Emotional Eating Phenotype is Associated with Central Dopamine D2 Receptor Binding Independent of Body Mass Index (2015)

Go to:

Abstract

PET studies have provided mixed evidence regarding central D2/D3 dopamine receptor binding and its relationship with obesity as measured by body mass index (BMI). Other aspects of obesity may be more tightly coupled to the dopaminergic system. We characterized obesity-associated behaviors and determined if these related to central D2 receptor (D2R) specific binding independent of BMI. Twenty-two obese and 17 normal-weight participants completed eating- and reward-related questionnaires and underwent PET scans using the D2R-selective and nondisplaceable radioligand (N-[11C]methyl)benperidol. Questionnaires were grouped by domain (eating related to emotion, eating related to reward, non-eating behavior motivated by reward or sensitivity to punishment). Normalized, summed scores for each domain were compared between obese and normal-weight groups and correlated with striatal and midbrain D2R binding. Compared to normal-weight individuals, the obese group self-reported higher rates of eating related to both emotion and reward (p < 0.001), greater sensitivity to punishment (p = 0.06), and lower non-food reward behavior (p  < 0.01). Across normal-weight and obese participants, self-reported emotional eating and non-food reward behavior positively correlated with striatal (p < 0.05) and midbrain (p < 0.05) D2R binding, respectively. In conclusion, an emotional eating phenotype may reflect altered central D2R function better than other commonly used obesity-related measures such as BMI.

Reward-related behavioral and neurocircuitry dysfunction may contribute to obesity1 and provide therapeutic targets for prevention and treatment of the disease. However, the role of striatal dopamine (DA) signaling in human obesity remains unclear due to mixed results of PET/SPECT studies that assess the relationship between body mass index (BMI) and D2/D3 DA receptor (D2/D3R) availability. Some studies have found that striatal D2/D3R availability is lower in obesity and negatively correlates with BMI2,3,4 while others find no difference5,6,7 or higher D2/D3R availability in obese versus normal weight individuals8 or with increasing BMI9. Using a highly specific and non-displaceable ligand, we found no significant associations of D2 receptor subtype (D2R) binding with obesity or BMI10.

Differences in human obesity DA PET study findings may be due to several factors. For instance, the study samples used have had different degrees of obesity, ranging from overweight (BMI 25.0-29.9 kg/m2)3,6,9 and mild Class I (BMI 30.0-34.9 kg/m2)3 obesity to more severe Class III (BMI ≥ 40.0 kg/m2)2,4,5,8,9,10 obesity. Obesity phenotype and DA signaling abnormalities may differ across classes of obesity1,6. To further complicate interpretation, most of these studies employed radioligands with important limitations. Specifically, [11C]raclopride and [18F]fallypride do not distinguish between D2R and D3R11, which are localized differently across the brain and may be functionally distinct12. Furthermore, these radioligands are displaceable by DA, so D2/D3R availability measures are influenced by endogenous DA release as well as by D2/D3R binding per se13,14,15.

Although BMI is not consistently correlated with D2/D3R availability16, behavioral aspects of obesity may have a closer relationship to DA signaling. To address this issue and the limitations described above, we assessed obesity-associated characteristics that may relate to DA signaling, such as emotion- and reward-based eating and behavior motivated by non-food reward and sensitivity to punishment, in obese and normal weight participants. We investigated whether these characteristics correlated with striatal D2R using (N-[11C]methyl)benperidol ([11C]NMB), a PET radioligand DA D2 receptor antagonist that is highly selective for D2R over D3R17 and other G-protein receptors and is not displaced by endogenous DA release18. In addition, since novelty seeking behavior is associated with midbrain D2/D3R binding19, we explored the relationship between midbrain D2R binding and obesity-associated behavior.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 17 normal-weight and 22 obese individuals (see Table 1). One individual in the normal-weight group was slightly overweight (BMI = 25.9 kg/m2) but percent body fat and other weight parameters met normal-weight criteria. Selected data from 15 participants from each group were reported previously10. After an overnight fast (at least 8 hr), participants underwent comprehensive medical evaluation, routine blood tests, hemoglobin A1C, and an oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT). Individuals with self-reported history of diabetes, A1C ≥ 6.5%, or OGTT results that indicated impaired fasting glucose, impaired oral glucose tolerance, or diabetes were excluded. Individuals were also screened and excluded for IQ < 8020 (WASI), and conditions including parkinsonism, lifetime psychosis, mania, substance dependence, major depression, social phobia, eating disorders (including binge eating disorder) and panic disorder by neurological examination and psychiatric interview (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV21). Current smoking and medications related to DA function were also exclusionary. No participant had smoked tobacco during the past 11 mos. or used medications related to DA function during the past month. All participants provided written informed consent. All procedures were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by Washington University’s Human Research Protection Office and Radioactive Drug Research Committee.

Table 1 

Participant Characteristics

Questionnaires

During the day of the OGTT, immediately and 1 hr after which a light snack and lunch were provided, respectively, participants completed questionnaires addressing DA-related constructs, or domains, of interest: 1) eating behavior related to emotion including avoidance of negative affect; 2) eating behavior related to reward including craving for palatable foods and inability to limit intake of sweet foods; 3) non-food reward behavior, including approach, sensitivity, motivation, and expectancy for non-food reward stimuli; and 4) punishment avoidance including inhibition, sensitivity, and expectancy. Self-report questionnaires or subscales of self-report questionnaires were included in these different domains (Table 2) based on their descriptions in original manuscripts introducing and validating the questionnaire. Scores for each questionnaire or subscale were converted into z-scores and summed together with other measures included in the domain to yield final domain scores for each individual.

