevented appetitive increases (Figure 2a; eating, SCH23390 plus raclopride versus SCH23390 alone, F<1, p = 1.000). In short, we conclude that only local endogenous D1 receptor neurotransmission is needed to enable glutamate disruptions in rostral sites of medial shell to stimulate appetitive behavior and food intake. By contrast, local D2 receptor neurotransmission is essentially irrelevant to rostral eating stimulation, being neither necessary nor even contributing additively in any detectable way (and possibly even inhibiting the stimulation of eating at caudal sites, perhaps via generation of fearful reactions as described below that could compete with or suppress appetitive eating).

Ruling out general suppression of appetitive/fearful behavior by dopamine antagonists

Finally, the prevention of DNQX-induced increases in food intake or eating by D1 receptor blockade appeared to reflect a specific interaction of dopamine receptors with glutamate disruptions rather than a general independent suppression of eating motivation or capacity induced by dopamine blockade. Neither microinjections of the D1 antagonist by itself (without DNQX) nor of the D2 antagonist by itself (without DNQX) suppressed baseline levels of eating below control vehicle levels of about 1 gram of chow per session (eating: SCH23390, F(1,14) = 1.9, p = .194, 149 sec +/− 52 SEM on SCH23390 versus 166 sec +/− 54 SEM on vehicle; raclopride: F(1,14) < 1, p = .389, 227 sec +/− 56 SEM; grams intake: SCH23390, F(1,14) < 1, p = .514, 1.15 grams +/− .36 SEM on SCH23390 versus .94 grams +/− .23 SEM on vehicle; raclopride, F(1,14) = 3.9, p = .068, 1.82 grams +/− .42 SEM). Thus local dopamine blockade in NAc at these doses did not impair either normal levels of motivation to eat or the motor capacity for ingestive movements. Instead our results seem to reflect a specific role of D1 receptor dopamine signals in enabling local AMPA receptor glutamate disruptions in rostral shell to stimulate eating behavior to high levels.

Fearful behaviors elicited by local glutamate disruption depend on concurrent local D1 and D2 receptor stimulation from endogenous dopamine

By contrast, simultaneous endogenous signaling at both D1 and D2 receptors in caudal sites of medial shell appeared necessary for DNQX microinjection to generate intense fearful behaviors (Figures 13). Mixing either the D1 antagonist or the D2 antagonist with DNQX effectively prevented the production of any defensive treading at caudal sites, as well as the generation of any distress calls or escape reactions to human touch that otherwise were potentiated by DNQX microinjections (Figures 2c–e, 3b–c; defensive treading: SCH23390, F(1,10) = 7.1, p = 0.024, raclopride, F(1,10) = 5.4, p = 0.043; escape attempts & jumps: DNQX alone: 40% of rats, DNQX plus SCH23390: 0%, p = 0.031 [compared to DNQX, McNemar’s test], DNQX plus raclopride: 13%, p = .219; distress calls: DNQX alone: 73% of rats, DNQX plus SCH23390: 13% of rats, p = .012, DNQX plus raclopride: 20% of rats, p = .008). In short, all fearful behaviors remained at near-zero control levels when either dopamine antagonist was mixed with DNQX.

Ruling out general suppression by dopamine antagonist microinjections

Again, D1 and D2 receptor contributions to DNQX fear induction appeared to reflect a specific interaction of these dopamine receptors with the glutamate disruption in caudal shell, because giving microinjections of either or both dopamine antagonists in the absence of DNQX did not change defensive treading from vehicle baseline levels (treading: SCH23390, F(1,14) < 1, p = .913; raclopride, F(1,14) < 1, p = .476). However, it must be noted that vehicle levels of fearful behaviors were near zero already, raising the possibility that a floor effect could have obscured a general suppression of fearful behavior by dopamine blockade. Therefore we turn to other evidence, which also suggests that dopamine antagonist microinjections, either with DNQX or by themselves, did not generally prevent most behaviors. For example, grooming, a nonvalenced behavior that was emitted at substantial rates after vehicle, remained unsuppressed by local blockade of D1 or D2 receptors. Dopamine antagonists alone did not suppress spontaneous grooming (average of 9.33 +/− 1.35 bouts on vehicle versus 8.09 +/− 1.13 on SCH23390 and 8.40 +/− 1.22 on raclopride; F<1). Likewise, adding dopamine antagonists to DNQX did not suppress grooming behavior (F<1). Microinjections of the dopamine antagonists alone did moderately suppress locomotion expressed as rears and cage crosses by about 50% from vehicle levels, though this suppression was nowhere near as strong as the abolition of DNQX-induced elevations of eating or fearful defensive treading described above (rears: SCH23390, F(1,13) = 17.6, p = .001, raclopride, F(1,13) = 9.8, p = .008; cage crosses: SCH23390, F(1,13) = 19.3, p < .001, raclopride, F(1,13) = 13.1, p = .002). Further, DNQX microinjections stimulated locomotion to double or triple vehicle levels, and adding SCH23390 or raclopride to the DNQX microinjection did not prevent that rise in cage crosses and rears (main effect of DNQX: cage crosses, F(1,33) = 12.0, p = .002; rears, F(1,33) = 6.8, p = .014; SCH23390: F<1 for rears and cage crosses; raclopride: cage crosses, F(1,19) = 2.2, p = .154; rears, F(1,19) = 3.2, p = .091). Thus general suppression effects of dopamine antagonists were either missing or minimal, and did not appear sufficient to account for the abolition of DNQX-stimulated motivated behaviors described above.

