A Behavioral and Circuit Model Based on Sugar Addiction in Rats (2009)

. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 Mar 16.

PMCID: PMC4361030

NIHMSID: NIHMS669567

Abstract

The distinction between natural addiction and drug addiction is interesting from many points of view, including scientific and medical perspectives. “Natural addictions” are those based on activation of a physiobehavioral system, such as the one that controls metabolism, foraging, and eating to achieve energy balance. “Drug addictions” activate many systems based on their pharmacology. This review discusses the following questions: (1) When does food produce a natural addiction? Sugar causes signs of addiction if the scheduling conditions are appropriate to cause binge eating. (2) Why does addictive-like behavior result? Bingeing on a 10% sucrose solution repeatedly releases dopamine in the nucleus accumbens, and it delays the release of acetylcholine, thereby postponing satiety. Opioid involvement is shown by withdrawal caused by naloxone or food deprivation. Bingeing, withdrawal, and abstinence-induced motivation are described as the basis for a vicious cycle leading to excessive eating. (3) Which foods can lead to natural addiction? A variety of sugars, saccharin, and sham feeding are compared with bingeing on high-fat diets, which seem to lack sugar’s opioid-withdrawal characteristic. (4) How does natural food addiction relate to obesity? Low basal dopamine may be a common factor, leading to “eating for dopamine.” (5) In a neural model, the accumbens is depicted as having separate GABA output pathways for approach and avoidance, both controlled by dopamine and acetylcholine. These outputs, in turn, control lateral hypothalamic glutamate release, which starts a meal, and GABA release, which stops it.

Keywords: dopamine, acetylcholine, accumbens, binge, bulimia

NATURAL AND DRUG ADDICTIONS

The definition of addiction is open to debate. An early view described drug addiction as being due to a lack of will power, making addiction a moral condition. Later, addiction was described in modern terms of neuropsychopharmacology as a “disease” caused by drug-induced chronic adaptations in brain function that change a voluntary behavior into an uncontrollable habit. This view of drug addiction as a disease-state partially shifts the blame from the person to the drug; however, both views depict the end result in terms of compulsive behavior and loss of control. Recently, there has been a move in the direction of deemphasizing drugs and suggesting that addiction, including addiction to activities such as eating or sexual behavior, be framed as unusually strong, desires for pleasure. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders sidestepped the issue of addiction, per se, and focused on the criteria for “dependence,” with continued, life-disruptive, substance abuse as the benchmark for diagnosis. Disruptive behavior is continued despite knowledge of persistent physical or psychological problems, which are likely caused or exacerbated by the substance of abuse. Debates are now appearing in anticipation of the next diagnostic manual. Our view, based largely on evidence from laboratory animal research, is that addiction to sugar could be a problem and can involve the same neural adaptations and behavioral alterations as addiction to drugs., These changes are observed in instances of aberrant feeding, which can be modeled in the laboratory. The closest human condition to our laboratory animal model would be binge eating disorder or bulimia nervosa. Evidence for addiction in patients with eating disorders has been presented., Brain imaging studies have focused attention on addiction-like changes in the obese population, where the psychological risks of dependency are compounded by medical risks, including cardiovascular impairment and type-2 diabetes.,

To understand “addiction,” one must identify the neural systems that cause it. Addictive drugs act, in part, via systems that evolved for ingestive and perhaps reproductive behaviors. This means that addiction to specific behavior patterns may have evolved through genetic benefits that selected animals with innately programmed addictive processes. If so, there are 2 major kinds of addiction, both of which can become compulsive and sometimes dangerous: (1) survival behavior, such as that which leads to risky behavior for eating and mating and (2) maladaptive behavior that bypasses the normal inhibitory sensory signals and artificially stimulates the reward systems, as in the case of drugs of abuse.

In summary, natural addiction can occur when environmental stimuli act via designated, normal receptor systems, such as sugar acting via glucoreceptors. In this case, the “system” involved is one that evolved with energy regulation as the survival benefit. Drug addiction can result from compounds that can bypass sensory inputs and act within a system that is characterized by its neurochemical function. Thus, drugs such as psychostimulants or opiates may activate multiple systems with diverse physiobehavioral functions. It would be illogical to claim that only drugs can be addictive, if it could be proven that natural stimulation, such as activation of the energy control system, can be sufficient for the addictive process to occur.

WHEN DOES SUGAR PRODUCE A NATURAL ADDICTION? EATING IN BINGES CAN FACILITATE ADDICTION

After 10 years of research on sugar addiction,,, we still use the same basic technique to obtain clear signs of food dependency by imposing a feeding schedule that repeatedly induces sugar bingeing after a period of fasting. In our animal model of sugar bingeing, a “binge” is defined simply as an unusually large meal, compared with animals eating the same diet ad libitum. Periodic, 12-hour food restriction is used to create hunger and anticipation of eating. Then the animals are offered 25% glucose (or 10% sucrose to simulate the sugar concentration of a soft drink) along with their rodent chow. The opportunity to begin the first meal of the day is delayed 4 hours beyond the time they would have normally started eating at dark onset. Over the course of 3 weeks, this daily food restriction and delayed feeding results in 32% of the rat’s caloric intake coming from sugar. Rats on this daily 12-hour schedule of sugar and chow escalate their total daily sugar intake during the weeks of access. It is interesting to note that some rats with 12-hour access to sugar take not only a large meal at the onset of access but they also binge spontaneously throughout the feeding period.

Rats with ad libitum access to the sugar solution are a valuable control group. They drink sugar even during the inactive, light phase. These animals consume the same large quantities of sugar solution as bingeing rats; however, it is spread out over the course of 24 hours. We do not see evidence of binge-eating behavior with ad libitum sugar access. As a result, they do not show signs of dependency. Thus, it is the intermittent feeding schedule that seems to be critical for inducing bingeing and the subsequent signs of dependency. In Figure 1, bingeing is indicated as the first stage in route to addiction.

