Sugar Addiction: From Evolution to Revolution (2018)

Front Psychiatry. 2018; 9: 545.

Published online 2018 Nov 7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00545

PMCID: PMC6234835

PMID: 30464748

David A. Wiss,1 Nicole Avena,2 and Pedro Rada3,*

Abstract

The obesity epidemic has been widely publicized in the media worldwide. Investigators at all levels have been looking for factors that have contributed to the development of this epidemic. Two major theories have been proposed: (1) sedentary lifestyle and (2) variety and ease of inexpensive palatable foods. In the present review, we analyze how nutrients like sugar that are often used to make foods more appealing could also lead to habituation and even in some cases addiction thereby uniquely contributing to the obesity epidemic. We review the evolutionary aspects of feeding and how they have shaped the human brain to function in “survival mode” signaling to “eat as much as you can while you can.” This leads to our present understanding of how the dopaminergic system is involved in reward and its functions in hedonistic rewards, like eating of highly palatable foods, and drug addiction. We also review how other neurotransmitters, like acetylcholine, interact in the satiation processes to counteract the dopamine system. Lastly, we analyze the important question of whether there is sufficient empirical evidence of sugar addiction, discussed within the broader context of food addiction.

Keywords: obesity, food addiction, drug addiction, sucrose, feeding behavior, dopamine, acetylcholine, nucleus accumbens

Introduction

Obesity has become one of the biggest health care burdens since the second World War ended, increasing morbidity and lowering life expectancy (, ). It is a major contributing factor to several chronic conditions, including cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and cancer (). Given the social and economic burden associated with the “obesity epidemic” there has been considerable global interest across many disciplines including medicine, nutrition, neuroscience, psychology, sociology, and public health in order to reverse this trend. Numerous interventions have been proposed, but there has been minimal progress to date. This obesity crisis affects not only developed countries but less developed ones as well, with up to 30% or more of its population categorized as overweight or obese (, ). The disproportionate increase in body weight has intensified in the last 30 years (, , ).

Virtually all investigators have asked the question of what has changed in this relatively short period of time? A common theory is an increase in sedentary lifestyles. Some contend that this alone explains the epidemic, arguing that energy expenditure, rather than food consumption, has significantly decreased in modern society compared to our hunter-gatherer ancestors (). Multiple studies support this concept of a direct correlation between physical inactivity, television watching hours, and obesity (). A second theory is the availability and consumption of highly palatable foods, which has surged in the past few decades. Nestle reported the appearance of 11,000 new food products added to the supermarket shelves every year in 1998 (), introducing countless new and attractive flavor combinations for food consumers. Investigations into the link between the “food environment” and obesity have led to the conclusion that ubiquitous access to relatively inexpensive and convenient “snack” foods have changed normal eating behavior, including less time spent preparing meals at home (). Industrialization of the food supply has decreased the cost of energy dense foods by adding refined sugars, grains, and/or fats to their products (). Consumption of these processed foods has increased in children () and toddlers ().

While behavioral and lifestyle interventions remain the mainstream “treatment” approach for obesity, dietary adherence remains an obstacle (). Recent research suggests that highly processed foods are addictive and the hedonic mechanisms (pleasure-seeking pathways) may play a critical role in the pathogenesis of obesity (). It has also been suggested that the focus on calorie counting is misguided, and that future strategies should emphasize dietary quality and individual factors such as hormonal regulation of metabolism (), and the gut microbiome (). Given the challenges that many people face controlling their appetites in today’s “food environment” it appears that public policy changes will be required to modify the conditions in which food choices are made (). According to Gearhardt and Brownell () “it will be important to look at the widespread subclinical impact of potentially addictive foods through the use of public health approaches” (). The goal of this paper is to review the human predilection for refined sugars and how they reshape the brain, with its implications for public health policy.

The nutrition transition theory

The nutrition transition theory first emerged to describe global trends toward a “Western diet” containing refined foods high in fat and sugar, and low in fiber (). Later the term was used to capture a correlation with increased BMI and changing economic and agricultural factors. Early identified factors include urbanization, economic growth, technical change, and culture () while more recent descriptions of the critical underlying factors include technology, urbanization, economic welfare relative to the cost of food, and expansion of global trade (). The nutrition transition theory is not a new concept. Previous models have included the demographic and epidemiological transitions. Popkin and Gordon-Larsen identify that both historic processes precede the nutrition transition (). The epidemiological transition describes a shift from high prevalence of disease associated with famine, malnutrition, and poor sanitation, to a pattern of high prevalence of chronic and degenerative disease associated with urban-industrial lifestyles (). This ecological framework analyzes changes at the societal level, examining how agricultural and food supply chains impact global dietary patterns. The theory suggests that “upstream” interventions (supply-side) will be more effective than addressing the lower hanging fruit (i.e., exercise, calorie restriction).

The nutrition transition theory is also supported by compelling evidence suggesting that a wide range of animals have also been gaining weight in recent years (, ). Other terms that support the “environmental theory of obesity” include “globesity” at the most distal levels, and the “neighborhood effect” at more proximal levels (). Notwithstanding, the “neighborhood effect” has far-reaching social implications, given that the neighborhood where one lives is merely a proxy for socioeconomic status. Recently, other research has suggested that discussions of nutritional inequality emphasizing supply-side factors are less indicative of consumption patterns than demand-side differences (), lending support to the food addiction (FA) hypothesis.

Evolutionary and genetic aspects of feeding

Adipose tissue in mammals play an important role in survival by preparing the body for periods of famine (). From an evolutionary perspective, the increase in body fat prepared animals for times of food scarcity, in fact, those accumulating body fat had an advantage compared to those that did not (). However, this occurred in times when humans had insecure food supply (hunter-gatherer) and could spend many days on a hypocaloric diet. During prehistoric times, the excessive increase in body weight was dampened by physical activity needed in the search of food, moreover, excessive fat would mean, as a predator, lower chances of catching the prey and vice versa (). So, even if copious quantities of food were eaten, there was a natural brake mediated by physical activity.

When did this panorama change? The first change was the advent of agriculture and animal domestication ~10,000 years ago, leading people to become producers by gathering and securing food supply (). Of course, farming depended on climate and plagues which could decimate crops resulting in famine (). The second change was the industrialization of food supply (industrial revolution of the nineteen century) allowing for mass production of flour and sugar (), with the ulterior manufacturing, in the last decades, of processed and ultra-processed foods that are inexpensive and highly caloric (abundant sugars, salts, fats) (, ). These two developments are linked to food availability and how food is refined and commercialized. Meanwhile, a third important revolution happened over the past few decades: the arrival and public accessibility of automobiles, television sets, and later the computer leading us toward a sedentary lifestyle (). When all three transformations are combined, we can see that caloric intake has risen while calorie expenditure has significantly decreased, leading to the obesity epidemic.

Although humans have culturally and technologically evolved, our genome has changed very little in the last 10,000 years (). This means that our brain circuitry is still programmed to eat more in times of food abundance preparing for periods of starvation (). Recent genetic studies have focused on gene polymorphisms related to specific nutrients and obesity (). This area of investigation has been called nutrigenetics and suggests that epigenetic factors influence the expression of predisposing genes in certain populations. For example, positive associations have been found between the fat-mass and obesity associated gene (FTO) and BMI (). Many investigators are interested in genes such as beta-adrenergic receptor 2 (ADRB2) and melanocortin receptor 4 (MCR4), since their expression may be altered following ingestions of carbohydrates (sugar) (). Researchers have found a significant interaction between sugar-sweetened beverages and genetic-predisposition score calculated on the basis of 32 BMI-associated loci suggesting that people carrying this trait, when exposed to sweetened beverages, BMI, and adiposity will be augmented (). In addition, other investigators have found that at chromosome16p11.2 different variations of this gene can affect consumption of sweet foods (, ). The question at this point is: how can we link sugar ingestion to addictive behavior?