Local mode of dopamine-glutamate interaction switches flexibly as ambience reverses motivation valence

Environmental ambience flips motivational valence

As expected, for most sites in the intermediate two-thirds of medial shell (i.e., all sites between far rostral 20% and far caudal 20%), changing environmental ambience from dark, quiet and familiar (similar to rats’ home-room) to stressfully bright and noisy (extra light and raucous music) reversed the valence of motivated behavior generated by DNQX microinjections (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008) (Figure 4). Rats emitted almost exclusively appetitive behavior in the Home environment after DNQX microinjections, but emitted substantial amounts of fearful behaviors as well when tested in the Stressful environment after DNQX at the same NAc sites. The familiar, low-stimulation and presumably comfortable conditions of the Home environment (which rats have been shown to prefer to standard lab illumination condition; Reynolds and Berridge, 2008) caused the appetitive-stimulating zone within NAc to expand from rostral sites and invade caudal sites of medial shell as well, so that 90% of all medial shell locations generated intense eating behavior and food intake (greater than 200% of vehicle; Figure 4a). Concomitantly, the Home environment virtually eliminated DNQX-induction of fearful behaviors, such as distress vocalizations, escape attempts or defensive treading (Figure 4a–b; treading, DNQX, F(1,7) = 3.5, p = .102; drug × site interaction, F(1,7) < 1, p = .476). Consequently, the size of the fear-inducing zone severely shrank in the Home environment, leaving most mid-caudal sites unable to generate fearful reactions. Thus only one rat (which had the farthest caudal shell site) displayed more than 20 seconds of defensive treading in the Home environment, or emitted a distress vocalization when touched after the test (Figure 4b).

Figure 4 

Environmental ambience shifts glutamate-dopamine interaction mode

In contrast, the loud and bright Stressful environment (which rats avoid over lab conditions and quickly learn to turn off when given the opportunity; Reynolds and Berridge, 2008) expanded the caudal fear-inducing zone to include substantial mid-rostral areas of medial shell, and increased the levels of defensive treading stimulated by DNQX to over 600% the corresponding levels induced in the Home environment (Figure 4b; DNQX, F(1,7) = 23.8, p = .002; site × drug interaction, F(1,7) < 1, p = .429). Similarly, the Stressful environment increased the incidence of distress vocalizations generated after DNQX when the rats were touched by the experimenter at the end of the session by five-fold compared to the Home environment (Figure 4d; 50% of rats versus 10% at Home; McNemar’s test, p = .063). Conversely, the Stressful environment eliminated pure appetitive sites in the mid rostrocaudal zone, converting them into either mixed valence or purely fearful sites (Figure 4c). The Stressful environment also reduced the intensity of appetitive behaviors induced by DNQX at midrostral sites to approximately 50% of Home levels, even for sites that still generated any eating (average of 507 sec +/− 142 SEM in the Stressful Environment versus 879 sec +/− 87 SEM in the Home Environment; drug × environment interaction, eating, F(1,7) = 6.0, p = .044; food intake, F(1,7) = 2.9, p = .013).

Fearful mode requires D2 receptor involvement, but appetitive mode does not

The most important novel finding here was that D1/D2 receptor requirements for endogenous dopamine stimulation at a given site dynamically changed with environmental ambience shifts in a manner tied to motivational valence generated by DNQX at the moment rather than to rostrocaudal location per se. Each DNQX site had two modes: appetitive and fearful, depending on external ambience of the moment. The appetitive mode (i.e. DNQX-stimulation of eating induced by the dark, quiet and familiar Home environment) did not require D2 receptor activation to enhance eating, whereas the fearful mode (i.e. DNQX-stimulation of defensive treading behavior and distress vocalizations induced by the loud and bright Stressful environment) always required D2 receptor activation for every site to stimulate fear, regardless of rostrocaudal location (just as caudal sites had required D2 for DNQX generation of fear in the previous experiment) (Figure 4). Flips in valence mode, between appetitive and defensive, occurred for 90% of sites tested, which comprised nearly all possible intermediate rostrocaudal locations in medial shell. For the remaining 10% of sites (n = 1), DNQX microinjected into far caudal shell always generated fearful behaviors in both environments (and fearful behaviors were always eliminated by D2 blockade).

More specifically, adding the D2 antagonist to DNQX microinjection completely blocked distress calls and defensive treading behavior at all sites that otherwise generated fear after DNQX in the Stressful environment (Figure 4; rostral sites, raclopride, F(1,4) = 19.9, p = .021, all rats, raclopride, F(1,7) = 10.7, p = .022, site × drug interaction, F(1,7) < 1, p = .730). However, the D2 antagonist never blocked or suppressed eating behavior (i.e., appetitive motivation) generated at the same sites by DNQX in the Home environment; in fact, adding the D2 antagonist actually enhanced the levels of eating behavior generated by DNQX in the Stressful environment to 463% of vehicle levels and 140% of levels on DNQX alone for the same sites (Figure 4c; average of 712 sec +/− 178 SEM on DNQX plus raclopride versus 507 sec on DNQX alone and 153 sec on vehicle). In the Stressful environment, D2 blockade magnified DNQX-stimulation of eating and increased grams of food consumed, regardless of rostrocaudal location (within the intermediate zone), confirming that local D2 neurotransmission is not only unnecessary for eating enhancement but actually can oppose the generation of intense eating by local AMPA receptor blockade in medial shell (eating, raclopride, F(1,7) = 18.5, p = .008; site × drug interaction, F(1,7) < 1, p = .651; food intake, raclopride, F(1,7) = 5.6, p = .064, site × drug interaction, F(1,6) = 2.5, p = .163). While in the Standard environment D2 blockade disinhibited DNQX-eating only in caudal shell (Figure 2a), the Stressful environment expanded the fear generating zone and likewise expanded the zone in which D2-blockade disinhibits DNQX-eating to include mid-rostral zones of medial shell (Figure 4c; eating, raclopride × environment × site interaction, F(1,25) = 6.2, p = .020).