FIGURE 1 

Schematic representation of some criteria used to classify substances of abuse as described by Koob and Le Moal. We have applied these criteria to the study of food addiction. Limited daily access to a sugar solution leads to bingeing and ensuing opiate-like

WHY DOES SUGAR BINGEING RESULT IN ADDICTIVE-LIKE BEHAVIORS?

Bingeing causes repeated, excessive dopamine (DA) release and opioid stimulation that is followed, during abstinence, by progressive changes that enhance the likelihood of relapse.

Opioid Adaptations and Signs of Withdrawal

The comparison of sugar addiction with drug addiction has been reviewed in detail., In just a few weeks on the intermittent, 12-hour sugar-chow feeding schedule, rats will show signs of opiate-like “withdrawal” in response to naloxone (3 mg/kg s.c.), which proves opioid involvement and suggests opioid “dependency.” Withdrawal is also seen without naloxone, when both food and sugar are denied for 24 hours.,, Our quantitative polymerase chain reaction (qPCR) and autoradiographic evidence in sugar-bingeing rats shows downregulated enkephalin mRNA and upregulated mureceptor binding in the nucleus accumbens (NAc). This is interpreted to mean that repeated sugar bingeing releases opioids, such as enkephalin or beta-endorphin, and the brain compensates by expressing less of these opioid peptides in certain regions. Perhaps the postsynaptic cells respond to less of these peptides by expressing or exposing more mu-opioid receptors. If the receptors are then blocked by naloxone, or the rats are food deprived, the animals display anxiety in an elevated plus-maze, and depression in a swim test (Kim et al, unpublished). These behavioral and neurochemical alterations are accepted indications of opiate-like “withdrawal” in animal models.

Dopaminergic Adaptation and Signs of Sensitization

An opioid system in the ventral midbrain is partially responsible for stimulating DA cells during the consumption of highly palatable foods., In various parts of the striatum, sugar bingeing results in an increase in DA binding to D1 receptors coupled with a decrease in D2-receptor binding. This may occur because each binge releases DA sufficiently to raise extracellular levels to about 123% of baseline., Unlike typical feeding patterns, DA release in response to binge eating does not diminish with repeated meals, as normally seen with food that is no longer novel., As seen in Figure 2, the restriction-refeeding conditions imposed by our laboratory model of binge eating cause a surge of DA, even after 21 days of daily exposure. Repeated surges of DA may alter the gene production and intracellular signaling mechanisms of postsynaptic neurons, presumably leading to neural adaptations that compensate for excessive DA stimulation.

FIGURE 2 

Rats with intermittent access to sugar release DA in response to drinking sucrose for 60 minutes on day 21. DA, as measured by in vivo microdialysis, increases for the daily intermittent sucrose and chow rats (open circles) on days 1, 2, and 21; in contrast,

Repeated psychostimulant activation of the mesolimbic DA system causes behavioral sensitization. Evidence suggests that the mesolimbic DA system is also altered by sugar bingeing. An amphetamine challenge causes locomotor hyperactivity in rats with a history of bingeing on sugar. The effect occurred 9 days after the rats stopped bingeing, suggesting that changes in DA function are long lasting. Conversely, when rats are sensitized by daily injections of amphetamine, they show hyperactivity 10 days later when they drink sugar. We interpret this to mean that sugar bingeing and amphetamine injections sensitize the same DA system, resulting in behavioral cross-sensitization.

Abstinence-induced Signs of Increased Motivation

Other long-lasting effects of sugar bingeing include a) enhanced lever pressing for sugar after 2 weeks of abstinence, b) enhanced voluntary alcohol intake in rats with a history of sugar-bingeing, and c) enhanced responding for sugar-associated cues. These phenomena are referred to as the sugar “deprivation effect,” the alcohol “gateway effect,” and cue “incubation effect,” respectively. They all occur during abstinence, weeks after daily sugar bingeing stopped. Because they are seen during abstinence, it is tempting to categorize them as signs of “craving.” Conservatively, they can be viewed as signs of enhanced motivation, which is integral to relapse to substance abuse.,,

In summary, sugar has the addictive-like properties of both a psychostimulant and an opiate. Cross-sensitization with amphetamine is clearly dopaminergic and important in some stages of addiction. The naloxone-induced withdrawal and abstinence-induced incubation of responding for sugar-associated cues have opioid components. This leads to the suggestion that sugar bingeing results in behavioral and neurochemical signs of excessive dopaminergic and opioid stimulation, which contribute to long-term changes in motivational behavior (Fig. 1).

Compulsion and life-disruptive consequences are evident in some people who suffer from binge eating disorder, bulimia nervosa, or obesity; thus, some people may be “dependent” by Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders criteria. This raises the obvious question: do they have a food addiction? The animal model discussed above suggests it is possible that some binge eaters and bulimics could be addicted to sugar, but this does not explain all eating disorders or obesity although much has been published on this highly speculative topic.

WHICH FOODS ARE POTENTIALLY ADDICTIVE? THERE IS SOMETHING SPECIAL ABOUT SUGAR

Sugar

There is more to food addiction than food restriction and bingeing. The type of nutrient that the animal ingests is also important. Our studies of food addiction have largely focused on sugar (sucrose or glucose). The positive results may relate to sugar as a special nutrient. It has its own receptor system in the tongue,, the intestines,, the liver, pancreas, and brain. Glucoreceptors provide life-saving information to the ingestive behavior system and its associated learning, emotion, and motivational systems. In all probability, sugar addiction in rats is engendered by excessive, repeated activation of this pervasive sugar sensory system.