Table 2 

Behavioral Domains. Normal-weight n = 17; Obese n = 21-22.

The following questionnaires were included in the Eating Related to Emotion domain: The Emotional Eating Scale22 (EES) assesses urge to eat due to negative emotion. The Dutch Eating Behavior ‘Emotional’ subscale23 (DEBQ ES) consists of summed self-ratings of tendencies to eat in response to both ‘diffuse’ (e.g., bored) and ‘clearly labeled’ (e.g., anger) emotions. The ‘mood-altering effect’ subscale of the Sweet Taste Questionnaire24 (STQ MAE) assesses the degree to which eating sweet foods alters mood in a positive manner.

The following questionnaires were included in the Eating Related to Reward domain: The Binge Eating Scale25 (BES) assesses the degree to which one experiences binge eating, including behavior (e.g., eating in secret) and emotions that occur before and after a binge (e.g. lack of control). The ‘Impaired control over eating sweets’ subscale of the STQ24 (STQ IC) is a measure of one’s ability to refrain from eating sweets. We used the total score on the Food Craving Inventory26 (FCI) to measure general craving for sweet and high-carbohydrate or fatty foods.

The following questionnaires were included in the Non-food Reward domain: The Behavioral Activation System (BAS) portion of the BIS/BAS27 questionnaire consists of three subscales: drive, fun-seeking and reward responsiveness. It is meant to measure BAS sensitivity. Individuals with stronger BAS should be more sensitive and derive more pleasure upon exposure to reward cues28,29. The sensitivity to reward portion of the Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire30 (SPSRQ) also assesses BAS functioning. The reward expectancy portion of the Generalized Reward and Punishment Expectancy Scales31 measures optimism and expectancy of positive life events. The ‘curiosity behavior’, or novelty-seeking, dimension of the Temperament and Character Inventory32 (TCI-R) reflects bias towards active novelty-seeking, impulsivity and approach towards reward cues. The ‘reward dependency’ dimension of the TCI reflects bias towards prosocial behavior and social approval. The ‘persistence’ dimension of the TCI reflects degree of perseverance despite fatigue and other obstacles.

The following questionnaires were included in the Punishment domain: The Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) portion of the BIS/BAS27 questionnaire measures BIS sensitivity. People with stronger BIS sensitivity should be more sensitive to and experience greater anxiety in response to punishment cues28,29. The punishment portions of the GRAPES31 and SPSRQ30 assess punishment expectancy and sensitivity, respectively. The ‘damage avoidance’ section of the TCI-R32 assesses bias towards behavior aimed at avoiding harm.

MRI and PET Acquisition

On a day separate from the day of the OGTT, participants underwent MRI and 2 hr PET scans, which took place between 0900 and 1700. Methods for [11C]NMB synthesis, MRI and PET scan acquisitions are described previously10. Each participant intravenously received 6.4 – 18.1 mCi containing <7.3 μg unlabeled NMB. [11C]NMB purity was ≥96% and specific activity ≥1066 Ci/mmol (39 TBq/mmol). Since [11C]NMB is not displaceable by endogenous DA18, participants were not asked to fast or otherwise modify their food intake on the night before or day of the scans.

ROI-based Analyses

The methods for our ROI-based analyses are described in Eisenstein et al.10,33. The neuroimaging software FreeSurfer (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) was used for segmentation of striatal regions34. To limit multiple comparisons, D2R specific binding (BPND) for each ROI was averaged across left and right hemispheres. Putamen and caudate D2R BPNDs were averaged to obtain a composite dorsal striatal BPND and ventral striatal BPND included average nucleus accumbens D2R BPND. The midbrain region was traced on each individual’s MPRAGE as previously described33.

Voxel-based Analyses

We conducted voxel-based analyses to determine whether specific striatal or midbrain clusters of D2R binding related to BMI or Eating Related to Emotion, Eating Related to Reward, Non-food Reward, and Punishment behavioral domain scores. Images of D2R BPND across the brain were generated for each participant and smoothed with a 6 mm full width at half maximum kernel. These images were averaged across normal-weight and obese individuals and thresholded at BPND = 0 to use as an explicit mask for regions including striatum or subcortical regions only. Positive and negative associations between D2R binding and dependent variables were tested at the voxel level using SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm).

Primary Statistical Analyses

Much of the data was managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted by the Biostatistics Division of Washington University School of Medicine35. Group demographic variables were compared with Pearson Chi Square, Mann Whitney U, or t-tests. Dorsal and ventral striatal BPND were compared with repeated measures ANCOVA covarying for age, ethnicity, and education. Midbrain D2R BPND and domain scores were compared between normal-weight and obese groups with ANCOVAs covarying for age, ethnicity and education. Significant findings for a behavioral domain were followed up with exploratory ANCOVAs of each questionnaire contributing to that domain. Separate hierarchical linear regression models with appropriate covariates (age, ethnicity, education level, and/or BMI) were used to analyze the ability of each variable of interest to predict striatal or midbrain D2R BPND. These analyses also yielded partial correlations describing the unique variance contributed by each variable of interest to BPND. For voxelwise analyses, correlations between D2R binding and BMI and behavioral domain scores were calculated as Pearson’s r and tested for significance with Student’s one-sample t-tests covaried for age, ethnicity, education, and, for behavioral domains, BMI, at each voxel. For SPM analyses, p  ≤ 0.001, after multiple comparison correction, at voxelwise level was considered significant. For all other analyses, significance level was set at α ≤ 0.05.