Evolution of addictive drugs

When Charles Darwin postulated the evolution theory, he suggested that a trait would emerge if it contributed to survival and increase the reproductive success of a species. Plants have evolved protective measures to prevent herbivores from eating them. For example, some alkaloids that give the plant a bitter taste cause avoidance by most species in the animal kingdom (, ). Nonetheless, many animal species including hominids, as well as prehistoric humans, ingested lesser amounts of toxic substances and obtained benefits for their own survival (). Thus, a coevolution occurred as different traits were evolving in animals for the detection of caloric nutrients in foods (i.e., carbohydrates), traits emerged that permitted the ingestion of small amount of toxic plants to prevent diseases or to improve physical conditions (). This would explain the chewing of cocaine or tobacco leaves by aborigines in the Americas allowing them better physical fitness to cope with fatigue and a better chance to catch prey or find food (). One could argue that, like our dependency on nutritive foods to survive, we were also partially dependent on certain toxic plants. What made them addictive? Analogous to nutrients, humans learned how to process these toxic plants, increasing their potency, as it is done in modern times, conferring drugs and foods with a salient rewarding response. Thus, in both cases (food or drugs) an “evolutionary mismatch” has occurred by which human technology has been able to alter environmental conditions much faster than the changes that occur in our central nervous system (, ). Ultimately, early in our evolution the ingestion of food or drugs emerged as positive reinforcement and evolved common neural circuits for reward, and that has not changed over time, owing to their sharing of similar neural mechanisms in addictive behavior ().

Neural circuits for reward

The limbic system consists of different brain regions engaged in various aspects of emotions. Historically, it included a bidirectional pathway between the hippocampus and the hypothalamus (). Over time, other structures have been added to the circuit including: the amygdala, the nucleus accumbens (ventral striatum) and the prefrontal cortex. The functions of these structures are complex, and their diverse mechanisms of action are still being elucidated. Various neurotransmitters in this circuit (like GABA, glutamate, and opioids) are involved in several aspects of reward (, ), however, the dopaminergic pathway from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) to the nucleus accumbens (NAc) has received the most attention in the “reward” cascade (). To summarize, blocking the dopaminergic pathway between the VTA and the NAc inhibits instrumental responding for food and became the foundation of the dopamine (DA) hypothesis of reward (). Later, studies have demonstrated that “reward” is a vague term () that consists of at least three components: hedonics (“liking”), reinforcement (learning) and motivation (incentive, “wanting”) (). DA in the NAc seems to have a preponderant role in the latter two components (learning and incentive motivation) and less in the former (hedonics) where the opioid and GABA system appear to play a stronger role (, ).

Food “reward” and accumbens dopamine

Although the exact contribution of accumbens DA in reward is still unclear, most researchers agree that it is involved in feeding behavior. For instance, original studies in the 1970’s have shown that a lesion in the striatonigral DA pathway with 6-OH-dopamine provoked a profound aphagia and adipsia (). This finding was later corroborated in DA-deficient mice that also became hypoactive, aphagic, and adipsic (). Similarly, lever pressing for food pellets in animals increases DA release in the NAc (), however, not during rat chow free-feeding (, ) suggesting that DA in the accumbens regulates instrumental learning. Others have observed that accumbens DA increases during rat chow feeding only if rats were food deprived (, ) or in the presence of palatable foods (). Interestingly, increased DA while eating highly palatable food wanes after repeated exposure (, , ) and this returns if palatable foods are switched to a different one () suggesting a role of this neurotransmitter in the NAc for novelty recognition. Additionally, it has been shown that DA neurons respond to exposure of a novel food and if that novel food is paired with a cue, in a subsequent exposure, food alone will not induce neuronal firing while the cue alone does, suggesting that DA neurons are involved in conditioned learning (, ). Cue-invigorating food-seeking may be considered adaptive, but the maladaptive eating in the absence of hunger forms the basis for the FA hypothesis. It has been shown that limited or intermittent access to highly palatable foods increase cue-reactivity to these foods, which has implications for the consequences of extreme dieting behavior in humans ().

Another preponderance of evidence for the engagement of accumbens DA on feeding behavior comes from studies utilizing orexigenic peptides. It is well known that some peptides in various brain sites are capable of initiating feeding behavior, for instance, paraventricular injection of galanin, ghrelin, or opioids will promote food intake even if rats are satiated (). These peptides, systemically or locally injected in the paraventricular nuclei, increased NAc DA (). Inversely, local injection of cholecystokinin (CCK), an anorexigenic peptide, decreased DA release in the NAc (). It appears that accumbens DA plays more of a role in the anticipatory behavior than in the consummatory behavior. Stomach-derived ghrelin has known action on orexigenic neurons in the hypothalamus, and receptors have been identified in the VTA, hippocampus, and amygdala (, ). Ghrelin appears to be implicated in rewarding aspects of eating distinct from homeostatic mechanisms that promote food consumption when energy stores are low, thus may be a key driver in the motivational aspects (“wanting”) of consuming palatable foods beyond metabolic need (, ).

Finally, pharmacological manipulation of the DA system has led to contradictory results. On the one hand, DA injected directly into the NAc is capable of increasing ingestive behavior (, ). However, others have not been able to modify feeding behavior when specific DA agonists or antagonists were used (, ). Recently, chemogenetically activating DA neurons in the VTA that project to the NAc disrupted feeding patterns (). In part, these dissimilar findings show that it is very hard to propose that only one neurotransmitter or hormone is responsible for driving behavior.

Dysfunction of the dopaminergic system in obese subjects

Investigators can identify animals that have propensity to become obese in a 5-day weight gain on a high-fat diet (OP rats) (). In these OP rats, a deficit of exocytosis mechanisms in the DA neuron was found, as well as a decrease in accumbal DA basal levels (, ). Similarly, rats made obese with a “cafeteria diet” exhibited decreased basal levels of DA in the NAc, and show a blunted DA response to the taste of rat chow, while increasing DA release in response to a highly-palatable food (). Human studies using neuroimaging determined that obese patients had a lower sensitivity of the accumbens DA () and a decrease in DA-D2 receptor availability (, ). Several studies have used the term “reward deficiency syndrome” to describe a genetic dysfunction of the DA-D2 receptor leading to substance-seeking (food, drugs) behavior in humans (). Variations in the DA-D2 gene have also been associated with impulsivity and a preference for smaller more immediate rewards compared to larger but delayed ones (delay discounting) (). It is possible that obese subjects compensate for the depressed DA basal levels by overeating palatable foods (). Conversely, optogenetical-induced increase in basal DA release inhibits consumption behavior (). How can these results be reconciled with other studies? DA is released phasically and tonically with possible divergent tasks (, ). Basal DA levels are likely to determine the tonic response of the system, thus could confer a complete opposite response.

Drugs of addiction and accumbens dopamine

Most drugs of addiction activate the VTA-NAc pathway whether they are systemically injected () or locally applied in the accumbens (, ). Furthermore, drugs that increase DA release in the NAc are also self-administered (). Thus, drugs of addiction, like food, increase DA release in the NAc, however with drugs, this increment occurs repeatedly every time it is given, compared to a decline in release observed with palatable food. Blunted striatal DA and decreased DA-D2 receptor availability (measured using radiotracers as binding potential relative to nonspecific binding) have been repeatedly identified in position emission tomography (PET) scans of drug-addicted human subjects and is likely to be both a result and a cause of an addictive disorder (). Given the similarities in human PET scans between drug abusers and obese subjects (), additional research is needed to identify neurobiological risk factors for addiction-like eating. Animal studies suggest that overconsumption of each can be a predisposing factor for the other (, ).