Saccharin and Sweet-taste

It would be interesting to test artificial sweeteners to see whether the oral component of sweetness is sufficient to produce dependency. We used 12-hour intermittent access to chow and 0.1% saccharin solution to simulate the taste of a “diet soft drink.” After 8 days of this dietary regimen, animals were deprived of food and saccharin for 36 hours, with somatic signs related to anxiety scored every 12 hours. Depriving the rats of food and saccharin led to increased instances of teeth chattering, head shakes, and forepaw tremors over the 36hour period. This aversive state was readily counteracted by 5 mg/kg of morphine or access to a saccharin solution (Hoebel and McCarthy, unpublished). Thus, we suspect that scheduled saccharin binges may stimulate dopamine and opioid-induced dependency, much like the case with sucrose. This is not surprising, given extensive research in the Carroll laboratory suggesting that saccharin can be a substitute for cocaine, and saccharin preference is a marker for addiction liability., Further support for the extreme reinforcing value of saccharin, and its relation to addiction, comes from Ahmed and coworkers, who have shown that some rats prefer saccharin to cocaine self-administration.

Another way to test the power of the sweetness of sugar without the concomitant calories is to purge the stomach by opening a gastric fistula while rats drink 10% sucrose. As one would expect, sham drinkers consume excessive amounts of sugar because of the relative lack of satiety signals. After 3 weeks of sham-binge eating, the taste of a sham-meal of sucrose will still increase extracellular DA to 131% of baseline.

Postingestive Carbohydrates

Real sucrose intake is probably more addictive than either saccharin or sham intake, because extensive evidence shows that intestinal glucose receptors and other postingestional factors are important for the sugar reward that is manifested in conditioned taste preference. Flavors associated with intragastric feeding are preferred, and they release accumbens DA. We conclude on the basis of these conditioning studies that carbohydrate postingestive cues could contribute to the DA or opioid release that is triggered by sugar during acquisition, maintenance, and reinstatement of a binge.

A Surprising Feature of Fat

We were surprised by our inability to obtain naloxone-induced anxiety using the plus-maze test as an indication of a withdrawal state in rats on a high-fat diet. Withdrawal failed to emerge in rats given vegetable fat (Crisco) along with standard chow pellets, or given a nutritionally complete diet of high-sucrose, high-fat pellets. Both the pure vegetable fat and the high-fat pellets were consumed avidly on a binge-inducing schedule. Either the animals were not dependent on the fat or it was a type of addiction that does not cause opiate-like withdrawal. In terms of withdrawal, fat may be to sugar as cocaine is to heroin; that is to say, there are fewer observable behavioral manifestation of withdrawal with cocaine compared with heroin and similarly, fat compared with sugar. Because of this, we have been biased toward looking for signs of opiate-like withdrawal in rats bingeing on sugar. If the opioid system is not perturbed to a significant degree in rats bingeing on fat, then opiate-like withdrawal signs will not emerge. Although it is clear that sugar releases opioids that prolong a meal,, fat might not be effective in this way. Fat is less satiating than carbohydrate, calorie for calorie, but sugar may actually suppresses satiety, just as it can suppress pain and discomfort in general., We have also speculated that fat-stimulated peptides such as galanin, which show increased mRNA expression in response to a high-fat meal and also inhibit some opioid systems, might thus reduce sugar-stimulated opioid-based withdrawal. Thus, although fat does not seem to produce opioid-based dependence, it may still be addictive, but in a way that we have not yet measured.

IS THERE A LINK BETWEEN BINGE EATING AND OBESITY? IT DEPENDS ON THE DIET

Sucrose or Glucose Bingeing, Alone, Does Not Cause Obesity

In terms of overall body weight, some studies have found that bingeing on fat or sugar does not result in weight dysregulation,, whereas others have shown an increase in body weight. In our laboratory, rats that binge on glucose or sucrose show many of the same signs as animals taking drugs of abuse, as described above, and serve as animal models of sugar addiction, but they compensate for the sugar calories by eating less chow and thus control their body weight., A control group with ad libitum access to sugar also compensates for their caloric intake such that they do not become obese.

Sweet-fat Bingeing Does Increase Body Weight

Although animals bingeing on a 10% sugar solution demonstrate an ability to regulate their body weight, those that are maintained on a similar bingeing diet, but with a sweet, high-fat food source, do show weight gain. Animals that were given 2-hour access to this palatable diet showed bingeing patterns, even though they had ad libitum access to a nutritionally complete diet for the remainder of the day. Body weight increased because of the large binge meals, and then it decreased between binges as a result of self-restricted intake of standard chow. However, despite these daily fluctuations in body weight, the animals with access to sweet-fat chow every day gained significantly more weight than the control group with ad libitum access to standard chow. This could lend insight to the connection between binge eating and obesity.

Low Basal Dopamine

To test the theory that some obese people are food addicts, we need obese rats. Extensive work in the Pothos laboratory shows that inbred obesity-prone rats and obese cafeteria-diet rats have low basal DA and impaired DA release. This is thought to have underlying causes related, in part, to weight-related changes in insulin and leptin sensitivity in the control of DA cell firing., We know thatunderweight rats on a restricted diet also have low basal DA. Thus, it seems that both high- and low-weight animals may be hyperphagic as a means of restoring their extracellular DA level. This is analogous to rats self-administering cocaine in a manner that keeps their DA elevated. In fact, sugar-bingeing rats that are food restricted to the point of weight loss release more DA than usual when allowed to binge again, and thus they would raise their own DA level.

A SIMPLIFIED NEURAL CIRCUIT MODEL OF ACCUMBENS FUNCTION

Given that sugar dependency, like obesity, is related both to basal DA levels and to food-induced release of DA, we need a model depicting the role of DA circuitry in behavioral motivation. One would expect this circuit to interact with opioid systems. We have proposed a model in which the NAc has separate GABA outputs for motivation that are similar to the well-documented outputs in the dorsal striatum for locomotion. Just as neurotransmitter imbalance in the motor system leads to Huntington Chorea and Parkinson disease,, neurotransmitter imbalance in the accumbens may be related to general motivational hyperactivity and depression. Specific instances may be manifest as hyperphagia and anorexia. Taking our clues from the extensive Parkinson disease literature, we propose that there is an accumbens GABA output pathway that is specialized for positive, “go” motivation (“approach”), including learned approach and appetitive behavior, and another for negative, “no-go” motivation (“avoidance”), including learned aversion., Focusing on the shell, the approach pathway would be the “direct path” with dynorphin and Substance P as cotransmitters. The avoidance path presumably uses enkephalin as a cotransmitter and takes an “indirect path” to the thalamus and ventral midbrain. Cortex-striatal-pallidum-thalamus-cortex loops may circle around several times in a spiral, leading from cognitive processes to motor activity. Striatal-midbrain pathways have also been described as a spiral, with the shell influencing the core, which influences the medial striatum and then the dorsallateral striatum. This brings the ventral midbrain with its ascending DA and GABA neurons into the schema for cognition to be transformed into action. Directly or indirectly the accumbens outputs also reach the hypothalamus. In the lateral hypothalamus, glutamate inputs initiate eating and GABA stops it. This was shown by both microinjection and our microdialysis studies.,