Results

Participant Characteristics

Normal-weight and obese groups are described in Table 1. We did not have complete Non-food Reward and Punishment questionnaire datasets from one obese individual and another obese individual did not undergo a PET scan. Therefore, analyzed data sets including these variables consist of 21 obese and 17 normal-weight individuals. One normal-weight participant’s midbrain D2R BPND was too low to be quantified by our processing software and analyses including this variable included 20 or 21 obese and 16 normal-weight participants.

BMI and Central D2R Specific Binding

As in our previous report on a subset of these individuals10, after covarying for age, ethinicity and education level, obese and normal-weight groups did not differ in striatal BPND (normal weight mean total striatal BPND = 10.30, S.D. = 1.17; obese mean total striatal BPND = 10.22, S.D. = 1.34; F1,33 = 1.98, p = 0.17). Across both groups, dorsal striatal D2R BPND was greater than ventral striatal BPND at a marginally significant level (dorsal mean BPND = 4.09, S.D. = 0.52; ventral mean BPND = 2.08, S.D. = 0.29; F1,33 = 3.87, p = 0.06) and there was no significant interaction between group and striatal region (F1,33 = 1.98, p = 0.17). Midbrain D2R BPND was not different between normal-weight and obese groups (normal-weight mean BPND = 0.27, S.D = 0.14; obese mean BPND = 0.27, S.D. = 0.09; F1,32 = 0.15, p = 0.70).

Controlling for age, ethnicity and education, BMI did not predict striatal BPND across all participants (dorsal R2 change = 0.07. F1,33 = 2.61, p = 0.12; ventral R2 change = 0.00. F1,33 = 0.02, p = 0.90) (Fig. 1), or within either group (normal-weight: dorsal R2 change = 0.01; F1,12 = 0.19, p = 0.67, ventral R2 change = 0.00. F1,12 = 0.002, p = 0.97; obese: dorsal R2 change = 0.03; F1,16 = 0.62, p = 0.44, ventral R2 change = 0.04; F1,16 = 0.99, p = 0.33). Similarly, BMI did not predict midbrain D2R BPND across normal-weight and obese participants (R2 change = 0.00. F1,32 = 0.001, p = 0.98) or within either group (normal-weight: R2 change = 0.05; F1,11 = 0.55, p = 0.48; obese: R2 change = 0.12; F1,16 = 2.51, p = 0.13).

Figure 1 

BMI and striatal D2R are not significantly correlated across normal-weight (clear circles) and obese (filled circles) groups.

Obesity-associated Behavior

Table 2 presents group mean (S.D.) summed z-scores for each domain and raw scores for each questionnaire.

The obese group had higher mean domain scores on Eating Related to Emotion (F1,34 = 11.62, p < 0.01; Fig. 2A) and Eating Related to Reward (F1,34 = 28.47, p < 0.001; Fig. 2B) and a lower mean domain score on Non-food Reward (F1,33 = 5.37, p = 0.03; Fig. 2C). Punishment domain scores were higher in obese relative to normal-weight at a marginally significant level (F1,33 = 3.69, p = 0.06; Fig. 2D).

Figure 2 

Behaviors thought to be tightly coupled to dopamine signaling differ between normal-weight and obese individuals.

Within the Eating Related to Emotion domain, scores on all three questionnaires were correlated with each other (0.63 ≤ r39 ≤ 0.80, p < 0.001) and the obese group scored significantly higher than the normal-weight group on EES (F1,33 = 6.42, p = 0.02) and DEBQ ES (F1,33 = 4.75, p = 0.04) and marginally significantly higher on STQ MAE (F1,33 = 3.48, p = 0.07). BMI was associated with the summed domain score across the entire sample (r39 = 0.46, p < 0.01) but not when examined just within obese (r22 = −0.24, p = 0.29) or normal-weight (r17 = 0.09, p = 0.74).

The z-scores on the three questionnaires included in the Eating Related to Reward domain were correlated with each other (r39 = 0.43, p ≤ 0.01). The obese group scored higher on the BES (F1,34 = 19.57, p < 0.001), STQ IC (F1,34 = 14.77, p = 0.001) and the FCI (F1,34 = 10.35, p = 0.003). BMI related to the summed domain score in the entire sample (r39 = 0.37, p < 0.02) but not within obese (r22 = 0.07, p = 0.78) or normal-weight (r17 = −0.03, p = 0.91).

Within the Non-food Reward domain, the individual questionnaires did not correlate (0.03 ≤ r38 ≤ 0.28, p  ≥ 0.09). The obese group had a lower mean score than the normal-weight group on the behavioral approach subscale of the BIS/BAS (F1,33 = 6.47, p = 0.02). Groups did not differ significantly on any of the other Reward domain scales (SPSRQ: F1,33 = 0.21, p = 0.65; TCI-R: F1,33 = 0.44, p = 0.51) except at a marginally significant level on the GRAPES reward expectancy subscale (obese < normal-weight, F1,33 = 3.25, p = 0.08). BMI did not correlate significantly with the summed domain score in the entire sample (r38 = −0.11, p = 0.51) or within normal-weight (r17 = 0.39, p = 0.12; Fig. 3A). However, BMI was correlated with the reward summed domain score within obese (r21 = 0.54, p = 0.01; Fig. 3B).

