BMI Modulates Calorie-Dependent Dopamine Changes in Accumbens from Glucose Intake (2014)

PLoS One. 2014 Jul 7;9(7):e101585. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0101585.

Wang GJ1, Tomasi D1, Convit A2, Logan J3, Wong CT1, Shumay E1, Fowler JS4, Volkow ND1.

Abstract

Objective

Dopamine mediates the rewarding effects of food that can lead to overeating and obesity, which then trigger metabolic neuroadaptations that further perpetuate excessive food consumption. We tested the hypothesis that the dopamine response to calorie intake (independent of palatability) in striatal brain regions is attenuated with increases in weight.

Method

We used positron emission tomography with [11C]raclopride to measure dopamine changes triggered by calorie intake by contrasting the effects of an artificial sweetener (sucralose) devoid of calories to that of glucose to assess their association with body mass index (BMI) in nineteen healthy participants (BMI range 21–35).

Results

Neither the measured blood glucose concentrations prior to the sucralose and the glucose challenge days, nor the glucose concentrations following the glucose challenge vary as a function of BMI. In contrast the dopamine changes in ventral striatum (assessed as changes in non-displaceable binding potential of [11C]raclopride) triggered by calorie intake (contrast glucose – sucralose) were significantly correlated with BMI (r = 0.68) indicating opposite responses in lean than in obese individuals. Specifically whereas in normal weight individuals (BMI <25) consumption of calories was associated with increases in dopamine in the ventral striatum in obese individuals it was associated with decreases in dopamine.

Conclusion

These findings show reduced dopamine release in ventral striatum with calorie consumption in obese subjects, which might contribute to their excessive food intake to compensate for the deficit between the expected and the actual response to food consumption.

Figures

Citation: Wang G-J, Tomasi D, Convit A, Logan J, Wong CT, et al. (2014) BMI Modulates Calorie-Dependent Dopamine Changes in Accumbens from Glucose Intake. PLoS ONE 9(7): e101585. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0101585

Editor: Sidney Arthur Simon, Duke University Medical Center, United States of America

Received: April 21, 2014; Accepted: June 9, 2014; Published: July 7, 2014

This is an open-access article, free of all copyright, and may be freely reproduced, distributed, transmitted, modified, built upon, or otherwise used by anyone for any lawful purpose. The work is made available under the Creative Commons CC0 public domain dedication.

Data Availability: The authors confirm that all data underlying the findings are fully available without restriction. All data are within the manuscript.

Funding: U. S. Department of Energy OBER: DE-ACO2-76CH00016 for infrastructure support of Brookhaven National Laboratory and Royalty Funds to GJW. National Institute of Health: Z01AA000550 to NDV, R01DK064087-09 to AC, K01DA025280 to ES. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exits.

Introduction

Brain dopamine (DA) modulates eating behaviors through its modulation of reward and incentive salience [1]. Activation of DA in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) occurs with exposure to novel food rewards but with repeated exposures DA increases instead shift to the cues that predict the food reward [2]. The mesolimbic DA system is critical for reinforcing food palatability and highly palatable foods increase DA in NAc [3], whereas DA antagonists attenuate the hedonic value of sucrose [4]. DA also mediates the rewarding effects of food that are driven by energy content [5]. Rodent studies revealed intragastric administration of glucose increased DA in NAc [6], which was an effect dependent on glucose utilization, since administration of an anti-metabolic glucose analog lowered DA. This indicates that DA neurons respond to the energetic value of nutrients independent of taste and implicates postingestive factors in calorie related DA increases in NAc. Moreover in humans neuroimaging studies have shown that sucrose solution but not a non-caloric sweet solution activates the midbrain, which is where DA neurons are located [7]. DA neurons are also activated by visual, auditory and somatosensory stimuli that predict food reward [8]. Excessive food consumption can lead to obesity, which, in turn triggers metabolic adaptations that further perpetuate excessive food consumption. Some of these neuro-adaptations occur in DA pathways as evidenced by clinical and preclinical studies documenting a reduction in DA D2 receptors in striatum with obesity [9].