As shown in Figure 3, DA input from the midbrain to the NAc may act to stimulate approach and inhibit avoidance, thus fostering behavior repetition. Excitation is envisioned via D1 receptors on the GABA-dynorphin “approach” neurons and inhibition via D2 types on the GABA-enkephalin “avoidance” neurons. Indeed, local D2 stimulation can induce signs of aversion, such as gaping and chin rubbing. DA acting via D2 receptors reduces GABA striatal-pallidum neuron responsiveness to glutamate and promotes long-term depression of glutamatergic transmission. D1 receptors are reported to promote responses to strong-coordinated gluta-mate input and long-term potentiation, at least in the GABA neurons that project to the nigra., D1 receptors in the caudate potentiated reward-related eye movements, and again, D2-receptor function was the opposite. This provides support for the schema shown in Figure 3 to the extent that the accumbens shell is organized along lines similar to the dorsal striatum. There are different views expressed in the literature describing the paths from the accumbens to the pallidum, nigra, and the hypothalamus. Each may have different functions with regard to acquisition and expression of conditioned responses and instrumental performance. Within the accumbens, the shell and core must be distinguished, in terms of both their functions and their action sequence. Moreover, subsecond measurements by in vivo voltammetry show DA release within “microenvironments” of the accumbens may vary with functionally specific subpopulations of DA inputs.

FIGURE 3 

Simplified diagram showing opposing DA and ACh influences on dual GABA outputs that are theoretically associated with approach behavior and avoidance behavior. The left side of the diagram represents the nucleus accumbens. Note that the DA input on the

DA surges in response to drugs of abuse cause downstream changes, such as postsynaptic, intracellular accumulation of Delta FosB, which could alter gene production for receptors and other cellular components as a form of compensation; this could then foster restorative reinstatement of drug taking during abstinence. We suggest that if this cascade of intracellular changes can occur in response to drugs of abuse, it might also occur when repeated surges of DA are caused by sugar bingeing., This hypothesis is supported by recent evidence showing that natural reinforcers, such as sucrose and sexual behavior, alter Delta FosB expression in the NAc.

Acetylcholine interneurons may act as an opponent process to halt behavior by doing the opposite of DA at some accumbens synapses as suggested in Figure 3. ACh theoretically inhibits appetitive approach and stimulates the aversion-avoidance path; this could be due to synaptic effects at muscarinic M2 and M1 receptors, respectively (Fig. 3). Numerous studies in the rat support the view that accumbens ACh interneurons inhibit behavior, including the inhibition of feeding behavior and cocaine intake.,,, A muscarinic agonist applied locally to the accumbens can cause behavioral depression in the swim test and a relatively specific M1 antagonist alleviates depression. Dynorphin and other transmitters also enter into the control of this system with depression as one of the outcomes. A conditioned taste aversion releases ACh and neostigmine, used to raise local ACh levels, is sufficient to engender an aversion to a flavor that was previously paired with the cholinergic injection. This suggests that excessive ACh can cause an aversive state that is manifest as a conditioned taste aversion. The possible actions of other muscarinic and nicotinic drugs in the accumbens do not fit our model,, and are discussed elsewhere in light of the possibility that some muscarinic agonists release DA and some muscarinic antagonists may act via M2 receptors to release ACh., ACh interneurons may be inhibited by DA via D2 receptors, as reviewed by Surmeier et al. This suggestion fits with Figure 3, which indicates that less ACh release would reduce activity in the “avoidance pathway” and promote “approach.”

Having suggested that surges of DA caused by sugar bingeing might act via known mechanisms to promote addiction, it is cogent to note that sham feeding, which can reduce ACh satiety signals, would make the overall accumbens response even more like the DA response one sees with some drugs of abuse such as opiates and alcohol. It is tempting to speculate that this translates to human binge-purge disorder as seen in bulimia. Sugar bingeing and purging, according to the rat experiments, would produce DA release that is uninhibited by ACh in the accumbens.

The accumbens GABA outputs, under the opposing influences of DA and ACh, participate in the control of lateral hypothalamic glutamate and GABA release. Rada’s group has new data showing that the accumbens GABA output cells have muscarinic receptors, and that a muscarinic agonist injected in the NAc causes significant changes in glutamate and GABA release in the lateral hypothalamus (Rada et al, unpublished). This is consistent with microdialysis and local injection evidence that lateral hypothalamic glutamate is involved in starting a meal and GABA in stopping it.,, Thus, the model is supported by evidence that accumbens outputs participate in the control of hypothalamic feeding and satiety systems. In the accumbens, DA and ACh may start and stop the motivation to eat by controlling these functions through glutamate and GABA release in the hypothalamus. Clearly, this is an oversimplification, but it is a theory that our data currently support and may, therefore, be part of the larger picture that will eventually emerge.