Figure 3 

Although the obese group self-reported lower rates of non-food reward behavior relative to the normal-weight group, higher BMI was associated with higher rates of non-food reward behavior within obese individuals.

Within the Punishment domain, scores on all questionnaires were correlated with each other (0.54 ≤ r39 ≤ 0.79, p ≤ 0.001). The obese group tended to score higher on the behavioral inhibition portion of the BIS/BAS (F1,33 = 3.11, p = 0.09) and the damage avoidance subscale of the TCI-R (F1,33 = 3.17, p  = 0.08) than the normal-weight group; these differences were marginally significant. Obese and normal-weight groups did not differ on the punishment expectancy subscale of the GRAPES (F1,33 = 1.10, p = 0.30) or sensitivity to punishment subscale of SPRSQ (F1,33 = 2.30, p = 0.14). BMI did not correlate significantly with the summed domain score in the entire sample (r38 = 0.15, p = 0.37) or within normal-weight (r17 = 0.21, p = 0.43) or obese (r21 = −0.35, p = 0.12) groups.

Obesity-associated Behavior and Central D2R BPND

After covarying age, ethnicity, education level, and BMI, the Eating Related to Emotion domain score related to dorsal striatal BPND (R2 change = 0.13. F1,32 = 7.51, p = 0.01; partial r = 0.44; Fig. 4A) but Eating Related to Reward (R2 change = 0.02. F1,32 = 1.15, p = 0.29), Non-food Reward (R2 change = 0.01. F1,31 = 0.31, p = 0.58) and Punishment (R2 change = 0.00. F1,31 = 0.06, p = 0.81) domain scores did not. Within the Eating Related to Emotion domain, EES (R2 change = 0.08. F1,32 = 5.48, p = 0.03, partial r = 0.38), DEBQ ES (R2 change = 0.12. F1,32 = 6.88, p = 0.01, partial r = 0.42) and STQ MAE (R2 change = 0.10. F1,32 = 4.48, p = 0.04, partial r = 0.35) scores were associated with dorsal striatal BPND .

Figure 4 

Self-reported emotional eating correlates with striatal D2R binding independent of BMI across normal-weight (clear circles) and obese (filled circles) individuals.

After covarying age, ethnicity, education level, and BMI, Eating Related to Emotion domain scores (R2 change = 0.11. F1,32 = 5.18, p = 0.03) related to ventral striatal BPND (Fig. 4B) but Eating Related to Reward (R2 change = 0.05. F1,32 = 2.33, p = 0.14), Non-food Reward (R2 change = 0.00. F1,31 = 0.19, p = 0.67) and Punishment (R2 change = 0.02. F1,31 = 0.72, p = 0.40) domain scores did not. Within the Eating Related to Emotion domain, DEBQ ES (R2 change = 0.10. F1,32 = 4.71, p = 0.04, partial r = 0.36) scores significantly correlated with ventral striatal BPND. STQ MAE (R2 change = 0.08. F1,32 = 3.93, p = 0.06; partial r = 0.33) and EES (R2 change = 0.07. F1,32 = 3.17, p = 0.09; partial r = 0.33) scores correlated with ventral striatal BPND at a marginally significant level.

After covarying age, ethnicity, education level, and BMI, midbrain D2R BPND was related to Eating Related to Emotion domain scores (R2 change = 0.10. F1,31 = 4.88, p = 0.04; partial r = 0.37, Fig. 5A). Within this domain, higher midbrain D2R BPND significantly related to higher EES (R2 change = 0.14. F1,31 = 6.48, p = 0.02; partial r = 0.42) and DEBQ ES (R2 change = 0.09. F1,31 = 4.71, p = 0.04; partial r = 0.36) scores but was not related to STQ MAE (R2 change = 0.03. F1,31 = 1.23, p = 0.28) scores. Midbrain D2R BPND was also related to Non-food Reward domain scores (R2 change = 0.13. F1,30 = 4.82, p = 0.04; partial r = 0.37, Fig. 5B). Within the Non-food Reward domain, higher midbrain D2R BPND related to higher scores on the BAS (R2 change = 0.10. F1,30 = 3.83, p = 0.06; partial r = 0.34) and reward sensitivity subscale of the SPSRQ (R2 change = 0.09. F1,30 = 3.73, p = 0.06; partial r = 0.33) at marginally significant levels but were not associated with scores on the reward expectancy subscale of the GRAPES (R2 change = 0.01. F1,30 = 0.30, p = 0.59 ) or reward-related TCI-R scales (R2 change = 0.02. F1,30 = 0.78, p = 0.38). Midbrain D2R BPND was not associated with Eating Related to Reward (R2 change = 0.00. F1,31 = 0.01, p = 0.93) or Punishment (R2 change = 0.00. F1,3 = 0.05, p = 0.83) domain scores.

Figure 5 

Midbrain D2R binding correlates with self-reported reward-related and eating behavior independent of BMI across normal-weight (clear circles) and obese (filled circles) individuals.