Dopamine receptor roles flip reversibly between multiple transitions

In rats that displayed ambivalent (both) motivations in the Stressful environment (60% of rats), DNQX-induced eating peaked in the first 15 minutes, while defensive treading peaked later in the trial (30 – 45 minutes after the microinjection, Figure 5a). During the 20 minutes period of maximal overlap between appetitive and defensive behavior (minutes 10 – 30), most rats transitioned from appetitive to defensive only once (16%) or 2 to 6 times (50%). With relatively few transitions during the hour, any single minute was likely to consist of pure rather than mixed motivated behaviors (Figure 5b), consistent with previous reports (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008). Dopamine D2 receptor blockade did not block eating behavior (which dominated in the first 20 minutes of the session), but effectively blocked defensive treading behavior (which dominated in the final 20 minutes).

Figure 5 

Appetitive and defensive behavior elicited from mixed valence sites in the Stressful environment

However, two rats stood out as especially ambivalent, transitioning between appetitive and defensive behavior more than 25 times each within the hour after pure DNQX microinjections in the Stressful environment. This represented the closest approach to simultaneous display of opposite motivations that we observed. Even in these rats, however, D2 receptor blockade consistently blocked only defensive behavior emitted under the loud and bright conditions, and never appetitive behavior (in either Stressful or Home environments) (example rat, Figure 5c) which continued to occur at similar levels and time points after DNQX plus D2 antagonist microinjection as after pure DNQX in the corresponding environment. Thus motivated behavior produced by dopamine-glutamate interactions appeared to be able shift rapidly and repeatedly between appetitive and fearful modes. When environmental conditions fostered ambivalence in a susceptible individual, a site could flip valence modes more than 20 times in a single hour.

Fos plume analysis: defining size of microinjection local impact

Localization of function was aided by assessing the extent of local impact of drug microinjections on nearby tissue, as reflected in Fos plumes around the microinjection center (Figure 1b). Rats used previously for behavioral testing in the environmental shift group were assessed for Fos plumes after the end of the experiment. However, as anticipated, we confirmed that rats that had already completed behavioral testing had shrunken Fos plumes compared to the dedicated Fos group that received only a single microinjection, indicating that DNQX-induced plumes from rats that received 6 previous microinjections no longer represent the maximal impact radius of drug spread. DNQX produced plumes in the dedicated Fos group that were nearly 4 times larger in volume (nearly 2 times larger in radius) than in the previously behaviorally-tested group (F(9,90) = 3.3, p < .002). Therefore, when mapping functional drug spread in all figures, we relied on plume radius data from the dedicated Fos group (matched to initial behavioral test conditions) to avoid underestimation when assessing the maximal spread of local impact for microinjections, and to construct plume maps for localization of function. However, all other data besides plume radii shown in maps were obtained exclusively from the behaviorally-tested group (i.e., colors and bar graphs reflecting intensities of eating and fearful behaviors induced at particular sites).

Pure DNQX microinjections produced plume centers of double the intensity of vehicle-level Fos expression, in a small volume of 0.02 mm3 for the dedicated Fos group (Figure 1b, top middle; radius = 0.18 +/− 0.04 mm SEM). Rats that had received 6 previous microinjections had an even smaller volume center of 0.004 mm3 (radius = 0.1 mm). Surrounding plume centers, Fos expression in the maximal group had a larger halo of 0.23 mm3 volume of milder elevation >1.5 times vehicle levels (radius = 0.38 +/− 0.05 mm SEM; rats previously tested 6 times had smaller outer halos of 0.05 mm3 volume, radius = .23 mm). Addition of the D1 antagonist (SCH23390) shrank plumes and attenuated the intensity of DNQX-induced elevations in local Fos expression (Figure 1b, bottom middle; DNQX versus DNQX plus SCH23390, Post hoc pairwise comparison with Sidak corrections, p < 0.01). SCH23390 shrank the total volume of DNQX Fos plumes to less than 0.18mm3 (outer halo radius = 0.35 +/− 0.05 mm SEM). By contrast, addition of the D2 antagonist (raclopride) expanded intense centers of Fos expression and enhanced DNQX-induced elevation in local Fos expression (Figure 1b, bottom left; DNQX versus DNQX plus raclopride, Post hoc pairwise comparisons with Sidak corrections, p < 0.05). Raclopride expanded the inner center of doubled Fos expression produced by DNQX to a volume of 0.15 mm3 (radius = .33 +/− 0.042 mm SEM), and left unchanged the radius and intensity of the outer plume halo (of 1.5x expression). We note that the D1 antagonist apparently predominates over the D2 antagonist in effects on local Fos when both are microinjected jointly with DNQX, as DNQX Fos plumes shrink following the addition of combined D1 and D2 antagonists (Faure et al., 2008).