CONCLUSIONS

This article summarizes data suggesting that, repeated, excessive sugar intake can lead to changes in brain and behavior that are remarkably similar to the effects of drugs of abuse. Thus, sugar may be addictive under special circumstances. On the other hand, bingeing on fat, or even sweet-fat, has given negative results as far as withdrawal is concerned, suggesting that different neural systems are involved. A high-fat diet, if rats binge on it every day, can lead to extra weight gain. Rats prone to obesity on a high-fat diet show low-basal DA levels in the NAc, as do underweight rats, suggesting that both may overeat opportunistically in a manner that restores DA levels. Surges of binge-induced DA may be partially responsible for the neural adaptations manifest as locomotor sensitization and abstinence-induced enhancement of motivation for the food. Opioids are another important part of the picture, but the exact system is not known, because opioids can induce feeding in many brain regions. It seems that opioids may be responsible for the withdrawal signs and for abstinence-induced incubation of cue-induced relapse. ACh in the NAc is one of the countervailing forces in this process. Sugar bingeing seems to postpone ACh release, and sham feeding greatly attenuates it. This is all consistent with a model in which DA stimulates approach and inhibits avoidance outputs in the NAc. ACh does the opposite, unless it is circumvented by drugs of abuse, sugar bingeing, or purging.

Acknowledgments

Supported by USPHS Grants DA10608, MH65024, and AA12882 (to BGH) and fellowship DK-079793 (to NMA).