Voxel-based Analysis

While positive BPND-behavioral relationships appeared to be present in striatum and midbrain at less stringent criterion for statistical significance, there were no significant relationships observed between D2R binding and BMI or any of the behavioral domain scores at the voxel-wise level (p > 0.001 for all tests).

Discussion

Our current findings contribute to the obesity and neuroimaging literature in several important ways. First, we characterize four different types of putatively DA-related behavior in rigorously screened, moderately obese and normal-weight participants using well-validated and reliable questionnaires. To our knowledge, no other study has investigated these behaviors simultaneously in obese and normal-weight individuals to the same extent. Second, our D2R binding measurements are not confounded by D3R binding and competition with endogenous DA because we used the relatively novel radioligand [11C]NMB, which is unique due to its high affinity and selectivity for D2R that appears impervious to endogenous DA. These radioligand properties permit us to quantify D2R specific binding levels rather than D2/D3R availability and avoid the influence of endogenous DA levels. Finally, we detected relationships between D2R binding and behavioral phenotypes, as measured by several validated and reliable self-report questionnaires. These relationships were specific to two of four behavioral domains we investigated and were independent of BMI. Moreover, BMI itself did not correlate with D2R specific binding. These data underscore the complex interaction among eating- and reward-related behavior, BMI, and measures of a key central reward system (striatal and midbrain D2R specific binding). Our findings that eating- and reward-related behavior linearly relate to striatal and midbrain D2R, respectively, support the notion that regulation of food intake and reward-driven behavior involve a central reward, motor, and habit formation system, even though D2R specific binding was not associated with BMI.

With our ROI-based analyses, we demonstrate that obesity-associated behavior, specifically self-reported higher rates of eating to avoid negative emotion, correlates with higher striatal D2R binding in vivo across obese and normal-weight participants, independent of BMI. This finding is consistent with the recent report that striatal D2/D3R availability is positively associated with a dimension of the Three-factor Eating Questionnaire, ‘opportunistic eating’9, which reflects habitual, emotional, and situational susceptibility to disinhibited eating36. Our finding is consistent with theirs, but extends the results by using several validated questionnaires related to emotional eating and a D2-selective radioligand. Our results are also in line with those of a study that showed multi-locus genetic profile scores reflecting enhanced DA function (including the ANKK single nucleotide polymorphism associated with D2R levels) relate to more emotional and binge eating37. Our findings differ from Volkow et al.38 in which greater emotionality was associated with lower dorsal striatal D2/D3 receptor availability. However, only nonobese participants were studied by Volkow et al.38 and screening criteria and the properties of the PET radioligand used were different than those in our study. Although not statistically significant, higher dorsal striatal D2R binding in our sample tended to relate to higher BMI across normal-weight and moderately obese individuals, similar to Dunn et al.8. Perhaps, as others propose1,6,7, striatal DA system overactivity induced by repeated emotional overeating in less severe forms of overweight or obesity eventually downregulates striatal D2/D3R, presenting as lower receptor availability in extremely obese individuals as in Wang et al.4 and de Weijer et al2. Alternatively, obese individuals with relatively higher striatal D2R binding may be protected from developing more severe forms of obesity. Unfortunately, scanner weight-limits and size of the bore precluded inclusion of severely or morbidly obese individuals in the current study. Future investigations should employ longitudinal and/or cross-sectional studies to determine whether striatal D2R and obesity-associated behaviors change in accordance with large changes in BMI (i.e. from moderate to severe obesity).

Our ROI-based analyses also showed that midbrain D2R binding related to self-reported emotional eating and non-food reward-related behavior in a positive manner across normal-weight and obese groups. This is not surprising given the midbrain’s roles in motivation, habit formation39, and activity geared towards obtaining reward40. Our results are in apparent contrast with those of Savage et al.19, in which a negative relationship between novelty-seeking and substantia nigra D2/D3R availability, as measured by [18F]fallypride, was observed in normal-weight but not obese individuals. However novelty-seeking was not specifically addressed in our study – it comprised one subscale of the TCI-R questionnaire. In addition, unlike the D2R-selective [11C]NMB, [18F]fallypride binds to both D2R and D3R and is sensitive to competition with endogenous DA41. Our results are in agreement with those of a previous study in which higher trait motivation related to higher midbrain and ventral striatal D2/D3R availability as measured by [11C]raclopride42. In our study, the relationship between midbrain D2R and non-food reward-related behavior appears to be driven by scores on the BAS27 and the SPSRQ30, which are meant to reflect responsivity to and drive for reward and reward sensitivity, respectively. In contrast to striatal D2R, midbrain D2R are thought to be almost exclusively located presynaptically and, when activated by DA transmission arising locally and from afferent projections, function as inhibitory receptors on cell bodies and dendrites of dopaminergic neurons, resulting in decreased DA release in midbrain and striatum43,44,45,46. Therefore, the midbrain may modulate DA transmission in mesostriatal reward circuitry through this negative feedback loop45. Since we observed positive correlations between behavior and D2R in both striatal and midbrain regions independent of BMI, our data indicate that D2R levels within this reward pathway may reflect degree of motivation for or sensitivity to obtaining non-food reward and alleviating negative emotions via eating in normal-weight and obese individuals. However, our findings should be interpreted with caution since they are correlational and future studies may experimentally test this hypothesis and alternative explanations.