Discussion

In rostral shell, only endogenous dopamine signaling at D1-like receptors was needed for DNQX microinjections to stimulate 5-fold increases in eating. By contrast, in caudal shell, simultaneous signaling at D1- and D2-like receptors was needed for DNQX to generate 10-times increases in fearful reactions (distress calls, escape attempts and active defensive treading directed at objects in cage or beyond). Yet, rostral sites in medial shell were not simply D1 dominant nor were caudal sites D1–D2 co-dominant for generation of motivations by glutamate disruptions. Most intermediate sites in shell switched flexibly between generating appetitive and fearful motivations when environmental ambience changed. For those sites, D2 activity was always required for fear generation by DNQX microinjection (in the stressful environment) but never required for appetitive generation of eating (in the familiar home environment). Not only was D2 signaling unnecessary, D2 receptor blockade actually disinhibited DNQX-stimulation of eating at sites when placement/environment combination otherwise facilitated fear. In short, rostrocaudal placement strongly biases the valence of motivational salience produced by glutamatergic disruptions, but dopamine interaction modes are more closely tied to appetitive/fearful valence generated at a given moment than to location per se (Reynolds and Berridge, 2008).

Mechanism of interaction between dopamine and glutamate blockade

The precise mechanism of NAc dopamine-glutamate interaction in generating intense incentive salience versus fearful salience remains a puzzle. Purely speculatively, we offer several possibilities. In the absence of glutamatergic input during AMPA blockade, NAc neurons reduce already low rates of firing, become hyperpolarized, and possibly disinhibit downstream targets in ventral pallidum (VP), lateral hypothalamus (LH) and ventral tegmentum (VTA) to stimulate motivated behaviors (Taber and Fibiger, 1997; Kelley, 1999; Meredith et al., 2008; Roitman et al., 2008; Krause et al., 2010). However, if dopamine primarily modulates glutamatergic depolarizations (Calabresi et al., 1997) then dopamine might be viewed as largely irrelevant to such hyperpolarizations.

Still, one possibility is that D2 receptor activation attenuates remaining excitatory AMPA postsynaptic impact (Cepeda et al., 1993), and so D2 blockade might prevent AMPA attenuation, disrupting local hyperpolarizations. Alternatively, D1-receptor activation may facilitate hyperpolarization in relatively inhibited neurons (Higashi et al., 1989; Pennartz et al., 1992; Moyer et al., 2007; Surmeier et al., 2007), and so D1 blockade might likewise disrupt those hyperpolarizations. Presynaptic mechanisms might also contribute, based on potential suppression of glutamate release by NAc D1 receptor activation on hippocampal or amygdala terminals, and similar presynaptic D2 suppression at prefrontal terminals (Pennartz et al., 1992; Nicola et al., 1996; Charara and Grace, 2003; Bamford et al., 2004). Presynaptic dopamine blockade might disrupt such suppressions, and consequently increase glutamate release, potentially overcoming DNQX effects.

A remaining class of explanation could involve more subtle dopamine/glutamate interaction. For example, DNQX microinjections might shift AMPA/NMDA activation ratios towards NMDA, potentially relevant if NMDA receptors provide current contributions in the absence of AMPA currents (Cull-Candy and Leszkiewicz, 2004; Hull et al., 2009). Additionally, DNQX-induced local hyperpolarization may, via GABAergic connections between neighbors, laterally disinhibit surrounding neurons (Mao and Massaquoi, 2007; Faure et al., 2008 ; Tepper et al., 2008). Dopamine blockade could counteract both of these effects by disrupting both NMDA-mediated currents (Cepeda et al., 1993; Surmeier et al., 2007; Sun et al., 2008) and lateral inhibition (Taverna et al., 2005; Grace et al., 2007; Moyer et al., 2007; Nicola, 2007). The actual roles of these or other mechanisms in generating these phenomena will need future clarification.

Direct and indirect output pathways in D1 and D2 dependent motivation

Direct and indirect pathways from shell may differentially contribute to incentive versus aversive motivation (Hikida et al., 2010). In general for striatum, D2-expressing outputs travel chiefly via the indirect pathway, and D1-expressing outputs travel via the direct pathway (Gerfen and Young, 1988; Gerfen et al., 1990; Bertran-Gonzalez et al., 2008; Matamales et al., 2009). For NAc medial shell in particular, D1-expressing neurons similarly constitute the direct output pathway to VTA, whereas equal populations of D1 and D2-dominant neurons project along the indirect pathway to VP and LH (Figure 6) (Haber et al., 1985; Heimer et al., 1991; Lu et al., 1998; Zhou et al., 2003; Humphries and Prescott, 2010). Additionally, 15% – 30% of shell neurons, likely projecting along the indirect pathway, co-express both D1 and D2 receptors, which sometimes form a conjoined heteromer (Humphries and Prescott, 2010; Perreault et al., 2010; Perreault et al., 2011). Speculatively, the importance of D1 receptors in enabling glutamate disruptions to generate appetitive behavior might reflect a primacy of the direct pathway from NAc to VTA. In contrast, the need for D1 and D2 co-activation for DNQX-fear generation might highlight a greater contribution of the indirect pathway.

Figure 6 

Mesocorticolimbic circuits impacted by glutamate-dopamine interactions

Valence mode shifts and rostrocaudal biases: Mesocorticolimbic circuits

Shifts between familiar and stressful environmental ambience modulate mesocorticolimbic circuits, likely altering glutamatergic inputs to NAc from prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala (BLA), hippocampus and thalamus (Swanson, 2005; Zahm, 2006; Belujon and Grace, 2008), which may interact with D1/D2 dopamine signals. For example, after theta burst firing from the BLA, rostral shell neurons can show decreased responsiveness to subsequent BLA stimulations, whereas neurons in caudal shell are more likely to increase subsequent firing to the same BLA stimulations, a difference which requires D2 receptors and which might modulate the size of appetitive vs. fear-generation zones within medial shell (Gill and Grace, 2011). Particular features of mesocorticolimbic inputs may also be important for the shell’s intrinsic rostrocaudal gradient. For instance, norepinephrine from hindbrain is released chiefly in caudal regions of shell, facilitated by dopamine D1 stimulation but inhibited by D2, and might help modulate motivation valence (Berridge et al., 1997; Delfs et al., 1998; Vanderschuren et al., 1999; Schroeter et al., 2000; Park et al., 2010). Finally, point-to-point corticolimbic targeting from prefrontal cortex zones to subregions of medial shell, VP/LH and their downstream targets, permit multiple segregated loops to travel through mesocorticolimbic circuits (Thompson and Swanson, 2010), which could further contribute to localization of desire and dread generators.