Here we hypothesized that in obesity the response to calorie consumption would be attenuated just as has been shown for drug consumption in addiction [10][12]. For this purpose we used positron emission tomography (PET) and [11C]raclopride (D2/D3 receptor radiotracer sensitive to competition with endogenous DA) [13] to assess if calorie induced DA increases in ventral striatum (where NAc is located) are dependent on body mass index (BMI). This is possible because [11C]raclopride’s binding to D2/D3 receptors is sensitive to the concentration of endogenous DA; such that when DA levels increase the specific binding of [11C]raclopride decreases and when DA levels decreases [11C]raclopride’s specific binding increases [12], [14]. In order to control for the effects of glucose’s palatability (sweetness), we contrasted the effects of sucralose (artificial sweetener devoid of calories) to those of glucose. Thus the contrast between the two sweet solutions (one with calories and one without calories) enabled us to measure the changes in DA that are attributable to the calories independent of the food’s palatability.

Methods

This study was carried out at Brookhaven National Laboratory (BNL) and the Stony Brook University’s Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects approved the protocol. Written informed consent was obtained from participants prior to study initiation. Nineteen subjects were included in the study if they were right-handed, 40–60 years old, healthy and had 21≤ BMI ≤35 kg/m2. Exclusion criteria included history or presence of any medical condition that may alter cerebral function; diabetes mellitus; present or past history of an Axis I diagnosis (including depression or anxiety disorder) as per DSM IV; eating disorders; alcohol or drug abuse or dependence (including nicotine). Subjects were asked to have their last meal completed by 7 PM the evening before the day of the imaging visits and were scanned between 15 and 17 hours after their last meal. Subjects were informed that blood sugar levels would be checked during the study to help ensure that they refrained from eating.

Study Design

Subjects had two imaging visits: on one day of study (Day A) the subject took a 75 gram oral glucose drink (Trutola, VWR, PA); on the other day (Day B) the subject took an oral placebo drink (sucralose, 0.348 mg/ml [JK Sucralose Inc., NJ] that is of equal volume and sweetness level to the glucose solution). PET started at 10 minutes after completion of the glucose/placebo drink. PET scans were run on a Siemens ECAT HR+ and [11C]raclopride was prepared according to methods published previously [15]. Scans were started immediately after tracer injection of 8 mCi or less of [11C]raclopride and carried out for a total of 60 minutes. Blood samples for glucose levels were obtained prior to the drinks, immediately upon completion of the glucose/placebo drink, then every 5 minutes for 30 minutes, at 60, 90 & 120 minutes. PET was carried out at the approximate same time of day for all subjects. Subjects were asked to fast and stay hydrated overnight (at least 12 hours) prior to the start of any study procedures on each day of imaging study. Days A and B were randomized across subjects. These two scan days were separated between 2–42 days with an average of 16±10 days.

Clinical Scales

The eating behavior questionnaires were obtained during the screening visit using the Three Factors Eating Questionnaire-Eating Inventory (TFEQ-EI) to assess the following three dimensions of eating behavior: cognitive processes; behavioral adaptation; and control and the Gormally Binge Eating Disorder Scale (GBEDS) to look at binge eating behavior and associated psychopathology [16]. To assess the palatability of the glucose and sucralose drinks, the subjects were asked to assess quality of sweetness, sweetness level and likeness of the sweetness using self-reports [rated from 1 (less) to 10 (most)] immediately after they consumed the drinks. Linear regression analysis was used to analyze the association between these self-reports and BMI. Pair t-tests were used to compare differences in these self-reports between the glucose and sucralose drinks.

Blood Glucose Concentration Measurement

Plasma samples were analyzed for glucose concentration using a Beckman Glucose Analyzer 2 (Brea, California), which determines glucose by means of the oxygen rate method employing a Beckman oxygen electrode. A measured volume of sample is pipetted into an enzyme reagent in a cup containing an electrode that responds to and reports oxygen concentration in mg of glucose/100 mL. Paired t-tests were used to analyze the differences in blood glucose levels, independently for each time point. Linear regression analysis was used to assess the association between blood glucose level and BMI.

Data Analysis

The time-activity curves for tissue concentration in striatum and in cerebellum along with the time activity curves for [11C]raclopride were used to calculate the distribution volume (DV) in the pixels for the whole image. Specifically, we estimated for each voxel the DV, which corresponds to the equilibrium measurement of the ratio of the radiotracer’s tissue concentration to that of its plasma concentration using a graphical analysis technique for reversible systems [17]. A custom Montreal Neurological Institute template, which we previously developed using the distribution volume images from 34 healthy subjects acquired with [11C]raclopride and the same scanning sequence, was used for the spatial normalization of the DV images. For the binding potential (BPND) images we normalized the DV in each voxel to that in the cerebellum (left and right regions-of-interest), which correspond to dopamine (DA) D2/D3 receptor availability [17]. The BPND images were then spatially smoothed using an 8-mm Gaussian kernel to minimize the variability of the brain anatomy across subjects. Differences in BPND between glucose and sucralose were used to estimate changes in DA triggered by calories.