Our moderately obese participants self-reported higher rates of emotion- and reward-based eating behavior but less non-food reward behavior relative to normal-weight individuals. Obese individuals also tended to self-report sensitivity to punishment to a greater extent than normal-weight individuals. Other studies also show higher rates of eating due to emotional distress in obesity7,47,48,49,50 as well as positive correlations between food-related reward behavior and BMI26,51,52,53. However, our results contrast with a previous study that showed an inverse relationship between BMI and non-food reward behavior in obese people54. Although our obese group reported lower rates of non-food reward behavior relative to the normal-weight group, BMI was still positively related to non-food reward behavior within obese participants. One possible explanation for our finding is that while moderately obese individuals self-report reduced non-food reward-based behavior relative to normal-weight participants, there remains a gradient in which both food and non-food reward sensitivity is greater in obese individuals with higher BMI. Alternatively, there may be reward-insensitive and reward-sensitive subtypes of moderate obesity. Finally, few studies have evaluated punishment-related behavior in obesity but Franken and Muris55 found no significant correlation between sensitivity to punishment and food craving in participants ranging from underweight to obese while another study demonstrated lower behavioral inhibition in obese individuals7. Taken together, our behavioral findings support the idea that obese individuals may experience ‘reward deficiency syndrome’56, in which over-consumption of food may compensate for reduced ability to experience pleasure from other activities. Alternatively, RDS in obesity may be secondary to stronger hedonic response to food in individuals with enhanced striatal DA function37, putting them at risk of overeating and eventually overriding desire for other rewarding stimuli. Longitudinal investigation of the effect of intervention-induced changes in BMI on reward-related behavior will help clarify this relationship.

There are some limitations to the current study. First, we urge caution in interpreting our findings regarding relationships between central D2R binding and behavior because, admittedly, several hierarchical linear regression analyses were performed without stringent multiple comparisons correction. However, our findings are supported by previous studies: Guo et al.9 detected a relationship of a similar nature between dorsal D2/D3R binding and ‘opportunistic eating’ and the midbrain is known to function as a modulator of motivation for food and non-food reward39,40,57. Still, due to the novel nature of our findings and the small sample on which they are based, these results will require replication. Further, we did not find any specific clusters of D2R binding within striatum or midbrain that related to eating or reward-based behavior. Our voxelwise analyses was likely less sensitive to these relationships due to variability in D2R binding at the voxelwise level; in contrast, the ROI-based analyses reduced variability in these measures due to use of mean binding potential across regions that were eroded to minimize partial volume effects of neighboring regions known to have less D2R binding. Second, our results cannot explain whether emotional eating or non-food reward behavior precedes higher central D2R binding or vice versa, a key question in terms of understanding, preventing, or treating obesity. Also, due to time constraints, we did not control for whether participants were fasted or sated while completing relevant questionnaires and computer tasks. While this is an important factor to control for in the future, we cannot know how hunger state may have affected our results here since we did not ask participants to rate satiety. In regards to the PET scan, [11C]NMB is not displaceable by endogenous DA and therefore D2R binding potential should not be impacted by satiety state. Finally, this study was designed to obtain baseline striatal D2R binding in normal-weight and obese individuals unconfounded by health conditions and medications that interact with or affect DA signaling. Consequently, our results do not generalize to normal-weight or obese individuals with clinical diagnosis-level mental disorders thought to underlie some types of eating behaviors that may involve DA signaling including depression, impulsivity, binge eating and substance abuse. Effects of the interactions between obesity and these disorders on central D2R are highly important and merit further investigation. Despite these limitations, our results offer a template for testable hypotheses that address the limitations described.

In summary, relative to the normal-weight group, the obese group self-reported lower rates of non-food reward behavior and higher rates of eating behavior related to negative affect, sensitivity to rewarding properties of palatable foods, and sensitivity to punishment. Self-reported emotional eating positively correlated with striatal and midrain D2R binding across normal-weight and obese individuals. Higher rates of self-reported non-food reward-related behavior were associated with higher midbrain D2R binding. Taken together, our findings indicate that there are fundamental differences in self-reported eating and reward-related behavior between normal-weight and obese individuals and that, across both groups of individuals, D2R binding levels in the mesostriatal DA system may reflect degree of motivation to alleviate negative emotion through eating and for obtainment of non-food reward. Longitudinal investigations of how these variables interact and contribute to excessive body weight will help identify potential pharmacological and behavioral targets for prevention and/or treatment of obesity.

Additional Information

How to cite this article: Eisenstein, S. A. et al. Emotional Eating Phenotype is Associated with Central Dopamine D2 Receptor Binding Independent of Body Mass Index. Sci. Rep. 5, 11283; doi: 10.1038/srep11283 (2015).

Acknowledgments

Dr. Sarah A. Eisenstein and Dr. Tamara Hershey are the guarantors of this work, had full access to all of the data, and take full responsibility for the integrity of data and the accuracy of data analysis. This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (R01 DK085575, T32 DA007261, T32 DA007313, K24 MH087913 and R21 MH098670), Clinical and Translational Science Award (UL1 TR000448), Siteman Comprehensive Cancer Center and NCI Cancer Center Support (P30 CA091842), Barnes Jewish Hospital Foundation (Elliot Stein Family Fund), and the McDonnell Center for Higher Brain Function.