Accumbens acetylcholine and satiety signaling

Acetylcholine (ACh) is released by local interneurons that compromise less than 2% of neurons in the NAc (, ). They have an extensive axonal arborization and form synapses in the medium spiny output neuron (). The idea that ACh opposes DA function in the striatum comes from research on Parkinson’s disease (PD). It is known that anticholinergic (antimuscarinic) drugs were the first medications used in the treatment of PD antagonizing mainly M1 receptors (, ). This indicates that DA normally exerts an inhibitory action on striatal ACh interneurons as demonstrated in rats (). In addition, L-dopa induced hyperlocomotion in DA-deficient mice is suppressed by cholinergic agonists (). Separately, anticholinergic drugs are abused () probably by increasing DA activity in the striatum (), thus, an antagonistic association probably exists between DA and ACh in the NAc and striatum.

ACh in the NAc appears to have a modulatory effect on feeding behavior. During free-feeding, ACh increased at the end of a meal () and during ingestion of a palatable food it reached a maximum after the animal stopped eating (, ). This increment disappeared in sham-fed animals that had a gastric fistula opened compared to controls with a closed gastric fistula (). Bilateral perfusion in the NAc of the indirect ACh agonist, neostigmine, reduced food intake in food-deprived animals (). Conversely, lesion of the cholinergic interneuron in the NAc with a specific toxin (AF64A) produced a significant increase in food intake (). Moreover, injection of the anorectic drug combination phentermine/fenfluramine increased ACh release in the NAc (). All these results suggest that ACh in the NAc probably signals satiety. Recently, researchers found that increasing the activity of the cholinergic interneuron in the NAc reduced palatable food consumption, lending support to the hypothesis that NAc-ACh acts as a stop signal ().

What happens if food becomes an aversive stimulus? Using a conditioned taste aversion paradigm, it has been shown that the aversive stimulus (in this case saccharin) would decrease DA release () while increasing ACh output (). Furthermore, injection of neostigmine (indirect ACh agonist) is sufficient to provoke a conditioned taste aversion (). Therefore, an increase in DA simultaneous to an increase in ACh release in the NAc signals satiety (stop) but if the change in release of these neurotransmitters is divergent (decrease in DA and increase in ACh) then the stimulus becomes aversive (). Taken together, animal feeding induces an initial and long-lasting increase in DA release followed by an increase in ACh output signaling satiation, making the animal feel satisfied (DA release) and stop the behavior (ACh).

Effect of drugs of abuse and withdrawal on acetylcholine release in the NAc

Drugs of addiction exert differential responses on the accumbens cholinergic interneuron. One could separate these drugs by their effect on feeding, for example, ACh release is decreased or not changed in the NAc if the drug increases food intake (opioids, alcohol, benzodiazepines) () while those that act as anorectic (cocaine, amphetamine, nicotine) produce the opposite effect, an increase in ACh release (, ). Moreover, cholinergic ablation in the NAc increased sensitivity to cocaine (). What is common for most drugs of addiction is that during drug withdrawal ACh is increased in the NAc (, , ). In addition, enhanced functioning of the ACh interneuron in the NAc prevents addictive behaviors for cocaine and morphine (). The augmented release of ACh in the NAc occurs simultaneously to a decrease in DA release (, , , ), identical to the response observed during a conditioned taste aversion.

What is the difference between food and drugs of addiction?

First, feeding behavior, as with other “natural” behaviors, has a satiety system provided by the mechanical limitations of the stomach and peptides like CCK that signal satiety while drugs of addiction apparently do not. Secondly, even in the presence of a palatable meal, the pleasant effect seems to wane simultaneous to a blunting of the DA response (, , , ) even though in some cases “sensory-specific satiety” can lead to continued consumption behavior after a novel food is introduced (). Finally, the magnitude of the DA increase is lower during meal than during drug administration. Drugs of abuse not only release striatal DA but also block or reverse DA reuptake, creating a more potent reinforcement through the euphoric state (). Some authors have made the argument that there is no concrete evidence of withdrawal from food, especially when compared to drugs like opioids () and that calling food addictive risks trivializing more serious addictions (). Other arguments against FA have suggested “eating addiction” as behavioral rather than substance-related (). Evidence of withdrawal in animal models will be reviewed below.

Given that adolescence is a critical period of neurodevelopment, it appears as though exposure to sucrose during this time (rodents from postnatal day 30–46) leads to an escalated intake during the exposure period and a subsequent decrease in c-Fos-immunoreactive cells in the NAc (measured at postnatal day 70) which is involved in the processing of hedonic properties of sweet foods (). In this experiment, adult rats consumed less sugar after heightened exposure in the adolescent period, which is consistent with other findings (, ). These studies also demonstrate that sugar-exposed adolescents exhibit higher preference for cocaine () but not alcohol () in adulthood. Differences in the neurobiological substrates underlying intake behavior of food and drugs abuse are likely explained by changes in the motivational aspect of food intake rather than by deficits in hedonic processing (). These findings point to deficits in the “liking” component of sweet foods and drinks which offers insight into our understanding of reward-related disorders. Interaction effects between genetic predisposition to addiction and exposure to sugar during adolescence on the “wanting” mechanism in adulthood warrants further study.

Can sugar be addictive?

Before we can make a case for sugar as an addictive substance, we must first define addiction, which is now referred to as substance use disorder (SUD). The American Psychiatric Association defines addiction, in its web page for patients and family, as “a complex condition, a brain disease that is manifested by compulsive substance use despite harmful consequence.” Operationally, experts utilize the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) as a tool to unify diagnostic criteria in clinical and/or experimental design. The current version of this manual known as the DSM-5 includes a section for SUD and it incorporates eleven criteria for diagnosis. A patient must fulfill at least two of these criteria. In turn, these eleven criteria, by their characteristics, can be compiled into four broader groups () (see Table Table11).

Table 1

Four broader categories for eleven criteria used for substance use disorder (SUD).

A. Impaired Control1. Use larger amount and for longer than intended.
2. Craving.
3. Much time spent using.
4. Repeated attempts to quit and/or control use.
B. Social Impairment1. Social/interpersonal problems related to use.
2. Neglected major role to use.
3. Activities given up to use.
C. Continued use Despite
Risk
1. Hazardous use.
2. Physical/psychological problems related to use.
D. Pharmacologial Criteria1. Tolerance.
2. Withdrawal.

These guidelines are designed to help with the diagnosis of patients, however, scientists use them in animal models, discarding those that are unique to human behavior (i.e., social impairment). Our animal model for sugar addiction consists of rodents with restricted access to 10% sugar or 25% glucose solution during a 12-h period starting 4 h into their active cycle (as Bart Hoebel would remark “animals skipped breakfast”) for 21 days (details of the protocol can be found in Avena et al. (). We are able to examine the following criteria met by our model:

  1. Impaired control:
  1. Use larger amounts and for longer than intended: rats will typically escalate their sugar ingestion progressively from an initial 37 mL up to 112 mL by day 11 when they reach an asymptote that persists for the next 10 days (, ). Escalation cannot be attributed to neophobia which is easier to overcome. In addition, experimental and control animals drink about 6 mL in the first hour during the first day and doubles in experimental subjects (over 12 mL) on day 21, while controls (ad lib sugar) drank the same 6 mL as the first day (, ). This increase could be considered a “binge” (). Certainly, the gastrointestinal system has intrinsic mechanical restraints limiting the amount consumed during escalation of a sugar solution, if bypassed (i.e., with a gastric fistula), rats will binge over 40 mL in the first hour (). So, intermittent administration of sugar mimics those used for drug self-administration () and creates a “binge” pattern of intake that resembles the compulsive behavior seen in drug abuse (, ). Binge-like consumption patterns of sucrose has been associated with decreased dendritic length of the NAc shell which supports the formation of increased excitatory inputs (). Ghrelin’s ability to interact directly with DA reward circuitry and ACh receptor gene expression in the VTA has been implicated in the motivational aspects of feeding under high-sugar conditions () which is consistent with findings that ghrelin is necessary for reward from alcohol (, ) and drugs of abuse (). Meanwhile, a shortcoming here is that we cannot determine “intention” in our animal model the way it can be assessed in humans. Therefore, “intended” is an assumption.
  2. Craving: defined by the Cambridge Dictionary as “a strong feeling of wanting something” or “feeling of desire.” In laboratory conditions, it is defined as the motivation (“wanting”) to obtain an abused substance () and is indirectly studied in animal models using instrumental behavior. In one case, rats bar press to self-administer drugs of abuse and when forced to abstain they will keep pressing the bar although unrewarded (resistance to extinction). Second, rats will readily press the bar in presence of a cue that was previously associated to the drug (incubation) (). A third paradigm, used initially in alcohol addiction, is the alcohol-deprivation effect (ADE). Alcohol drinking rats will increase their consumption following an abstinence period (, ). Experiments carried out in rats trained to respond for sucrose, instead of drugs of abuse, exhibited resistance to extinction and incubation much like cocaine (). Moreover, the incubation response was attenuated by naloxone administration arguing in favor of the endogenous opioid involvement in sugar craving (). Additionally, rats trained to drink a non-caloric solution (saccharin) also showed incubation, consequently, the phenomenon depends on taste (hedonic) and not just the caloric content of the solution (). Lastly, rats trained for 28 days to drink a sucrose solution and deprived for 14 days displayed a sugar-deprivation effect analogous to ADE (). These results are an indirect measure of the motivation to use sugar (craving) and fulfills one of the DSM-5 criteria for SUD. Craving has been intimately related to high rates of relapse in drugs of abuse () and now with sugar.
  • B. Social impairment (unable to assess with animal model).
  • C. Continued use despite risk:
  1. Hazardous use: In the context of drug abuse, a conditioned suppression paradigm is utilized as an indicator of a compulsive behavior and gives indirect evidence of the power of craving (). Animals will seek a drug (i.e., cocaine) despite an aversive conditioned stimulus (). The results on sucrose consumption, using this paradigm, is controversial. On one hand, it was found that the conditioned stimulus suppressed sugar intake indicating that the animal would not take the risk (). In this case, rats were trained to obtain sucrose on a “seeking/taking” chain schedule that paralleled cocaine use, and the conditioned stimulus suppressed sucrose intake as well as increased seeking latency, however, in this paradigm we do not know whether rats were sugar-dependent or not. Meanwhile, others have found that mice on a highly palatable food diet were insensitive to the aversive conditioned stimulus () or would withstand an unpleasant environment to gain access to the meal (). Further research is needed to determine if sugar dependent rats will endure an aversive stimulus to seek the sugar solution.
  • D. Pharmacological criteria:
  1. Tolerance: is the gradual decrease in responsiveness to a drug demanding an increase in doses consumed to obtain the same initial effect (, ). In our model, rats progressively escalated their sugar intake as explained above and it probably argues in favor of a tolerance effect (, ).
  2. Withdrawal: corresponds to a set of signs and symptoms that a drug user presents once the drug is suspended or the specific antagonist is injected. One of the most clearly defined, in animals, are the signs of opiate withdrawal either spontaneous or induced with a specific antagonist (i.e., naltrexone, naloxone) including: wet-dog shakes, teeth-chattering, piloerection, diarrhea, grooming, rearing, writhing (). Two other symptoms in opiate withdrawal are anxiety and behavioral depression. The former is inferred in rats using the plus-maze and measuring the amount of time spent in the open or closed arms (). Spontaneous and naloxone-induced opiate withdrawal in rats decreased exploration into the open arms confirming the anxiogenic-like effect following the abandonment of the drug (). The latter symptom is explored using the forced-swim test and monitoring the amount of time swimming (). Morphine withdrawal causes a prolonged enhancement of immobility in rats confirming the behavioral depression induced when the drug was suspended ().

Sugar acts as an analgesic most likely by releasing endogenous opioids (). Hence, it is sensible to look for signs of opiate withdrawal in rats made dependent on sugar or palatable food (). Injection of naloxone in sugar-dependent rats generated several of the opiate withdrawal symptoms and anxiety-like response on the plus-maze (, ). Similarly, sugar deprivation (analogous to spontaneous drug withdrawal) produced signs of opiate withdrawal including anxiety-like behaviors (, ). Only recently have withdrawal symptoms been elucidated in humans meeting criteria for FA by way of predictive reference resetting (allostasis) controlled by the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex ().

Neurochemically, morphine withdrawal is accompanied by a decrease in accumbens DA release with a simultaneous ACh increase (, , ). An equal response was observed when sugar experienced rats were injected naloxone or sugar deprived (), confirming the involvement of the endogenous opioid system in the development of sugar dependency.

Additional aspects of sugar addiction are comparable to drug addiction

So far, this model of sugar addiction meets five of the criteria established in the DSM-5. In addition to the clinical criteria, there are other behavioral and neurochemical attributes observed in animal experimentation that we will discuss below.

Behavioral sensitization is a phenomenon linked to several facets of drug dependence and consists on a long-lasting increase in locomotor activity following repeated administration of psychostimulants or opioids (). Animals sensitized with one drug of abuse often show the same hyperactivity when a different drug is injected. This has been called cross-sensitization and occurs between different drugs of addiction (). For example, rats sensitized to 9-delta-tetracannabinol displayed a sensitized behavior when morphine was injected (). Equally, rats sensitized to cocaine are cross-sensitized to ethanol and vice-versa (). Comparable to drugs of abuse, sugar dependent rats show cross-sensitization to drugs of abuse and the other way around. For instance, rats maintained on an intermittent sugar schedule display cross-sensitization to amphetamine () and rats sensitized to amphetamine increase their locomotion when exposed to 10% sucrose solution (). Furthermore, sucrose intake has been shown to enhance behavioral sensitization induced by cocaine and ethanol (, ). Thus, intermittent sugar promotes behaviors observed with drugs of abuse.

Human research on behavioral sensitization has been used to explain the progressive nature of drug use and the role of internal and external cueing in the motivation process. Highly caloric food elicit the strongest DA response but it has been suggested that only a subset of susceptible individuals become conditioned for behavioral sensitization () likely due to genetic variability in the dopaminergic system. There is still some debate if individuals are more susceptible under conditions of reward hyposensitivity () or hypersensitivity (). There has also been discussion that energy density, but not sugar specifically plays the most important role in determining the reward value of food ().

The gateway hypothesis claims that legal drugs (alcohol or nicotine) precedes consumption of cannabinoids, and cannabinoids precede other illicit drugs (). In animal models of drug abuse this phenomenon appears to be linked to cross-sensitization and instead of increasing locomotor activity it increases the intake of another drug (“consummatory cross-sensitization”) (). For instance, exposure to cannabis in young adult rats enhanced opiate intake when adults (). In a separate experiment, pre-exposure of ethanol enhanced cocaine self-administration in adult mice (, ). Sugar dependent rats forced to abstain intensified their intake of 9% ethanol. In this case, sugar seems to act as a gateway to alcohol use ().

Other neurochemical similarities between drugs of abuse and sugar dependent rats have been observed. As previously described in this review, DA response to palatable food habituates following repeated exposure (, ), however, when sugar is given intermittently this effect disappears and like drugs of abuse, DA increases every time that the animal is exposed to sugar ().

Changes in mu-opioid and DA (D1 and D2) properties have also occurred in different experimental models of drug abuse. For instance, repeated cocaine application was correlated with upregulation of mu-opioid receptors (MORs) and increased binding of DA-D1 receptors (). Self-administration of cocaine in monkeys increased DA-D1 density and decreased DA-D2 receptors (). However, conflicting results have been detected for the DA-D1 receptor while a consistent DA-D2 receptor downregulation in cocaine addicted subjects occurred (), alike human studies (, ). In our sugar intermittent model, an increase in DA-D1 and MOR binding with an opposite response in DA-D2 binding was detected (). Posteriorly, studies show a decrease in DA-D2 mRNA or binding in the NAc of sugar and high-fructose corn syrup drinkers while the MOR mRNA increased only in high-fructose corn syrup drinkers (). Therefore, palatable food and drugs of abuse share similar neurotransmitter systems with changes in DA release, as well as in receptor function.