Caveats regarding D1 and D2 receptors in motivated behavior

We believe our findings do not necessarily conflict with others’ reports of D2/D3 involvement in incentive motivation (Bachtell et al., 2005; Bari and Pierce, 2005; Xi et al., 2006; Heidbreder et al., 2007; Gardner, 2008; Khaled et al., 2010; Song et al., 2011). As caveat, we note our findings are strictly limited to mechanisms that simultaneously involve: a) glutamate-dopamine interactions, b) within NAc medial shell, that c) generate intense elevation of appetitive/fearful motivations. Although our conclusions are consistent with reports that D1 (but not D2) blockade in NAc shell prevents appetitive VTA-stimulated eating (MacDonald et al., 2004) and prevents appetitive self-stimulation via optogenetic activation of glutamatergic amygdala-NAc projections (Stuber et al., 2011), as well as reports that D2 signaling contributes to active defensive behaviors (Filibeck et al., 1988; Puglisi-Allegra and Cabib, 1988), our results do not preclude other roles for D2/D3 receptors in generating appetitive motivation in different situations. In particular, we do not contradict appetitive roles produced in different brain structures, involving different reactions (e.g., learned rather than unconditioned) or that involve deficits below normal levels of motivation. Understanding dopamine receptor roles in generating motivations will eventually require integration of all relevant facts.

GABA and metabotropic glutamate generation of motivated behavior

We suggest that rostral dopamine/glutamate interactions here generated positive incentive salience, making food perceived as more attractive to eat. By contrast, caudal or negatively-valenced interactions generated fearful salience, making objects and experimenter perceived as threatening. We previously reported metabotropic glutamate blockade at sites throughout medial shell to generate fear and disgust (Richard and Berridge, 2011), and reported local GABAergic hyperpolarizations to generate rostrocaudal gradients of feeding and fear, similar to the keyboard pattern described here (Reynolds and Berridge, 2001; Faure et al., 2010). However, we do not suggest that the dopamine interactions with ionotropic glutamatergic disruptions identified here necessarily apply to metabotropic or to GABAergic NAc mechanisms of motivation. Dopamine involvement in those remains an open question. There are several neuronal differences (e.g., direct GABAergic hyperpolarizations of neurons versus glutamate blockade-mediated hyperpolarization) and functional differences (e.g., shifts in hedonic impact versus induction of motivated behavior) that could prove important.

Implications for psychopathology

Corticolimbic dopamine-glutamate interactions have been linked to both intense incentive salience and fearful salience, contributing to appetitive motivation in addiction and to intense fearful motivation in psychotic paranoia (Wang and McGinty, 1999; Barch, 2005; Taylor et al., 2005; Lapish et al., 2006; Faure et al., 2008; Jensen et al., 2008; Kalivas et al., 2009). Flips in the valence of pathologically intense motivational salience also can occur (Morrow et al., 2011). Amphetamine addicts can experience fearful “amphetamine psychosis” similar to paranoia, which may involve pathological exaggerations of fearful salience (Featherstone et al., 2007; Jensen et al., 2008; Howes and Kapur, 2009). Conversely, some schizophrenic patients exhibit higher brain activations that encode appetitive incentive salience (Elman et al., 2006; Diaconescu et al., 2011). Overall, understanding how glutamate-dopamine interactions within NAc shell create intense appetitive and/or fearful motivations may illuminate the mechanisms underlying such intense but opposite disorders of motivation.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants (DA015188 and MH63649 to KCB) and by a National Research Service Award fellowship to JMR (MH090602). We thank Stephen Burwell and Andy Deneen for assistance with histology, and Brandon Aragona, Geoffrey Murphy, Joshua Berke, and Benjamin Saunders for helpful comments and discussion.