REFERENCES

1. Satel SL. What should we expect from drug abusers? Psychiatr Serv. 1999;50:861. [PubMed]
2. Leshner AI. Addiction is a brain disease, and it matters. Science. 1997;278:45–47. [PubMed]
3. Bancroft J, Vukadinovic Z. Sexual addiction, sexual compulsivity, sexual impulsivity, or what? Toward a theoretical model. J Sex Res. 2004;41:225–234. [PubMed]
4. Comings DE, Gade-Andavolu R, Gonzalez N, et al. The additive effect of neurotransmitter genes in pathological gambling. Clin Genet. 2001;60:107–116. [PubMed]
5. Foddy B, Savulescu J. Addiction is not an affliction: addictive desires are merely pleasure-oriented desires. Am J Bioeth. 2007;7:29–32. [PubMed]
6. Lowe MR, Butryn ML. Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite? Physiol Behav. 2007;91:432–439. [PubMed]
7. Petry NM. Should the scope of addictive behaviors be broadened to include pathological gambling? Addiction. 2006;101(suppl 1):152–160. [PubMed]
8. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association; Washington, DC: 2000.
9. Nelson JE, Pearson HW, Sayers M, et al., editors. Guide to Drug Abuse Research Terminology. National Institute on Drug Abuse; Rockville: 1982.
10. O’Brien CP, Volkow N, Li TK. What’s in a word? Addiction versus dependence in DSM-V. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:764–765. [PubMed]
11. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Evidence of sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:20–39. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
12. Hoebel BG, Rada P, Mark GP, et al. Neural systems for reinforcement and inhibition of behavior: relevance to eating, addiction, and depression. In: Kahneman D, Diener E, Schwartz N, editors. Well-being: the Foundations of Hedonic Psychology. Russell Sage Foundation; New York: 1999. pp. 558–572.
13. Holderness CC, Brooks-Gunn J, Warren MP. Co-morbidity of eating disorders and substance abuse review of the literature. Int J Eat Disord. 1994;16:1–34. [PubMed]
14. Lienard Y, Vamecq J. The auto-addictive hypothesis of pathological eating disorders. Presse Med. 2004;23(suppl 18):33–40. (in French) [PubMed]
15. Volkow ND, Wise RA. How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat Neurosci. 2005;8:555–560. [PubMed]
16. Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Thanos PK, et al. Similarity between obesity and drug addiction as assessed by neurofunctional imaging: a concept review. J Addict Dis. 2004;23:39–53. [PubMed]
17. Colantuoni C, McCarthy J, Gibbs G, et al. Repeatedly restricted food access combined with highly palatable diet leads to opiate-like withdrawal symptoms during food deprivation in rats. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1997;23:517.
18. Colantuoni C, McCarthy J, Hoebel BG. Evidence for food addiction in rats. Appetite. 1997;29:391–392.
19. Avena N, Rada P, Hoebel B. Unit 9.23C Sugar bingeing in rats. In: Crawley J, Gerfen C, Rogawski M, et al., editors. Current Protocols in Neurosci. Wiley; Indianapolis: 2006. pp. 9.23C. 21–29.23C. 26.
20. Avena NM. Examining the addictive-like properties of binge eating using an animal model of sugar dependence. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2007;15:481–491. [PubMed]
21. Colantuoni C, Rada P, McCarthy J, et al. Evidence that intermittent, excessive sugar intake causes endogenous opioid dependence. Obes Res. 2002;10:478–488. [PubMed]
22. Spangler R, Wittkowski KM, Goddard NL, et al. Opiate-like effects of sugar on gene expression in reward areas of the rat brain. Brain Res Mol Brain Res. 2004;124:134–142. [PubMed]
23. Colantuoni C, Schwenker J, McCarthy J, et al. Excessive sugar intake alters binding to dopamine and mu-opioid receptors in the brain. Neuroreport. 2001;12:3549–3552. [PubMed]
24. Avena NM, Bocarsly ME, Rada P, et al. After daily bingeing on a sucrose solution, prolonged food deprivation induces anxiety and accumbens dopamine/acetylcholine imbalance. Physiol Behav. 2008;94:309–315. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
25. Schulteis G, Yackey M, Risbrough V, et al. Anxiogenic-like effects of spontaneous and naloxone-precipitated opiate withdrawal in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1998;60:727–731. [PubMed]
26. Sahr AE, Sindelar DK, Alexander-Chacko JT, et al. Activation of mesolimbic dopamine neurons during novel and daily limited access to palatable food is blocked by the opioid antagonist LY255582. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2008;295:R463–R471. [PubMed]
27. Tanda G, Di Chiara G. A dopamine-mu1 opioid link in the rat ventral tegmentum shared by palatable food (Fonzies) and non-psychostimulant drugs of abuse. Eur J Neurosci. 1998;10:1179–1187. [PubMed]
28. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Underweight rats have enhanced dopamine release and blunted acetylcholine response in the nucleus accumbens while bingeing on sucrose. Neuroscience. 2008;156:865–871. 2008. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
29. Rada P, Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly releases dopamine in the accumbens shell. Neuroscience. 2005;134:737–744. [PubMed]
30. Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Modulation of feeding-induced activation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by appetitive stimuli and its relation to motivational state. Eur J Neurosci. 1999;11:4389–4397. [PubMed]
31. Nestler EJ, Aghajanian GK. Molecular and cellular basis of addiction. Science. 1997;278:58–63. [PubMed]
32. Imperato A, Obinu MC, Carta G, et al. Reduction of dopamine release and synthesis by repeated amphetamine treatment: role in behavioral sensitization. Eur J Pharmacol. 1996;317:231–237. [PubMed]
33. Narendran R, Martinez D. Cocaine abuse and sensitization of striatal dopamine transmission: a critical review of the preclinical and clinical imaging literature. Synapse. 2008;62:851–869. [PubMed]
34. Unterwald EM, Kreek MJ, Cuntapay M. The frequency of cocaine administration impacts cocaine-induced receptor alterations. Brain Res. 2001;900:103–109. [PubMed]
35. Vanderschuren LJ, Kalivas PW. Alterations in dopaminergic and glutamatergic transmission in the induction and expression of behavioral sensitization: a critical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2000;151:99–120. [PubMed]
36. Vezina P. Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity and the self-administration of psychomotor stimulant drugs. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27:827–839. [PubMed]
37. Avena NM, Hoebel BG. A diet promoting sugar dependency causes behavioral cross-sensitization to a low dose of amphetamine. Neuroscience. 2003;122:17–20. [PubMed]
38. Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Amphetamine-sensitized rats show sugar-induced hyperactivity (cross-sensitization) and sugar hyperphagia. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2003;74:635–639. [PubMed]
39. Avena NM, Long KA, Hoebel BG. Sugar-dependent rats show enhanced responding for sugar after abstinence: evidence of a sugar deprivation effect. Physiol Behav. 2005;84:359–362. [PubMed]
40. Avena NM, Carrillo CA, Needham L, et al. Sugar-dependent rats show enhanced intake of unsweetened ethanol. Alcohol. 2004;34:203–209. [PubMed]
41. Grimm JW, Fyall AM, Osincup DP. Incubation of sucrose craving: effects of reduced training and sucrose pre-loading. Physiol Behav. 2005;84:73–79. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
42. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Neurobiology of Addiction. Elsevier; Amsterdam: 2006.
43. Weiss F. Neurobiology of craving, conditioned reward and relapse. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2005;5:9–19. [PubMed]
44. Grimm JW, Manaois M, Osincup D, et al. Naloxone attenuates incubated sucrose craving in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;194:537–544. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