In summary, rats in the intermittent sugar access schedule fulfill five of the eleven criteria in the DSM-5 and induces other brain changes that resemble drugs of abuse. Thus, confirming that sugar can be addictive and plays a key role in the broader construct of “food addiction,” at least in this animal model. A brief overview on human data will be summarized below, as well as some of the arguments against FA.

Addiction potential of highly palatable food related to maternal influence

Given ethical limitations, prospective studies examining the impact of extreme dietary imbalances (high-sugar, or high-fat) during human pregnancy cannot be undertaken. Rodent models show that such dietary extremes (high-sugar and/or high-fat) can impact fetal neurodevelopment, providing evidence of “addiction transfer” from mother to the newborn (). These animal studies highlight the importance of biological processes (absence of social factors) in the development of FA. Specifically, maternal exposure to drugs of abuse or to highly palatable foods during both the pre- and postnatal period alter behavior via the DA reward system (, ) and MOR () of the offspring. Intrauterine nutritional experiments in animal models have demonstrated perturbations in hormone (e.g., insulin, leptin, ghrelin) signaling that interact with the development of the reward system in the VTA. Both under- and overfeeding have the potential to increase obesity prevalence in the offspring by way of the DA and opioid systems () and such effects have been observed at the intergenerational level (, ). Changes in DNA methylation appear to modify genetic expression of DA transporter and MOR (). While more research has been conducted using a high-fat compared to high-sugar model, caloric sweeteners have been shown to favor hedonic over homeostatic mechanisms (). Hormonal regulation of food reward may partially explain why sucrose is preferred over artificial sweeteners.

Human research on “food addiction”

The major construct that has emerged from the theory of FA is the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS). Preliminary validation of the YFAS occurred in 2008 in order to “identify those exhibiting signs of addiction toward certain types of foods” (). The scale is designed to mirror established alcohol and drug addiction criteria described above. Questions were adapted to assess consumption of high-fat and high-sugar foods and were reviewed by a panel of experts as well as patients with binge eating disorder for feedback on wording. The authors concluded that the YFAS may be a useful tool in identifying individuals with addictive tendencies toward food and propose its use in exploring whether FA is a valid and useful concept. In 2016, the YFAS 2.0 was developed to maintain consistency with the current diagnostic understanding of SUDs described in the DSM-5 which also includes severity indicators ().

Evidence is accumulating on the overlap of neural circuitry and commonalities between drug abuse and FA in humans (). Population studies carried out using both YFAS and recently YFAS 2.0 have detected a prevalence of food addicts from as low as 5.4% to as high as 56% depending on the population studied (weighted mean prevalence reported at 19.9% in systematic review) (, ). Interestingly, this figure [19.9%] closely matches the prevalence of other legal drugs like alcohol () and tobacco (). When considering the association between FA and BMI, close to 20% were obese and little over 40% were underweight (). One could speculate on the reason of this disparate result. Addictive mechanisms serve a homeostatic function so that if food is scarce one will seek it and binge when found. Additionally, those in the underweight category may be dieting or displaying restrained eating patterns which can increase reward sensitivity for food. The failure of human models of food addiction using YFAS to control for dieting behaviors is a shortcoming of this construct (discussed below).

Dysfunction of the reward system in the presence of highly palatable food becomes a major driver in the prevalence of obesity. While there is an interaction between FA and obesity, they are not the same condition. We cannot discard FA because not all obese people are food-addicted and not all food-addicted are obese (). Many factors are involved in the appearance of obesity and food addiction is just one of them (), but when 15% of the US population consider themselves as “food addicts” of an estimated 330 million people (census.gov accessed July 2018), then close to 50 million people and (if estimates are correct) close to 20% are obese (), that gives us a figure of 10 million people that are both food-addicted and obese. This is a substantial number of people with maladaptive functioning. A recent systematic review and meta-analysis of human studies “support that altered general reward-related decision making is a salient neuropsychological factor across eating and weight disorders in adulthood” (). Taken together, the FA perspective suggests that biochemical changes and genetic predisposition to addiction can lead to excess food consumption independent of social factors. An important theme that has emerged is that FA is both an individual problem as well as a collective problem that should be addressed on a societal level. Given the obesity trends and more recently the opioid epidemic, it can be argued that addiction is the number one public health problem in the United States.

Food addiction and eating disorders

Research into the interaction between food addiction and eating disorders (EDs), specifically binge eating disorder (BED) and bulimia nervosa (BN), has led to conclusions of separate but related constructs. In one study of individuals with BN, 96% met criteria for FA (). It has been proposed that those who meet criteria for BN should be separated into distinct subtypes: hyporesponsive to reward (akin to anorexia nervosa) and those with hypersensitive reward circuitry (akin to FA) (). Approximately half of BED patients meet criteria for FA (). Overlapping mechanisms include reward dysfunction and impulsivity and unique features for BED include dietary restraint and shape/weight concerns ().

The biggest gap in our understanding of the interaction between FA and EDs is the restrictive eating component. There are many detractors of the FA hypothesis from the ED treatment community who argue that dieting (also referred to as restrained eating) is what causes elevated scores on the YFAS. It has also been argued that the role played by ingested substances is nonspecific meaning that they apply to EDs as well (). Future research should control for restrained eating, which has not been adequately done. So, it is not surprising that high prevalence of FA occurs in the underweight category (, ) and normal weight category in the case of BN (). Recently investigators have suggested that FA data can be incorporated into the case conceptualization of EDs from a trans-diagnostic perspective (, ). Conclusions suggest giving more consideration to the impact of highly palatable foods for some people seeking ED treatment. A few studies have linked FA and SUD (, ) but additional research should be conducted on individuals with SUD in order to further understand how eating behaviors can progress throughout the recovery process. Interaction effects between FA, SUD, and ED have not yet been adequately described.

Sugar and obesity

Considerable controversy exists with respect to sugar intake and obesity (). There is general consensus indicating that sugar (sucrose, fructose) is not a direct cause of obesity (, ), however, other studies have linked sugar-sweetened beverages (SSB) to an increase in body weight in children and adults (, ). Several reasons are offered to explain this discrepancy, but somehow SSB appears to be a special case. First, it is possible that liquid calories are not compensated by a total decrease in energy intake. Second, ingestion of SSB might be an indicator of unhealthy lifestyle (). None of these studies have linked SSB to sugar addiction so we cannot adequately assess the direct impact of compulsive SSB consumption on body weight.

Nutrition transition theory proposes that “with economic development populations shift from minimally processed diets rich in staple food of vegetable origin to diets high in meat, vegetable oils, and processed foods” (). As mentioned, this transition in diet is coupled with the obesity epidemic observed in developing countries (, ). Research shows that several developing countries in Asia are shifting their diets to preferentially processed foods and carbonated soft-drinks as the main “product vector” for sugar intake (). Similarly, a shift from minimally processed foods to ultra-processed (more added sugar, more saturated fat, more sodium, less fiber) food has been seen in Brazil (). Both studies condemned ultra-processed food as an important culprit in the obesity epidemics and ask policy-makers to include legislation and “regulatory approaches” to minimize the impact on health. This approach must be parallel to education programs.

Policy implications

While ecological approaches targeting global nutrition policy appear promising, agricultural systems remain directed by multibillion-dollar multinational food corporations rather than by governments. It is difficult to predict how emerging data on FA can impact policy, particularly given that corporations have fiduciary responsibilities to their shareholders which require them to maximize profits and may compromise other social and ecological goals (). Some public health experts propose that we will need to address food corporations similarly to how the tobacco industry was addressed in recent years, with interdiction and litigation (). It remains unclear how an understanding of FA will translate to behavior change, however, a recent survey suggests that framing certain foods as addictive may increase obesity-related policy support such as warning labels, similar to tobacco (). Other researchers believe that sugar addiction is too narrow and therefore still premature, warning against policy changes that are unlikely to have an impact since sugar is already so ubiquitous in the food supply ().