References

  • Bachtell RK, Whisler K, Karanian D, Self DW. Effects of intra-nucleus accumbens shell administration of dopamine agonists and antagonists on cocaine-taking and cocaine-seeking behaviors in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;183:41–53. [PubMed]
  • Bamford NS, Zhang H, Schmitz Y, Wu NP, Cepeda C, Levine MS, Schmauss C, Zakharenko SS, Zablow L, Sulzer D. Heterosynaptic dopamine neurotransmission selects sets of corticostriatal terminals. Neuron. 2004;42:653–663. [PubMed]
  • Barch DM. The relationships among cognition, motivation, and emotion in schizophrenia: how much and how little we know. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:875–881. [PubMed]
  • Bari AA, Pierce RC. D1-like and D2 dopamine receptor antagonists administered into the shell subregion of the rat nucleus accumbens decrease cocaine, but not food, reinforcement. Neuroscience. 2005;135:959–968. [PubMed]
  • Belujon P, Grace AA. Critical Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in the Regulation of Hippocampus-Accumbens Information Flow. J Neurosci. 2008;28:9797–9805. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Berridge CW, Stratford TL, Foote SL, Kelley AE. Distribution of dopamine beta-hydroxylase-like immunoreactive fibers within the shell subregion of the nucleus accumbens. Synapse. 1997;27:230–241. [PubMed]
  • Bertran-Gonzalez J, Bosch C, Maroteaux M, Matamales M, Herve D, Valjent E, Girault JA. Opposing patterns of signaling activation in dopamine D1 and D2 receptor-expressing striatal neurons in response to cocaine and haloperidol. J Neurosci. 2008;28:5671–5685. [PubMed]
  • Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. The mesoaccumbens dopamine in coping with stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011 [PubMed]
  • Calabresi P, Pisani A, Centonze D, Bernardi G. Synaptic plasticity and physiological interactions between dopamine and glutamate in the striatum. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1997;21:519–523. [PubMed]
  • Carlezon WA, Thomas MJ. Biological substrates of reward and aversion: A nucleus accumbens activity hypothesis. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:122–132. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Cepeda C, Buchwald NA, Levine MS. Neuromodulatory actions of dopamine in the neostriatum are dependent upon the excitatory amino acid receptor subtypes activated. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1993;90:9576–9580. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Charara A, Grace AA. Dopamine receptor subtypes selectively modulate excitatory afferents from the hippocampus and amygdala to rat nucleus accumbens neurons. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1412–1421. [PubMed]
  • Coss RG, Owings DH. Snake-Directed Behavior By Snake Naive and Experienced California Ground Squirrels in a Simulated Burrow. Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsychologie-Journal of Comparative Ethology. 1978;48:421–435.
  • Cull-Candy SG, Leszkiewicz DN. Role of distinct NMDA receptor subtypes at central synapses. Sci STKE. 2004;2004:re16. [PubMed]
  • Delfs JM, Zhu Y, Druhan JP, Aston-Jones GS. Origin of noradrenergic afferents to the shell subregion of the nucleus accumbens: anterograde and retrograde tract-tracing studies in the rat. Brain Res. 1998;806:127–140. [PubMed]
  • Diaconescu AO, Jensen J, Wang H, Willeit M, Menon M, Kapur S, McIntosh AR. Aberrant effective connectivity in schizophrenia patients during appetitive conditioning. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;4:239. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Elman I, Borsook D, Lukas SE. Food intake and reward mechanisms in patients with schizophrenia: implications for metabolic disturbances and treatment with second-generation antipsychotic agents. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:2091–2120. [PubMed]
  • Faure A, Richard JM, Berridge KC. Desire and dread from the nucleus accumbens: Cortical glutamate and subcortical GABA differentially generate motivation and hedonic impact in the rat. PloS one. 2010;5:e11223. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Faure A, Reynolds SM, Richard JM, Berridge KC. Mesolimbic dopamine in desire and dread: enabling motivation to be generated by localized glutamate disruptions in nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2008;28:7184–7192. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Featherstone RE, Kapur S, Fletcher PJ. The amphetamine-induced sensitized state as a model of schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2007;31:1556–1571. [PubMed]
  • Filibeck U, Cabib S, Castellano C, Puglisi-Allegra S. Chronic cocaine enhances defensive behaviour in the laboratory mouse: involvement of D2 dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1988;96:437–441. [PubMed]
  • Gardner EL. Use of animal models to develop antiaddiction medications. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2008;10:377–384. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gerfen CR, Young WS., 3rd Distribution of striatonigral and striatopallidal peptidergic neurons in both patch and matrix compartments: an in situ hybridization histochemistry and fluorescent retrograde tracing study. Brain Res. 1988;460:161–167. [PubMed]
  • Gerfen CR, Engber TM, Mahan LC, Susel Z, Chase TN, Monsma FJ, Jr, Sibley DR. D1 and D2 dopamine receptor-regulated gene expression of striatonigral and striatopallidal neurons. Science. 1990;250:1429–1432. [PubMed]
  • Gill KM, Grace AA. Heterogeneous processing of amygdala and hippocampal inputs in the rostral and caudal subregions of the nucleus accumbens. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011:1–14. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Grace AA, Floresco SB, Goto Y, Lodge DJ. Regulation of firing of dopaminergic neurons and control of goal-directed behaviors. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30:220–227. [PubMed]
  • Haber SN, Groenewegen HJ, Grove EA, Nauta WJ. Efferent connections of the ventral pallidum: evidence of a dual striato pallidofugal pathway. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1985;235:322–335. [PubMed]
  • Heidbreder CA, Andreoli M, Marcon C, Hutcheson DM, Gardner EL, Ashby CR., Jr Evidence for the role of dopamine D3 receptors in oral operant alcohol self-administration and reinstatement of alcohol-seeking behavior in mice. Addiction Biology. 2007;12:35–50. [PubMed]