Statistical Analyses

Multilinear regression analysis was used to analyze the association between BPND differences between the glucose and the sucralose (ΔBPND), which reflect changes in DA secondary to the calorie content of glucose. The Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8; Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK) was used for this purpose. Statistical significance was set as PFWE <0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons at the voxel level with a familywise error and small volume corrections within a 10-mm radius spherical region-of-interest (ROI). Follow up analyses were conducted on average ROI measures that were extracted using the coordinates obtained from SPM to assess the effect of behavioral measures (comprising scores of cognitive restraint of eating, disinhibition and hunger using TFEQ-EI and the binge eating score using GBEDS), blood glucose levels, age, and gender. Specifically, these variables were correlated with the average ΔBPND signals in the ROI after controlling by BMI. Statistical significance for these correlation analyses was set as P<0.05, uncorrected.

Results

The difference of blood glucose concentration did not vary as a function of BMI after the sucralose and glucose challenge (r<0.18, R2<0.03). There were no differences between the glucose and sucralose drinks on self-reports for quality of sweetness (glucose: 5.4±2.6. sucralose: 5.4±2.6); sweetness level (glucose: 6.8±2.5. sucralose: 6.2±2.5) and likeness of the sweetness (glucose: 4.7±2.8. sucralose: 4.8±3.0) and these self-reports were not influenced by the subject’s BMI. In contrast we observed a significant correlation between calorie-induced DA changes as assessed by ΔBPND (glucose – sucralose) in the ventral striatum (r = 0.68; P_FWE <0.004, P_FDR <0.05, voxels = 131, Fig. 1a) and BMI, such that the lower the BMI the greater the DA increases and the higher the BMI the greater the DA decreases in ventral striatum. The correlation remained significant after covaring with difference of blood glucose concentration (glucose – sucralose) (Fig.1b).

thumbnail

Figure 1. a: SPM images of brain dopamine changes.

Significant activated clusters show dopamine (DA) changes in nucleus accumbens for the contrast glucose > sucralose intake (ΔBPND). Note that increases in BPND reflect DA decreases (less competition from DA for [11C]raclopride to bind to D2/D3 receptors) whereas decreases in BPND reflect DA increases with glucose (as compared to sucralose) The SPM images were superimposed onto T2 weighted MR images in sagittal (left upper), coronal (right upper) and transverse (lower) views. The color bar indicates t-score values. b: Correlation between BMI and brain DA changes. The differences between DRD2 availability after the glucose and the sucralose intake (ΔBPND) were compared with BMI (kg/m2). The leaner subjects showed the largest DRD2 decreases with glucose in nucleus accumbens (consistent with DA increases) whereas the heavier subjects showed DRD2 increases (consistent with DA decreases). ΔBP*: corrected for changes in blood glucose levels (glucose – sucralose) within PET acquisition (0–60min).

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0101585.g001

DA changes in response to caloric intake (ΔBPND) were also significantly correlated with scores on the eating behavioral measures. Specifically, delta BPND in ventral striatum was significantly correlated with the measures of eating behaviors the TEFQ-EI scores of disinhibition (r = 0.52, p<0.02) and hunger (r = 0.6, p<0.006) and the GBES scores of binge eating (r = 0.61, p<0.006), such that subjects with greater scores on disinhibition, perception of hunger and binge eating showed decreases in DA with caloric intake. However, these correlations were not significant after covaring for BMI and sex.

Discussion

In this study, contrasting glucose with sucralose allowed us to assess the effects of calorie consumption in striatal DA signaling after controlling for reward responses associated with palatability. Thus the DA changes in the ventral striatum from this contrast reflect responses from the energy content of glucose consumption. The opposite patterns of DA responses in the ventral striatum in lean individuals who showed DA increases in contrast to the DA decreases observed in obese subjects, might reflect the differences between the expected and the actual response to caloric intake since DA responses are influenced by reward probability distributions [18]. Specifically a reward that is better than predicted elicits an activation of DA neurons and a reward that is worse than predicted induces inhibition [19]. Even though glucose concentrations in blood were similar between lean and obese subjects, the response to the caloric content in the obese subjects would have resulted in less than the predicted response resulting in inhibition of DA neurons and reduced DA release after the glucose drink. However because we did not obtain measures of D2/D3 receptor availability without the administration of a sweetened solution (baseline measure) we cannot rule out the possibility that the abnormal response in the obese subject is also driven by an abnormal response to sweetness and not just an abnormal response to calories.