The FA theory directly implicates the food industry, while the nutrition transition theory implicates other global industries also potentially negatively impacting our environment. We propose that the FA framework can lead to improved health outcomes but are more likely to be more pronounced in socially advantaged groups, given barriers created by socioeconomic status. Many public health interventions focused on obesity aim to reduce disparities between groups, which we believe can also have a meaningful impact on long-term health outcomes. Given the evidence reviewed herein, we make the case for sugar addiction in the animal model. Overlooking these findings will represent a missed opportunity for obesity-related policy and a potential public health revolution. Potential treatment strategies for FA have been reviewed elsewhere (). A commentary on the necessity as well as potential downsides of the food addiction model was published previously ().

Conclusion

The FA framework for understanding obesity is the notion that highly processed “hyperpalatable” foods have hijacked the reward centers in the brain thus impairing the decision-making process, similar to drugs of abuse. The major assumption is that biochemistry drives behavior. The sugar addiction theory bridges current gaps between food science and neuroscience, and between nutrition and psychology. This theory was originally developed from animal studies, however there is no shortage of compelling human data. While FA has been sensationalized in the popular press with headlines such as “Oreos More Addictive Than Cocaine?” we propose that processed FA in humans is much more like caffeine or nicotine addiction than it is like cocaine or heroin. There is a subtlety to food addiction where a significant majority of the people who meet criteria may not be aware of it, likely because it is not widely accepted as a social norm. Meanwhile, there have been non-clinical recovery movements of self-identified “food addicts” dating as far back as 1960 when Overeaters Anonymous was formed.

A seminal paper by Glass and McAtee envisioned a future for public health which integrates the natural and behavioral sciences with respect to the study of health. Their multilevel framework extends the “stream of causation” to include both social and biological influences. The authors use the term “embodiment” to describe the “sculpting of internal biological systems that occurs as a result of prolonged exposure to particular environments” (). These authors propose that next-generation models focus on how social environments affect the organism (human) which will affect the organs, cells, sub-cellular, and molecular levels, and how these will provide feedback at multiple levels. They argue that while social factors act as mediating risk regulators, explanations of obesity must incorporate the biological substrate: “whatever has changed in the environment that has led to exponential expansion in population body weight, must be conspiring with epigenetic and psychophysiological factors. Eating behavior is an example of a phenomenon that results from synergistic interactions among biological (hunger) and social (eating cues) levels” ().

To date the YFAS is the only validated measure to evaluate addiction-like eating. While there are over 100 original research studies using the YFAS and the tool has undergone several iterations (now YFAS 2.0), brain imaging studies in humans remain somewhat limited, and a gap remains between psychological assessment and reward-related brain circuitry. More importantly, FA research has not been able to account for all of the social factors (e.g., income, education, access, culture) that contribute to food consumption patterns. Additionally, FA is not limited to obesity, as this construct has been extended to non-obese populations which makes causal inference difficult to assess. Much of the appetite-related research does not include the term “food addiction” likely due to the cultural stigmas associated with addiction.

Finally, there is strong evidence of the existence of sugar addiction, both at preclinical and clinical level. Our model has demonstrated that five out of eleven criteria for SUD are met, specifically: use of larger amounts and for longer than intended, craving, hazardous use, tolerance, and withdrawal. From an evolutionary perspective, we must consider addiction as a normal trait that permitted humans to survive primitive conditions when food was scarce. As we evolved culturally, the neural circuits involved in addictive behaviors became dysfunctional and instead of helping us survive they are in fact compromising our health. From a revolutionary perspective, understanding the molecular, and neurological/psychological intricacies of addiction (sugar, drugs of abuse) will permit the discovery of new therapies (pharmacological and non-pharmacological) and possible management of at least one crucial factor in the occurrence of obesity.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Funding. This work is funded by Kildehoj-Santini (NMA).