  • Heimer L, Zahm DS, Churchill L, Kalivas PW, Wohltmann C. Specificity in the projection patterns of accumbal core and shell in the rat. Neuroscience. 1991;41:89–125. [PubMed]
  • Higashi H, Inanaga K, Nishi S, Uchimura N. Enhancement of dopamine actions on rat nucleus accumbens neurones in vitro after methamphetamine pre-treatment. J Physiol. 1989;408:587–603. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hikida T, Kimura K, Wada N, Funabiki K, Nakanishi S. Distinct roles of synaptic transmission in direct and indirect striatal pathways to reward and aversive behavior. Neuron. 2010;66:896–907. [PubMed]
  • Howes OD, Kapur S. The Dopamine Hypothesis of Schizophrenia: Version III-025EFThe Final Common Pathway. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2009;35:549–562. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hull C, Isaacson JS, Scanziani M. Postsynaptic mechanisms govern the differential excitation of cortical neurons by thalamic inputs. J Neurosci. 2009;29:9127–9136. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Humphries MD, Prescott TJ. The ventral basal ganglia, a selection mechanism at the crossroads of space, strategy, and reward. Prog Neurobiol. 2010;90:385–417. [PubMed]
  • Jensen J, Willeit M, Zipursky RB, Savina I, Smith AJ, Menon M, Crawley AP, Kapur S. The formation of abnormal associations in schizophrenia: neural and behavioral evidence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:473–479. [PubMed]
  • Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1403–1413. [PubMed]
  • Kalivas PW, LaLumiere RT, Knackstedt L, Shen HW. Glutamate transmission in addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56:169–173. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Kelley AE. Neural integrative activities of nucleus accumbens subregions in relation to learning and motivation. Psychobiology. 1999;27:198–213.
  • Kelley AE, Swanson CJ. Feeding induced by blockade of AMPA and kainate receptors within the ventral striatum: a microinfusion mapping study. Behavioural Brain Research. 1997;89:107–113. [PubMed]
  • Kelley AE, Baldo BA, Pratt WE, Will MJ. Corticostriatal-hypothalamic circuitry and food motivation: Integration of energy, action and reward. Physiol Behav. 2005;86:773–795. [PubMed]
  • Khaled MA, Farid Araki K, Li B, Coen KM, Marinelli PW, Varga J, Gaal J, Le Foll B. The selective dopamine D3 receptor antagonist SB 277011-A, but not the partial agonist BP 897, blocks cue-induced reinstatement of nicotine-seeking. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;13:181–190. [PubMed]
  • Krause M, German PW, Taha SA, Fields HL. A pause in nucleus accumbens neuron firing is required to initiate and maintain feeding. J Neurosci. 2010;30:4746–4756. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Lapish CC, Seamans JK, Chandler LJ. Glutamate-dopamine cotransmission and reward processing in addiction. Alcoholism-Clinical and Experimental Research. 2006;30:1451–1465. [PubMed]
  • Levita L, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Nucleus accumbens dopamine and learned fear revisited: a review and some new findings. Behavioural Brain Research. 2002;137:115–127. [PubMed]
  • Lu XY, Ghasemzadeh MB, Kalivas PW. Expression of D1 receptor, D2 receptor, substance P and enkephalin messenger RNAs in the neurons projecting from the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience. 1998;82:767–780. [PubMed]
  • MacDonald AF, Billington CJ, Levine AS. Alterations in food intake by opioid and dopamine signaling pathways between the ventral tegmental area and the shell of the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. 2004;1018:78–85. [PubMed]
  • Maldonado-Irizarry CS, Swanson CJ, Kelley AE. Glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell control feeding behavior via the lateral hypothalamus. Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15:6779–6788. [PubMed]
  • Mao ZH, Massaquoi SG. Dynamics of winner-take-all competition in recurrent neural networks with lateral inhibition. IEEE Trans Neural Netw. 2007;18:55–69. [PubMed]
  • Matamales M, Bertran-Gonzalez J, Salomon L, Degos B, Deniau JM, Valjent E, Herve D, Girault JA. Striatal medium-sized spiny neurons: identification by nuclear staining and study of neuronal subpopulations in BAC transgenic mice. PLoS One. 2009;4:e4770. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Matsumoto M, Hikosaka O. Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals. Nature. 2009;459:837–841. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Meredith GE, Baldo BA, Andrezjewski ME, Kelley AE. The structural basis for mapping behavior onto the ventral striatum and its subdivisions. Brain Struct Funct. 2008;213:17–27. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Morrow JD, Maren S, Robinson TE. Individual variation in the propensity to attribute incentive salience to an appetitive cue predicts the propensity to attribute motivational salience to an aversive cue. Behav Brain Res. 2011;220:238–243. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Moyer JT, Wolf JA, Finkel LH. Effects of dopaminergic modulation on the integrative properties of the ventral striatal medium spiny neuron. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3731–3748. [PubMed]
  • Nicola SM. The nucleus accumbens as part of a basal ganglia action selection circuit. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191:521–550. [PubMed]
  • Nicola SM, Kombian SB, Malenka RC. Psychostimulants depress excitatory synaptic transmission in the nucleus accumbens via presynaptic D1-like dopamine receptors. J Neurosci. 1996;16:1591–1604. [PubMed]
  • Park J, Aragona BJ, Kile BM, Carelli RM, Wightman RM. In vivo voltammetric monitoring of catecholamine release in subterritories of the nucleus accumbens shell. Neuroscience. 2010;169:132–142. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. New York: Academic Press; 2007.
  • Pennartz CM, Dolleman-Van der Weel MJ, Kitai ST, Lopes da Silva FH. Presynaptic dopamine D1 receptors attenuate excitatory and inhibitory limbic inputs to the shell region of the rat nucleus accumbens studied in vitro. J Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1325–1334. [PubMed]