In mice lacking functional sweet taste receptors sucrose but not an artificial sweetener increased DA in NAc [20], which is consistent with our findings showing DA increases in ventral striatum triggered by caloric ingestion in lean individuals. However such a response was not observed in obese individuals indicating a disruption of the brain DA responses to caloric content.

Greater scores on TEFQ disinhibition are associated with impaired control of food intake [21] and have been linked with worse frontal executive function [21], [22]. They are also consistent with our prior findings showing a significant correlation between food restraint scores and striatal DA increases induced by exposure to food cues [23], thus supporting an association between decreased striatal DA signaling and impaired self-control [24]. The correlation of hunger on the TFEQ with DA change in NAc with caloric provides further evidence for DA’s role in hunger perception in humans [25]. Finally the association between decreases in DA after glucose and greater binge eating scores is reminiscent of the decreases in stimulants induced DA increases in cocaine abusers whose behavior is characterized by compulsive cocaine intake [10], [12], [26]. Though it is tempting to invoke a hypo-responsivity of the DA reward circuit in obese subjects this is an inadequate descriptor; for we specifically observed a hypo-responsivity to the calorie consumption but it is plausible that they might have hyper-responsivity to exposure to food cues. It is therefore more likely that a discrepancy between an enhanced expectation and a reduced response to the calories consumed in the obese person might trigger the drive to continue eating in order to compensate for this deficit.

Acknowledgments

The PET study was carried out at Brookhaven National Laboratory. We thank J. Rotrosen of New York University for subject referral; D. Schlyer and M. Schueller for cyclotron operations; D. Warner, D. Alexoff and P. Vaska for PET operations; C. Shea, Y. Xu, L. Muench and P. King for radiotracer preparation and analysis, K. Torres for study protocol preparation, and B. Hubbard M. Jayne and P. Carter for patient care.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: GJW NDV. Performed the experiments: GJW AC CTW JSF. Analyzed the data: GJW DT JL ES. Contributed to the writing of the manuscript: GJW NDV.