References

1. World Health Organization Obesity and Overweight. Fact Sheet (2018). Available online at: http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight
2. McNamara JM, Houston AI, Higginson AD. Costs of foraging predispose animals to obesity-related mortality when food is constantly abundant. PLoS ONE (2015) 10:e0141811. 10.1371/journal.pone.0141811 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
3. Johnson RJ, Sánchez-Lozada LG, Andrews P, Lanaspa MA. Perspective: a historical and scientific perspective of sugar and its relation with obesity and diabetes. Adv Nutr An Int Rev J. (2017) 8:412–22. 10.3945/an.116.014654 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
4. Lopez KN, Knudson JD. Obesity: from the agricultural revolution to the contemporary pediatric epidemic. Congenit Heart Dis. (2012) 7:189–99. 10.1111/j.1747-0803.2011.00618.x [PubMed] [CrossRef]
5. Fleming T, Robinson M, Thomson B, Graetz N. Global, regional and national prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adults 1980-2013: a systematic analysis. Lancet (2014) 384:766–81. 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60460-8 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
6. Staub K, Bender N, Floris J, Pfister C, Rühli FJ. From undernutrition to overnutrition: the evolution of overweight and obesity among young men in Switzerland since the 19th century. Obes Facts (2016) 9:259–72. 10.1159/000446966 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
7. Prentice AM, Jebb SA. Obesity in britain: gluttony or sloth? Br Med J. (1995) 311:437 10.1136/bmj.311.7002.437 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
8. Singh GK, Siahpush M, Kogan MD. Rising social inequalities in US childhood obesity, 2003-2007. Ann Epidemiol. (2010) 20:40–52. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2009.09.008 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
9. Eisenmann JC, Bartee RT, Wang MQ. Physical activity, TV viewing, and weight in U.S. youth: 1999 youth risk behavior survey. Obes Res. (2002) 10:379–385. 10.1038/oby.2002.52 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
10. Eaton SB, Eaton SB. Physical inactivity, obesity, and type 2 diabetes: an evolutionary perspective. Res Q Exerc Sport (2017) 88:1–8. 10.1080/02701367.2016.1268519 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
11. Armelagos GJ. Brain evolution, the determinates of food choice, and the omnivore’s dilemma. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. (2014) 54:1330–41. 10.1080/10408398.2011.635817 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
12. Hall KD. Did the food environment cause the obesity epidemic? Obesity (2018) 26:11–13. 10.1002/oby.22073 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
13. Eicher-Miller H, Fulgoni V, Keast D. Processed food contributions to energy and nutrient intake differ among US children by race/ethnicity. Nutrients (2015) 7:10076–88. 10.3390/nu7125503 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
14. Welsh JA, Figueroa J. Intake of added sugars during the early toddles period. Nutr Today (2017) 52 (Suppl.):S60–S68. 10.1097/NT.0000000000000193 [CrossRef]
15. Williamson DA. Fifty years of behavioral/lifestyle interventions for overweight and obesity: where have we been and where are we going? Obesity (2017) 25:1867–75. 10.1002/oby.21914 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
16. Lee PC, Dixon JB. Food for thought: reward mechanisms and hedonic overeating in obesity. Curr Obes Rep. (2017) 6:353–61. 10.1007/s13679-017-0280-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
17. Camacho S, Ruppel A. Is the calorie concept a real solution to the obesity epidemic? Glob Health Action (2017) 10:1289650. 10.1080/16549716.2017.1289650 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
18. Aguirre M, Venema K. The art of targeting gut microbiota for tackling human obesity. Genes Nutr. (2015) 10:20. 10.1007/s12263-015-0472-4 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
19. Schwartz MB, Just DR, Chriqui JF, Ammerman AS. Appetite self-regulation: environmental and policy influences on eating behaviors. Obesity (2017) 25:S26–38. 10.1002/oby.21770 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
20. Gearhardt AN, Brownell KD. Can food and addiction change the game? Biol Psychiatry (2013) 73:802–3. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.07.024 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
21. Popkin BM. Nutritional patterns and transitions. Popul Dev Rev. (1993) 19:138–57.
22. Popkin BM, Gordon-Larsen P. The nutrition transition: worldwide obesity dynamics and their determinants. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. (2004) 28 (Suppl. 3):S2–9. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802804 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
23. Popkin BM. Nutrition transition and the global diabetes epidemic. Curr Diab Rep. (2015) 15:64. 10.1007/s11892-015-0631-4 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
24. Omran AR. The epidemiologic transition. A theory of the epidemiology of population change. Milbank Mem Fund Q (1971) 49:509–38. [PubMed]
25. Pretlow RA, Corbee RJ. Similarities between obesity in pets and children: the addiction model. Br J Nutr. (2016) 116:944–9. 10.1017/S0007114516002774 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
26. Klimentidis YC, Beasley TM, Lin H-Y, Murati G, Glass GE, Guyton M, et al. . Canaries in the coal mine: a cross-species analysis of the plurality of obesity epidemics. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. (2011) 278:1626–32. 10.1098/rspb.2010.1890 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
27. Black JL, Macinko J. Neighborhoods and obesity. Nutr Rev. (2008) 66:2–20. 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2007.00001.x [PubMed] [CrossRef]
28. Allcott H, Diamond R, Dubé J-P. The Geography of Poverty and Nutrition: Food Deserts and Food Choices Across the United States | Stanford Graduate School of Business (2018). Available online at: https://www.gsb.stanford.edu/faculty-research/working-papers/geography-poverty-nutrition-food-deserts-food-choices-across-united
29. Higginson AD, McNamara JM, Houston AI. The starvation-predation trade-off predicts trends in body size, muscularity, and adiposity between and within Taxa. Am Nat. (2012) 179:338–50. 10.1086/664457 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
30. Nettle D, Andrews C, Bateson M. Food insecurity as a driver of obesity in humans: the insurance hypothesis. Behav Brain Sci. (2016) 40:e105. 10.1017/S0140525X16000947 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
31. Eaton SB, Konner M. Paleolithic nutrition. A consideration of its nature and current implications. N Engl J Med. (1985) 312:283–9. 10.1056/NEJM198501313120505 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
32. Ludwig DS. Technology, diet, and the burden of chronic disease. JAMA (2011) 305:1352–53. 10.1001/jama.2011.380 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
33. Monteiro CA, Levy RB, Claro RM, Ribeiro de Castro IR, Cannon G. Increasing consumption of ultra-processed foods and likely impact on human health. Evidence from Brazil. Public Health Nutr. (2013) 16:2240–8. 10.1017/S1368980012005009 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
34. Steemburgo T, Azevedo MJ d, Martínez JA. Interação entre gene e nutriente e sua associação à obesidade e ao diabetes melito. Arq Bras Endocrinol Metabol. (2009) 53:497–508. 10.1590/S0004-27302009000500003 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
35. Qi Q, Chu AY, Kang JH, Jensen MK, Curhan GC, Pasquale LR, et al. . Sugar-sweetened beverages and genetic risk of obesity. N Engl J Med. (2013) 367:1387–96. 10.1056/NEJMoa1203039 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
36. Haslam DE, McKeown NM, Herman MA, Lichtenstein AH, Dashti HS. Interactions between genetics and sugar-sweetened beverage consumption on health outcomes: a review of gene-diet interaction studies. Front Endocrinol. (2018) 8:e00368. 10.3389/fendo.2017.00368 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
37. Castillo JJ, Orlando RA, Garver WS. Gene-nutrient interactions and susceptibility to human obesity. Genes Nutr. (2017) 12:1–9. 10.1186/s12263-017-0581-3 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
38. Hinney A, Nguyen TT, Scherag A, Friedel S, Brönner G, Müller TD, et al. . Genome wide association (GWA) study for early onset extreme obesity supports the role of Fat Mass and Obesity Associated Gene (FTO) Variants. PLoS ONE (2007) 2:e1361. 10.1371/journal.pone.0001361 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
39. Soto M, Chaumontet C, Mauduit C-D, Fromentin G, Palme R, Tomé D, et al. Intermittent access to a sucrose solution impairs metabolism in obesity-prone but not obesity-resistant mice. Physiol Behav. (2016) 154:175–83. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.012 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
40. Krashes MJ, Lowell BB, Garfield AS. Melanocortin-4 receptor–regulated energy homeostasis. Nat Neurosci. (2016) 19:206–19. 10.1038/nn.4202 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
41. Abete I, Navas-Carretero S, Marti A, Martinez JA. Nutrigenetics and nutrigenomics of caloric restriction. Prog Mol Biol Transl Sci. (2012) 108:323–46. 10.1016/B978-0-12-398397-8.00013-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
42. Keskitalo K, Tuorila H, Spector TD, Cherkas LF, Knaapila A, Silventoinen K, et al. . Same genetic components underlie different measures of sweet taste preference. Am J Clin Nutr. (2007) 86:1663–9. 10.1093/ajcn/86.5.1663 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
43. Keskitalo K, Knaapila A, Kallela M, Palotie A, Wessman M, Sammalisto S, et al. . Sweet taste preferences are partly genetically determined: identification of a trait locus on chromosome 16. Am J Clin Nutr. (2007) 86:55–63. 10.1093/ajcn/86.1.55 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
44. Davis C. Evolutionary and neuropsychological perspectives on addictive behaviors and addictive substances: relevance to the “food addiction” construct. Subst Abuse Rehabil. (2014) 5:129–37. 10.2147/SAR.S56835 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
45. Sullivan RJ, Hagen EH. Psychotropic substance-seeking: evolutionary pathology or adaptation? Addiction (2002) 97:389–400. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00024.x [PubMed] [CrossRef]
46. Nesse RM, Williams GC. Evolution and the origins of disease. Sci Am. (1998) 279:86–93. 10.1038/scientificamerican1198-86 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
47. Pani L. Is there an evolutionary mismatch between the normal physiology of the human dopaminergic system and current environmental conditions in industrialized countries ? Mol Psychiatry (2000) 5:467–75. 10.1038/sj.mp.4000759 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
48. Ahmed SH, Guillem K, Vandaele Y. Sugar addiction. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care (2013) 16:434–39. 10.1097/MCO.0b013e328361c8b8 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