45. Davis C, Claridge G. The eating disorders as addiction: a psychobiological perspective. Addict Behav. 1998;23:463–475. [PubMed]
46. Gillman MA, Lichtigfeld FJ. The opioids, dopamine, cholecystokinin, and eating disorders. Clin Neuropharmacol. 1986;9:91–97. [PubMed]
47. Heubner H. Eating Disorders and Other Addictive Behaviors. W. W. Norton; New York: 1993. Endorphins.
48. Marrazzi MA, Luby ED. The neurobiology of anorexia nervosa: an auto-addiction? In: Cohen M, Foa P, editors. The Brain as an Endocrine Organ. Spinger-Verlag; New York: 1990. pp. 46–95.
49. Mercer ME, Holder MD. Food cravings, endogenous opioid peptides, and food intake: a review. Appetite. 1997;29:325–352. [PubMed]
50. Riva G, Bacchetta M, Cesa G, et al. Is severe obesity a form of addiction? Rationale, clinical approach, and controlled clinical trial. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2006;9:457–479. [PubMed]
51. Chandrashekar J, Hoon MA, Ryba NJ, et al. The receptors and cells for mammalian taste. Nature. 2006;444:288–294. [PubMed]
52. Scott K. Taste recognition: food for thought. Neuron. 2005;48:455–464. [PubMed]
53. Mei N. Intestinal chemosensitivity. Physiol Rev. 1985;65:211–237. [PubMed]
54. Oomura Y, Yoshimatsu H. Neural network of glucose monitoring system. J Auton Nerv Syst. 1984;10:359–372. [PubMed]
55. Yamaguchi N. Sympathoadrenal system in neuroendocrine control of glucose: mechanisms involved in the liver, pancreas, and adrenal gland under hemorrhagic and hypoglycemic stress. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1992;70:167–206. [PubMed]
56. Levin BE. Metabolic sensing neurons and the control of energy homeostasis. Physiol Behav. 2006;89:486–489. [PubMed]
57. ME Carroll, Morgan AD, Anker JJ, et al. Selective breeding for differential saccharin intake as an animal model of drug abuse. Behav Pharmacol. 2008;19:435–460. [PubMed]
58. Morgan AD, Dess NK, Carroll ME. Escalation of intravenous cocaine self-administration, progressive-ratio performance, and reinstatement in rats selectively bred for high (HiS) and low (LoS) saccharin intake. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2005;178:41–51. [PubMed]
59. Lenoir M, Serre F, Cantin L, et al. Intense sweetness surpasses cocaine reward. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e698. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
60. Sclafani A, Ackroff K. The relationship between food reward and satiation revisited. Physiol Behav. 2004;82:89–95. [PubMed]
61. Avena NM, Rada P, Moise N, et al. Sucrose sham feeding on a binge schedule releases accumbens dopamine repeatedly and eliminates the acetylcholine satiety response. Neuroscience. 2006;139:813–820. [PubMed]
62. Myers KP, Sclafani A. Conditioned enhancement of flavor evaluation reinforced by intragastric glucose. I. Intake acceptance and preference analysis. Physiol Behav. 2001;74:481–493. [PubMed]
63. Sclafani A, Nissenbaum JW, Ackroff K. Learned preferences for real-fed and sham-fed polycose in rats: interaction of taste, postingestive reinforcement, and satiety. Physiol Behav. 1994;56:331–337. [PubMed]
64. Hajnal A, Smith GP, Norgren R. Oral sucrose stimulation increases accumbens dopamine in the rat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2004;286:R31–R37. [PubMed]
65. Mark GP, Smith SE, Rada PV, et al. An appetitively conditioned taste elicits a preferential increase in mesolimbic dopamine release. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994;48:651–660. [PubMed]
66. Sclafani A. Sweet taste signaling in the gut. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:14887–14888. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
67. Yu WZ, Silva RM, Sclafani A, et al. Pharmacology of flavor preference conditioning in sham-feeding rats: effects of dopamine receptor antagonists. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;65:635–647. [PubMed]
68. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Sugar vs. fat bingeing: differential implications for addictive-like behaviors. J Nutr. In press.
69. Sclafani A, Aravich P, Xenakis S. Dopaminergic and endorphinergic mediation of a sweet reward. In: Hoebel BG, Novin D, editors. The Neural Basis of Feeding and Reward. Haer Institute for Electrophysiological Research; Brunswick: 1982. pp. 507–516.
70. Siviy S, Calcagnetti D, Reid L. Opioids and palatability. In: Hoebel BG, Novin D, editors. The Neural Basis of Feeding and Reward. Haer Institute for Electrophysiological Research; Brunswick: 1982. pp. 517–524.
71. Blass E, Fitzgerald E, Kehoe P. Interactions between sucrose, pain and isolation distress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1987;26:483–489. [PubMed]
71. Blass EM, Shah A. Pain-reducing properties of sucrose in human newborns. Chem Senses. 1995;20:29–35. [PubMed]
73. Hawes JJ, Brunzell DH, Narasimhaiah R, et al. Galanin protects against behavioral and neurochemical correlates of opiate reward. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2008;33:1864–1873. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
74. Boggiano MM, Chandler PC, Viana JB, et al. Combined dieting and stress evoke exaggerated responses to opioids in binge-eating rats. Behav Neurosci. 2005;119:1207–1214. [PubMed]
75. Corwin RL, Wojnicki FH, Fisher JO, et al. Limited access to a dietary fat option affects ingestive behavior but not body composition in male rats. Physiol Behav. 1998;65:545–553. [PubMed]
76. Dimitriou SG, Rice HB, Corwin RL. Effects of limited access to a fat option on food intake and body composition in female rats. Int J Eat Disord. 2000;28:436–445. [PubMed]
77. Cottone P, Sabino V, Steardo L, et al. Opioid-dependent anticipatory negative contrast and binge-like eating in rats with limited access to highly preferred food. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:524–535. [PubMed]
78. Toida S, Takahashi M, Shimizu H, et al. Effect of high sucrose feeding on fat accumulation in the male Wistar rat. Obes Res. 1996;4:561–568. [PubMed]
79. Wideman CH, Nadzam GR, Murphy HM. Implications of an animal model of sugar addiction, withdrawal and relapse for human health. Nutr Neurosci. 2005;8:269–276. [PubMed]
80. Berner LA, Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Obesity. 2008. Bingeing, self-restriction, and increased body weight in rats with limited access to a sweet-fat diet. epub ahead of print. [PubMed]
81. Stunkard AJ. Eating patterns and obesity. Psychiatr Q. 1959;33:284–295. [PubMed]
82. Geiger BM, Behr GG, Frank LE, et al. Evidence for defective mesolimbic dopamine exocytosis in obesity-prone rats. FASEB J. 2008;22:2740–2746. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
83. Baskin DG, Figlewicz Lattemann D, Seeley RJ, et al. Insulin and leptin: dual adiposity signals to the brain for the regulation of food intake and body weight. Brain Res. 1999;848:114–123. [PubMed]
84. Palmiter RD. Is dopamine a physiologically relevant mediator of feeding behavior? Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:375–381. [PubMed]
85. Pothos EN, Creese I, Hoebel BG. Restricted eating with weight loss selectively decreases extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and alters dopamine response to amphetamine, morphine, and food intake. J Neurosci. 1995;15:6640–6650. [PubMed]
86. Wise RA, Newton P, Leeb K, et al. Fluctuations in nucleus accumbens dopamine concentration during intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1995;120:10–20. [PubMed]
87. Hoebel BG, Avena NM, Rada P. Accumbens dopamine-acetylcholine balance in approach and avoidance. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2007;7:617–627. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
88. Rivlin-Etzion M, Marmor O, Heimer G, et al. Basal ganglia oscillations and pathophysiology of movement disorders. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2006;16:629–637. [PubMed]