The authors thank the participants for their involvement. We also thank Samantha Ranck and Emily Bihun for assistance in study recruitment and data collection, and Heather Lugar, Jerrell Rutlin, and Johanna Hartlein (Washington University School of Medicine) for help in scanning participants and data processing.

Footnotes

Author Contributions S.A.E. and T.H. wrote the manuscript. S.A.E., A.N.B., D.M.G., J.V.A.D., J.M.K. and A.A.L. researched and processed data. S.A.E., D.M.G., J.V.A.D., M.Y.P., S.K., J.S.P., S.M.M., K.J.B. and T.H. contributed to study design and methods. All authors reviewed and edited the manuscript.

References

  1. Burger K. S. & Stice E. Variability in reward responsivity and obesity: evidence from brain imaging studies. Curr. Drug Abuse Rev. 4, 182–189 (2011). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  2. de Weijer B. A. et al. Lower striatal dopamine D2/3 receptor availability in obese compared with non-obese subjects. E.J.N.M.M.I. Res. 1, 37 (2011). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  3. Haltia L. T. et al. Effects of intravenous glucose on dopaminergic function in the human brain in vivo. Synapse 61, 748–756 (2007). [PubMed]
  4. Wang G. J. et al. Brain dopamine and obesity. Lancet 357, 354–357 (2001). [PubMed]
  5. Steele K. E. et al. Alterations of central dopamine receptors before and after gastric bypass surgery. Obes. Surg. 20, 369–374 (2010). [PubMed]
  6. Kessler R. M., Zald D. H., Ansari M. S., Li R. & Cowan R. L. Changes in dopamine release and dopamine D2/3 receptor levels with the development of mild obesity. Synapse 68, 317–320 (2014). [PubMed]
  7. Karlsson H. K. et al. Obesity is associated with decreased μ-opioid but unaltered dopamine D2 receptor availability in the brain. J. Neurosci., 35, 3959–3965 (2015). [PubMed]
  8. Dunn J. P. et al. Relationship of dopamine type 2 receptor binding potential with fasting neuroendocrine hormones and insulin sensitivity in human obesity. Diabetes Care 35, 1105–1111 (2012). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  9. Guo J., Simmons W. K., Herscovitch P., Martin A. & Hall K. D. Striatal dopamine D2-like receptor correlation patterns with human obesity and opportunistic eating behavior. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 1078–1084 (2014). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  10. Eisenstein S. A. et al. A comparison of D2 receptor specific binding in obese and normal-weight individuals using PET with (N-[(11)C]methyl)benperidol. Synapse 67, 748–756 (2013). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  11. Elsinga P. H., Hatano K. & Ishiwata K. PET tracers for imaging of the dopaminergic system. Curr. Med. Chem. 13, 2139–2153 (2006). [PubMed]
  12. Beaulieu J. M. & Gainetdinov R. R. The physiology, signaling, and pharmacology of dopamine receptors. Pharmacol. Rev. 63, 182–217 (2011). [PubMed]
  13. Cropley V. L. et al. Small effect of dopamine release and no effect of dopamine depletion on [18F]fallypride binding in healthy humans. Synapse 62, 399–408 (2008). [PubMed]
  14. Dewey S. L. et al. GABAergic inhibition of endogenous dopamine release measured in vivo with 11C-raclopride and positron emission tomography. J. Neurosci. 12, 3773–3780 (1992). [PubMed]
  15. Laruelle M. et al. SPECT imaging of striatal dopamine release after amphetamine challenge. J. Nucl. Med. 36, 1182–1190 (1995). [PubMed]
  16. de Weijer B. A. et al. Striatal dopamine receptor binding in morbidly obese women before and after gastric bypass surgery and its relationship with insulin sensitivity. Diabetologia 57, 1078–1080 (2014). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  17. Karimi M. et al. Decreased striatal dopamine receptor binding in primary focal dystonia: a D2 or D3 defect? Mov. Disord. 26, 100–106 (2011). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  18. Moerlein S. M., Perlmutter J. S., Markham J. & Welch M. J. In vivo kinetics of [18F](N-methyl)benperidol: a novel PET tracer for assessment of dopaminergic D2-like receptor binding. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 17, 833–845 (1997). [PubMed]
  19. Savage S. W. et al. Regulation of novelty seeking by midbrain dopamine D2/D3 signaling and ghrelin is altered in obesity. Obesity (Silver Spring) 22, 1452–1457 (2014). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  20. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Harcourt Assessment, San Antonio, TX, 1999).
  21. First M. B., Spitzer R. L., Gibbon M. & Williams J. B. W. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition. (SCID-I/NP). (Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, 2002).
  22. Arnow B., Kenardy J. & Agras W. S. The Emotional Eating Scale: the development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 18, 79–90 (1995). [PubMed]
  23. van Strien T., Frijters J. E. R., Bergers G. P. A. & Defares P. B. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 5, 295–315 (1986).
  24. Kampov-Polevoy A. B., Alterman A., Khalitov E. & Garbutt J. C. Sweet preference predicts mood altering effect of and impaired control over eating sweet foods. Eat. Behav. 7, 181–187 (2006). [PubMed]
  25. Gormally J., Black S., Daston S. & Rardin D. The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addict. Behav. 7, 47–55 (1982). [PubMed]
  26. White M. A., Whisenhunt B. L., Williamson D. A., Greenway F. L. & Netemeyer R. G. Development and validation of the food-craving inventory. Obes. Res. 10, 107–114 (2002). [PubMed]
  27. Carver C. S. & White T. L. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 319–333 (1994).