  • Perreault ML, O’Dowd BF, George SR. Dopamine receptor homooligomers and heterooligomers in schizophrenia. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2011;17:52–57. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Perreault ML, Hasbi A, Alijaniaram M, Fan T, Varghese G, Fletcher PJ, Seeman P, O’Dowd BF, George SR. The dopamine D1–D2 receptor heteromer localizes in dynorphin/enkephalin neurons: Increased high affinity state following amphetamine and in schizophrenia. J Biol Chem 2010 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Puglisi-Allegra S, Cabib S. Pharmacological evidence for a role of D2 dopamine receptors in the defensive behavior of the mouse. Behav Neural Biol. 1988;50:98–111. [PubMed]
  • Reynolds SM, Berridge KC. Fear and feeding in the nucleus accumbens shell: rostrocaudal segregation of GABA-elicited defensive behavior versus eating behavior. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:3261–3270. [PubMed]
  • Reynolds SM, Berridge KC. Glutamate motivational ensembles in nucleus accumbens: rostrocaudal shell gradients of fear and feeding. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;17:2187–2200. [PubMed]
  • Reynolds SM, Berridge KC. Emotional environments retune the valence of appetitive versus fearful functions in nucleus accumbens. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:423–425. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Richard JM, Berridge KC. Metabotropic glutamate receptor blockade in nucleus accumbens shell shifts affective valence towards fear and disgust. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:736–747. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Roitman MF, Wheeler RA, Wightman RM, Carelli RM. Real-time chemical responses in the nucleus accumbens differentiate rewarding and aversive stimuli. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:1376–1377. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Salamone JD, Correa M, Mingote SM, Weber SM. Beyond the reward hypothesis: alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Current Opinion In Pharmacology. 2005;5:34–41. [PubMed]
  • Schroeter S, Apparsundaram S, Wiley RG, Miner LH, Sesack SR, Blakely RD. Immunolocalization of the cocaine- and antidepressant-sensitive l-norepinephrine transporter. J Comp Neurol. 2000;420:211–232. [PubMed]
  • Schultz W. Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:203–210. [PubMed]
  • Song R, Yang RF, Wu N, Su RB, Li J, Peng XQ, Li X, Gaal J, Xi ZX, Gardner EL. YQA14: a novel dopamine D(3) receptor antagonist that inhibits cocaine self-administration in rats and mice, but not in D(3) receptor-knockout mice. Addict Biol 2011 [PubMed]
  • Stuber GD, Sparta DR, Stamatakis AM, van Leeuwen WA, Hardjoprajitno JE, Cho S, Tye KM, Kempadoo KA, Zhang F, Deisseroth K, Bonci A. Excitatory transmission from the amygdala to nucleus accumbens facilitates reward seeking. Nature 2011 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sun X, Milovanovic M, Zhao Y, Wolf M. Acute and chronic dopamine receptor stimulation modulates AMPA receptor trafficking in nucleus accumbens neurons cocultured with prefrontal cortex neurons. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4216–4230. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Surmeier DJ, Ding J, Day M, Wang Z, Shen W. D1 and D2 dopamine-receptor modulation of striatal glutamatergic signaling in striatal medium spiny neurons. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30:228–235. [PubMed]
  • Swanson LW. Anatomy of the soul as reflected in the cerebral hemispheres: neural circuits underlying voluntary control of basic motivated behaviors. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493:122–131. [PubMed]
  • Taber MT, Fibiger HC. Feeding-evoked dopamine release in the nucleus, accumbens: regulation by glutamatergic mechanisms. Neuroscience. 1997;76:1105–1112. [PubMed]
  • Taverna S, Canciani B, Pennartz CM. Dopamine D1-receptors modulate lateral inhibition between principal cells of the nucleus accumbens. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:1816–1819. [PubMed]
  • Taylor SF, Phan KL, Britton JC, Liberzon I. Neural response to emotional salience in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:984–995. [PubMed]
  • Tepper JM, Wilson CJ, Koos T. Feedforward and feedback inhibition in neostriatal GABAergic spiny neurons. Brain Res Rev. 2008;58:272–281. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Thompson RH, Swanson LW. Hypothesis-driven structural connectivity analysis supports network over hierarchical model of brain architecture. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:15235–15239. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Treit D, Pinel JP, Fibiger HC. Conditioned defensive burying: a new paradigm for the study of anxiolytic agents. Pharmacology, Biochemistry & Behavior. 1981;15:619–626. [PubMed]
  • Vanderschuren L, Wardeh G, De Vries TJ, Mulder AH, Schoffelmeer ANM. Opposing role of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in modulation of rat nucleus accumbens noradrenaline release. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:4123–4131. [PubMed]
  • Ventura R, Morrone C, Puglisi-Allegra S. Prefrontal/accumbal catecholamine system determines motivational salience attribution to both reward- and aversion-related stimuli. PNAS. 2007;104:5181–5186. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wang JQ, McGinty JF. Glutamate-dopamine interactions mediate the effects of psychostimulant drugs. Addiction Biology. 1999;4:141–150. [PubMed]
  • Wise RA. Dopamine and reward: the anhedonia hypothesis 30 years on. Neurotox Res. 2008;14:169–183. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Woodward ND, Cowan RL, Park S, Ansari MS, Baldwin RM, Li R, Doop M, Kessler RM, Zald DH. Correlation of individual differences in schizotypal personality traits with amphetamine-induced dopamine release in striatal and extrastriatal brain regions. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168:418–426. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Xi ZX, Newman AH, Gilbert JG, Pak AC, Peng XQ, Ashby CR, Jr, Gitajn L, Gardner EL. The novel dopamine D3 receptor antagonist NGB 2904 inhibits cocaine’s rewarding effects and cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1393–1405. [PubMed]
  • Zahm DS. The evolving theory of basal forebrain functional–anatomical ‘macrosystems’ Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30:148–172. [PubMed]
  • Zhou L, Furuta T, Kaneko T. Chemical organization of projection neurons in the rat accumbens nucleus and olfactory tubercle. Neuroscience. 2003;120:783–798. [PubMed]
  • Zubieta JK, Stohler CS. Neurobiological mechanisms of placebo responses. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1156:198–210. [PMC free article] [PubMed]