89. Utter AA, Basso MA. The basal ganglia: an overview of circuits and function. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;32:333–342. [PubMed]
90. Steiner H, Gerfen CR. Role of dynorphin and enkephalin in the regulation of striatal output pathways and behavior. Exp Brain Res. 1998;123:60–76. [PubMed]
91. Hoebel BG, Avena NM, Rada P. An accumbens dopamine-acetylcho-line system for approach and avoidance. In: Elliot A, editor. The Handbook of Approach and Avoidance. Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates; Mahwah, NJ: 2008. pp. 89–107.
92. Everitt BJ, Belin D, Economidou D, et al. Neural mechanisms underlying the vulnerability to develop compulsive drug-seeking habits and addiction. Philos Trans R Soc London B Biol Sci. 2008;363:3125–3135. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
93. Haber SN, Fudge JL, McFarland NR. Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. J Neurosci. 2000;20:2369–2382. [PubMed]
94. Kelley AE, Baldo BA, Pratt WE. A proposed hypothalamic-thalamicstriatal axis for the integration of energy balance, arousal, and food reward. J Comp Neurol. 2005;493:72–85. [PubMed]
95. Rada P, Mendialdua A, Hernandez L, et al. Extracellular glutamate increases in the lateral hypothalamus during meal initiation, and GABA peaks during satiation: microdialysis measurements every 30 s. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:222–227. [PubMed]
96. Stanley BG, Willett VL, 3rd, Donias HW, et al. The lateral hypothalamus: a primary site mediating excitatory amino acid-elicited eating. Brain Res. 1993;630:41–49. [PubMed]
97. Sederholm F, Johnson AE, Brodin U, et al. Dopamine D(2) receptors and ingestive behavior: brainstem mediates inhibition of intraoral intake and accumbens mediates aversive taste behavior in male rats. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2002;160:161–169. [PubMed]
98. Surmeier DJ, Ding J, Day M, et al. D1 and D2 dopamine-receptor modulation of striatal glutamatergic signaling in striatal medium spiny neurons. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:228–235. [PubMed]
99. See RE, McLaughlin J, Fuchs RA. Muscarinic receptor antagonism in the basolateral amygdala blocks acquisition of cocaine-stimulus association in a model of relapse to cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Neurosci. 2003;117:477–483. [PubMed]
100. Shen W, Flajolet M, Greengard P, et al. Dichotomous dopaminergic control of striatal synaptic plasticity. Science. 2008;321:848–851. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
101. Nakamura K, Hikosaka O. Role of dopamine in the primate caudate nucleus in reward modulation of saccades. J Neurosci. 2006;26:5360–5369. [PubMed]
102. Ahn S, Phillips AG. Dopamine efflux in the nucleus accumbens during within-session extinction, outcome-dependent, and habit-based instrumental responding for food reward. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2007;191:641–651. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
103. Mingote S, Pereira M, Farrar AM, et al. Systemic administration of the adenosine A(2A) agonist CGS 21680 induces sedation at doses that suppress lever pressing and food intake. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;89:345–351. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
104. Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. Reward-guided learning beyond dopamine in the nucleus accumbens: the integrative functions of cortico-basal ganglia networks. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;28:1437–1448. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
105. Bassareo V, De Luca MA, Di Chiara G. Differential expression of motivational stimulus properties by dopamine in nucleus accumbens shell versus core and prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2002;22:4709–4719. [PubMed]
106. Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Differential responsiveness of dopamine transmission to food-stimuli in nucleus accumbens shell/core compartments. Neuroscience. 1999;89:637–641. [PubMed]
107. Di Chiara G, Bassareo V. Reward system and addiction: what dopamine does and doesn’t do. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2007;7:69–76. [PubMed]
108. Floresco SB, McLaughlin RJ, Haluk DM. Opposing roles for the nucleus accumbens core and shell in cue-induced reinstatement of food-seeking behavior. Neuroscience. 2008;154:877–884. [PubMed]
109. Richardson NR, Gratton A. Changes in nucleus accumbens dopamine transmission associated with fixed- and variable-time schedule-induced feeding. Eur J Neurosci. 2008;27:2714–2723. [PubMed]
110. Wightman RM, Heien ML, Wassum KM, et al. Dopamine release is heterogeneous within microenvironments of the rat nucleus accumbens. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26:2046–2054. [PubMed]
111. Wallace DL, Vialou V, Rios L, et al. The influence of DeltaFosB in the nucleus accumbens on natural reward-related behavior. J Neurosci. 2008;28:10272–10277. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
112. Mark GP, Kinney AE, Grubb MC, et al. Injection of oxotremorine in nucleus accumbens shell reduces cocaine but not food self-administration in rats. Brain Res. 2006;1123:51–59. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
113. Mark GP, Rada P, Pothos E, et al. Effects of feeding and drinking on acetylcholine release in the nucleus accumbens, striatum, and hippocampus of freely behaving rats. J Neurochem. 1992;58:2269–2274. [PubMed]
114. Chau D, Rada PV, Kosloff RA, et al. Cholinergic, M1 receptors in the nucleus accumbens mediate behavioral depression. A possible downstream target for fluoxetine. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;877:769–774. [PubMed]
115. Nestler EJ, Carlezon WA., Jr. The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:1151–1159. [PubMed]
116. Mark GP, Weinberg JB, Rada PV, et al. Extracellular acetylcholine is increased in the nucleus accumbens following the presentation of an aversively conditioned taste stimulus. Brain Res. 1995;688:184–188. [PubMed]
117. Taylor KM, Davidson K, Mark GP, et al. Conditioned taste aversion induced by increased acetylcholine in the nucleus accumbens. Soc Neurosci. 1992:1066.
118. Ikemoto S, Glazier BS, Murphy JM, et al. Rats self-administer carbachol directly into the nucleus accumbens. Physiol Behav. 1998;63:811–814. [PubMed]
119. Perry ML, Baldo BA, Andrzejewski ME, et al. Muscarinic receptor antagonism causes a functional alteration in nucleus accumbens mu-opiate-mediated feeding behavior. Behav Brain Res. 2009;197:225–229. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
120. Rada P, Paez X, Hernandez L, et al. Microdialysis in the study of behavior reinforcement and inhibition. In: Westerink BH, Creamers T, editors. Handbook of Microdialysis: Methods, Application and Perspectives. Academic Press; New York: 2007. pp. 351–375.
121. Rada P, Mark GP, Pothos E, et al. Systemic morphine simultaneously decreases extracellular acetylcholine and increases dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. Neuropharmacol. 1991;30:1133–1136. [PubMed]
122. Rada P, Johnson DF, Lewis MJ, et al. In alcohol-treated rats, naloxone decreases extracellular dopamine and increases acetylcholine in the nucleus accumbens: evidence of opioid withdrawal. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;79:599–605. [PubMed]
123. Maldonado-Irizarry CS, Swanson CJ, Kelley AE. Glutamate receptors in the nucleus accumbens shell control feeding behavior via the lateral hypothalamus. J Neurosci. 1995;15:6779–6788. [PubMed]
124. Stanley BG, Ha LH, Spears LC, et al. Lateral hypothalamic injections of glutamate, kainic acid, D,L-alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-isoxazole propionic acid or N-methyl-D-aspartic acid rapidly elicit intense transient eating in rats. Brain Res. 1993;613:88–95. [PubMed]