  28. Gray J. A. A critique of Eysenck’s theory of personality. A model for personality. Eysenck H. J. (ed.) 246–276. (Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 1981).
  29. Gray J. A. The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. (Oxford University Press, New York, 1982).
  30. Torrubia R., Ávila C., Moltó J. & Caserus X. The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Pers. Ind. Dif. 31, 837–862 (2001).
  31. Ball S. A. & Zuckerman M. Sensation seeking, Eysenck’s personality dimensions and reinforcement sensitivity in concept formation. Pers. Indiv. Dif. 11, 343–353 (1990).
  32. Cloninger C. R., Przybeck T. R., Svrakic D. M. & Wetzel R. D. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): a guide to its development and use. (Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, 1994).
  33. Eisenstein S. A. et al. Characterization of extrastriatal D2 in vivo specific binding of [18F](N-methyl)benperidol using PET. Synapse 66, 770–780 (2012). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  34. Fischl B. et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355 (2002). [PubMed]
  35. Harris P. A. et al. Research electronic data (REDCap). A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research support. J. Biomed. Inform. 42, 377–381 (2009). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  36. Bond M. J., McDowell A. J. & Wilkinson J. Y. The measurement of dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger: an examination of the factor structure of the Three Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ). Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 25, 900–906 (2001). [PubMed]
  37. Davis C. et al. ‘Food addiction’ and its association with a dopaminergic multilocus genetic profile. Physiol. Behav. 118, 63–69 (2013). [PubMed]
  38. Volkow N. D. et al. Brain dopamine is associated with eating behaviors in humans. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 33, 136–142 (2003). [PubMed]
  39. Wise R. A. Brain reward circuitry: insights from unsensed incentives. Neuron, 36, 229–240, 2002. [PubMed]
  40. Guitart-Masip M. et al. Action controls dopaminergic enhancement of reward representations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A., 109, 7511–7516 (2012). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  41. Riccardi P. et al. Amphetamine-induced displacement of [18F] fallypride in striatum and extrastriatal regions in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 1016–1026 (2006). [PubMed]
  42. Volkow N. D. et al. Motivation deficit in ADHD is associated with dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Mol. Psychiatry, 16, 1147–1154 (2011). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  43. Bowery B., Rothwell L. A. & Seabrook G. R. Comparison between the pharmacology of dopamine receptors mediating the inhibition of cell firing in rat brain slices through the substantia nigra pars compacta and ventral tegmental area. Br. J. Pharmacol., 112, 873–880 (1994). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  44. Lacey M. G., Mercuri N. B. & North R. A. Dopamine acts on D2 receptors to increase potassium conductance in neurones of the rat substantia nigra zona compacta. J. Physiol. 392, 397–416, (1987). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  45. White F. J. Synaptic regulation of mesocorticolimbic dopamine neurons. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 19, 405–436, (1996). [PubMed]
  46. White F. J. & Wang R. Y. Pharmacological characterization of dopamine autoreceptors in the rat ventral tegmental area: microiontophoretic studies. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 231, 275–280, (1984). [PubMed]
  47. Abilés V. et al. Psychological characteristics of morbidly obese candidates for bariatric surgery. Obes. Surg. 20, 161–167 (2010). [PubMed]
  48. Baños R. M. et al. Relationship between eating styles and temperament in anorexia nervosa, healthy control, and morbid obesity female sample. Appetite 76, 76–83 (2014). [PubMed]
  49. Davis C., Strachan S. & Berkson M. Sensitivity to reward: implications for overeating and overweight. Appetite 42, 131–138 (2004). [PubMed]
  50. Delahanty L. M. et al. Psychological and behavioral correlates of baseline BMI in the Diabetes Prevention Program (DPP). Diabetes Care 25, 1992–1998 (2002). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  51. Epel E. S. et al. The reward-based eating drive scale: a self-report index of reward-based eating. PloS ONE 9, e101350 (2014). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  52. Pepino M. Y., Finkbeiner S. & Mennella J. A. Similarities in food cravings and mood states between obese women and women who smoke tobacco. Obesity (Silver Spring) 17, 1158–1163 (2009). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  53. Thomas E. A. et al. Eating-related behaviors and appetite during energy imbalance in obese-prone and obese-resistant individuals. Appetite 65, 96–102 (2013). [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  54. Davis C. & Fox J. Sensitivity to reward and body mass index (BMI): evidence for a non-linear relationship. Appetite 50, 43–49 (2008). [PubMed]
  55. Franken I. H. & Muris P. Individual differences in reward sensitivity are related to food craving and relative body weight in healthy women. Appetite 45, 198–201 (2005). [PubMed]
  56. Comings D. E. & Blum K. Reward deficiency syndrome: genetic aspects of behavioral disorders. Prog. Brain Res. 126, 325–341 (2000). [PubMed]
  57. Meye F. J. & Adan R. A. Feelings about food: the ventral tegmental area in food reward and emotional eating. Trends Pharmacol. Sci., 35, 31–40 (2014). [PubMed]

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group