References

  1. 1. Wise RA (2013) Dual roles of dopamine in food and drug seeking: the drive-reward paradox. Biol Psychiatry 73: 819–826. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.09.001
  2. 2. Richardson NR, Gratton A (2008) Changes in nucleus accumbens dopamine transmission associated with fixed- and variable-time schedule-induced feeding. Eur J Neurosci 27: 2714–2723. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06236.x
  3. View Article
  4. PubMed/NCBI
  5. Google Scholar
  6. View Article
  7. PubMed/NCBI
  8. Google Scholar
  9. View Article
  10. PubMed/NCBI
  11. Google Scholar
  12. View Article
  13. PubMed/NCBI
  14. Google Scholar
  15. View Article
  16. PubMed/NCBI
  17. Google Scholar
  18. View Article
  19. PubMed/NCBI
  20. Google Scholar
  21. View Article
  22. PubMed/NCBI
  23. Google Scholar
  24. View Article
  25. PubMed/NCBI
  26. Google Scholar
  27. View Article
  28. PubMed/NCBI
  29. Google Scholar
  30. View Article
  31. PubMed/NCBI
  32. Google Scholar
  33. 3. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ (2010) Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat Neurosci 13: 635–641. doi: 10.1038/nn.2519
  34. View Article
  35. PubMed/NCBI
  36. Google Scholar
  37. View Article
  38. PubMed/NCBI
  39. Google Scholar
  40. View Article
  41. PubMed/NCBI
  42. Google Scholar
  43. View Article
  44. PubMed/NCBI
  45. Google Scholar
  46. View Article
  47. PubMed/NCBI
  48. Google Scholar
  49. View Article
  50. PubMed/NCBI
  51. Google Scholar
  52. View Article
  53. PubMed/NCBI
  54. Google Scholar
  55. View Article
  56. PubMed/NCBI
  57. Google Scholar
  58. View Article
  59. PubMed/NCBI
  60. Google Scholar
  61. View Article
  62. PubMed/NCBI
  63. Google Scholar
  64. View Article
  65. PubMed/NCBI
  66. Google Scholar
  67. View Article
  68. PubMed/NCBI
  69. Google Scholar
  70. View Article
  71. PubMed/NCBI
  72. Google Scholar
  73. 4. Vigorito M, Kruse CB, Carretta JC (1994) Differential sensitivity of operant behaviors to changes in the concentration of a sucrose reinforcer: effects of pimozide. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 47: 515–522. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90153-8
  74. 5. Beeler JA, McCutcheon JE, Cao ZF, Murakami M, Alexander E, et al. (2012) Taste uncoupled from nutrition fails to sustain the reinforcing properties of food. Eur J Neurosci 36: 2533–2546. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08167.x
  75. 6. Bonacchi KB, Ackroff K, Sclafani A (2008) Sucrose taste but not Polycose taste conditions flavor preferences in rats. Physiol Behav 95: 235–244. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.06.006
  76. 7. Frank GK, Oberndorfer TA, Simmons AN, Paulus MP, Fudge JL, et al. (2008) Sucrose activates human taste pathways differently from artificial sweetener. Neuroimage 39: 1559–1569. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.061
  77. 8. Schultz W (2002) Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 36: 241–263. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00967-4
  78. 9. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Tomasi D, Baler RD (2013) The addictive dimensionality of obesity. Biol Psychiatry 73: 811–818. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.020
  79. 10. Martinez D, Narendran R, Foltin RW, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, et al. (2007) Amphetamine-induced dopamine release: markedly blunted in cocaine dependence and predictive of the choice to self-administer cocaine. Am J Psychiatry 164: 622–629. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.4.622
  80. 11. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gatley SJ, et al. (1997) Decreased striatal dopaminergic responsiveness in detoxified cocaine-dependent subjects. Nature 386: 830–833. doi: 10.1038/386830a0
  81. 12. Volkow ND, Tomasi D, Wang GJ, Logan J, Alexoff D, et al. (in press) Dopamine increases are markedly blunted in active cocaine abusers. Molecular Psychiatry
  82. 13. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Schlyer D, et al. (1994) Imaging endogenous dopamine competition with [11C]raclopride in the human brain. Synapse 16: 255–262. doi: 10.1002/syn.890160402
  83. 14. Kegeles LS, Abi-Dargham A, Frankle WG, Gil R, Cooper TB, et al. (2010) Increased synaptic dopamine function in associative regions of the striatum in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67: 231–239. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.10
  84. 15. Ehrin E, Farde L, de Paulis T, Eriksson L, Greitz T, et al. (1985) Preparation of 11C-labelled Raclopride, a new potent dopamine receptor antagonist: preliminary PET studies of cerebral dopamine receptors in the monkey. The International journal of applied radiation and isotopes 36: 269–273. doi: 10.1016/0020-708x(85)90083-3
  85. 16. Gormally J, Black S, Daston S, Rardin D (1982) The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addict Behav 7: 47–55. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(82)90024-7
  86. 17. Logan J, Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Wolf AP, Dewey SL, et al. (1990) Graphical analysis of reversible radioligand binding from time-activity measurements applied to [N-11C-methyl]-(-)-cocaine PET studies in human subjects. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 10: 740–747. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1990.127
  87. 18. Tobler PN, Fiorillo CD, Schultz W (2005) Adaptive coding of reward value by dopamine neurons. Science 307: 1642–1645. doi: 10.1126/science.1105370
  88. 19. Schultz W (2010) Dopamine signals for reward value and risk: basic and recent data. Behav Brain Funct 6: 24. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-24
  89. 20. de Araujo IE, Oliveira-Maia AJ, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Caron MG, et al. (2008) Food reward in the absence of taste receptor signaling. Neuron 57: 930–941. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.032
  90. 21. Maayan L, Hoogendoorn C, Sweat V, Convit A (2011) Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity (Silver Spring) 19: 1382–1387. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.15
  91. 22. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Goldstein RZ, et al. (2009) Inverse association between BMI and prefrontal metabolic activity in healthy adults. Obesity (Silver Spring) 17: 60–65. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.469
  92. 23. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Maynard L, Jayne M, Fowler JS, et al. (2003) Brain dopamine is associated with eating behaviors in humans. Int J Eat Disord 33: 136–142. doi: 10.1002/eat.10118
  93. 24. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Thanos PK, et al. (2008) Low dopamine striatal D2 receptors are associated with prefrontal metabolism in obese subjects: possible contributing factors. Neuroimage 42: 1537–1543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.002
  94. 25. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M, et al. (2002) “Nonhedonic” food motivation in humans involves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate amplifies this effect. Synapse 44: 175–180. doi: 10.1002/syn.10075
  95. 26. Wang GJ, Smith L, Volkow ND, Telang F, Logan J, et al. (2012) Decreased dopamine activity predicts relapse in methamphetamine abusers. Mol Psychiatry 17: 918–925. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.86