49. Ahmed SH, Lenoir M, Guillem K. Neurobiology of addiction versus drug use driven by lack of choice. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2013) 23:581–87. 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.028 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
50. Hagen EH, Roulette CJ, Sullivan RJ. Explaining human recreational use of “pesticides”: The neurotoxin regulation model of substance use vs. the hijack model and implications for age and sex differences in drug consumption. Front Psychiat. (2013) 4:142. 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00142 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
51. Papez J. A Proposed mechanisms of emotions. Arch Neurol Psychiat. (1937) 38:725–43.
52. Kalivas P, Volkow N. New medications for drug addiction hiding in glutamatergic neuroplasticity. Mol Psychiatry (2011) 16:974–86. 10.1109/TMI.2012.2196707 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
53. Kelley AE, Berridge KC. The neuroscience of natural rewards: relevance to addictive drugs. J Neurosci. (2002) 22:3306–11. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-09-03306.2002 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
54. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Rev. (1998) 28:309–69. [PubMed]
55. Di Chiara G. Nucleus accumbens shell and core dopamine: Differential role in behavior and addiction. Behav Brain Res. (2002) 137:75–114. 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00286-3 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
56. Ferrario CR, Labouèbe G, Liu S, Nieh EH, Routh VH, Xu S, et al. . Homeostasis meets motivation in the battle to control food intake. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:11469–81. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2338-16.2016 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
57. Hoebel BG, Avena NM, Bocarsly ME, Rada P. Natural addiction: a behavioral and circuit model based on sugar addiction in rats. J Addict Med. (2009) 3:33–41. 10.1097/ADM.0b013e31819aa621 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
58. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry (2016) 3:760–73. 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
59. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Baler RD. Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: Implications for obesity. Trends Cogn Sci. (2011) 15:37–46. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
60. Volkow ND, Wise RA, Baler R. The dopamine motive system : implications for drug and food addiction. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2017) 18:741–52. 10.1038/nrn.2017.130 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
61. Wise RA, Rompre PP. Brain dopamine and reward. Annu Rev Psychol. (1989) 40:191–225. 10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.001203 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
62. Salamone JD, Mercea C. The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron. (2012) 76:470–85. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
63. Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW. Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol. (2009) 9:65–73. 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
64. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron (2015) 86:646–4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
65. Nicola SM. Reassessing wanting and liking in the study of mesolimbic influence on food intake. Am J Physiol – Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2016) 311:R811–40. 10.1152/ajpregu.00234.2016 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
66. Ungerstedt U. Adipsia and aphagia after 6-hydroxydopamine induced degeneration of the nigro-striatal dopamine system. Acta Physiol Scand Suppl. (1971) 367:95–122. [PubMed]
67. Zhou QY, Palmiter RD. Dopamine-deficient mice are severely hypoactive, adipsic, and aphagic. Cell (1995) 83:1197–209. 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90145-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
68. Church WH, Justice JB, Neill DB. Detecting behaviorally relevant changes in extracellular dopamine with microdialysis. Brain Res. (1987) 412:397–9. 10.1016/0006-8993(87)91150-4 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
69. Hernandez L, Hoebel BG. Food reward and cocaine increase extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens as measured by microdialysis. Life Sci. (1988) 42:1705–12. 10.1016/0024-3205(88)90036-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
70. Ishiwari K, Weber SM, Mingote S, Correa M, Salamone JD. Accumbens dopamine and the regulation of effort in food-seeking behavior: modulation of work output by different ratio or force requirements. Behav Brain Res. (2004) 151:83–91. 10.1016/j.bbr.2003.08.007 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
71. Hernandez L, Hoebel BG. Feeding and hypothalamic stimulation increase dopamine turnover in the accumbens. Physiol Behav. (1988) 44:599–606. [PubMed]
72. Mark GP, Rada P, Pothos E, Hoebel BG. Effects of feeding and drinking on acetylcholine release in the nucleus accumbens, striatum, and hippocampus of freely behaving rats. J Neurochem. (1992) 58:2269–74. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1992.tb10973.x [PubMed] [CrossRef]
73. Yoshida M, Yokoo H, Mizoguchi K, Kawahara H, Tsuda A, Nishikawa T, et al. . Eating and drinking cause increased dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens and ventral tegmental area in the rat: measurement by in vivo microdialysis. Neurosci Lett. (1992) 139:73–6. [PubMed]
74. Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Differential influence of associative and nonassociative learning mechanisms on the responsiveness of prefrontal and accumbal dopamine transmission to food stimuli in rats fed ad libitum. J Neurosci. (1997) 17:851–61 10.1177/1087054705277198 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
75. Bassareo V, Di Chiara G. Differential responsiveness of dopamine transmission to food-stimuli in nucleus accumbens shell/core compartments. Neuroscience (1999) 89:637–41. [PubMed]
76. Hajnal A, Norgren R. Repeated access to sucrose augments dopamine turnover in the nucleus accumbens. Neuroreport (2002) 13:2213–6. 10.1097/01.wnr.0000044213.09266.38 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
77. Liang N-C, Hajnal A, Norgren R. Sham feeding corn oil increases accumbens dopamine in the rat. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2006) 291:R1236–9. 10.1152/ajpregu.00226.2006 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
78. Mark GP, Blander DS, Hoebel BG. A conditioned stimulus decreases extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens after the development of a learned taste aversion. Brain Res. (1991) 551:308–10. [PubMed]
79. Rada P, Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly releases dopamine in the accumbens shell. Neuroscience (2005) 134:737–44. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.04.043 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
80. Rada P, Avena NM, Barson JR, Hoebel BG, Leibowitz SF. A high-fat meal, or intraperitoneal administration of a fat emulsion, increases extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens. Brain Sci. (2012) 2:242–53. 10.3390/brainsci2020242 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
81. Wilson C, Nomikos GG, Collu M, Fibiger HC. Dopaminergic correlates of motivated behavior: importance of drive. J Neurosci. (1995) 15:5169–78. [PubMed]
82. Ahn S, Phillips AG. Dopaminergic correlates of sensory-specific satiety in the medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens of the rat. J Neurosci. (1999) 19:RC29. [PubMed]
83. Schultz W. Reward functions of the basal ganglia. J Neural Transm. (2016) 123:679–93. 10.1007/s00702-016-1510-0 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
84. Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science (1997) 275:1593–9. 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
85. Kosheleff AR, Araki J, Hsueh J, Le A, Quizon K, Ostlund SB, et al. . Pattern of access determines influence of junk food diet on cue sensitivity and palatability. Appetite (2018) 123:135–45. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.12.009 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
86. Bomberg EM, Grace MK, Wirth MM, Levine AS, Olszewski PK. Central ghrelin induces feeding driven by energy needs not by reward. Neuroreport (2007) 18:591–5. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280b07bb5 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
87. Gosnell BA. Central structures involved in opioid-induced feeding. Fed Proc. (1987) 46:163–7. [PubMed]
88. Kyrkouli SE, Stanley BG, Seirafi RD, Leibowitz SF. Stimulation of feeding by galanin: anatomical localization and behavioral specificity of this peptide’s effects in the brain. Peptides (1990) 11:995–1001. [PubMed]
89. Kyrkouli, Stavroula E, Stanley GB, Leibowitz SF. Galanin: stimulation of feeding induced by medial hypothalamic injection of this novel peptide. Eur J Pharmacol. (1986) 122:159–60. [PubMed]
90. Olszewski PK, Grace MK, Billington CJ, Levine AS. Hypothalamic paraventricular injections of ghrelin: Effect on feeding and c-Fos immunoreactivity. Peptides. (2003) 24:919–23. 10.1016/S0196-9781(03)00159-1 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
91. Quinn JG, O’Hare E, Levine AS, Kim EM. Evidence for a μ-opioid-opioid connection between the paraventricular nucleus and ventral tegmental area in the rat. Brain Res. (2003) 991:206–11. 10.1016/j.brainres.2003.08.020 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
92. Stanley BG, Lanthier D, Leibowitz SF. Multiple brain sites sensitive to feeding stimulation by opioid agonists: a cannula-mapping study. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1988) 31:825–32. [PubMed]
93. Rada P, Mark GP, Hoebel BG. Galanin in the hypothalamus raises dopamine and lowers acetylcholine release in the nucleus accumbens: a possible mechanism for hypothalamic initiation of feeding behavior. Brain Res. (1998) 798:1–6. [PubMed]
94. Rada P, Barson JR, Leibowitz SF, Hoebel BG. Opioids in the hypothalamus control dopamine and acetylcholine levels in the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. (2010) 1312:1–9. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.055 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
95. Quarta D, Di Francesco C, Melotto S, Mangiarini L, Heidbreder C, Hedou G. Systemic administration of ghrelin increases extracellular dopamine in the shell but not the core subdivision of the nucleus accumbens. Neurochem Int. (2009) 54:89–94. 10.1016/j.neuint.2008.12.006 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
96. Helm KA, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Cholecystokinin combined with serotonin in the hypothalamus limits accumbens dopamine release while increasing acetylcholine: A possible satiation mechanism. Brain Res. (2003) 963:290–7. 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)04051-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
97. Zigman JM, Jones JE, Lee CE, Saper CB, Elmquist JK. Expression of ghrelin receptor mRNA in the rat and the mouse brain. J Comp Neurol. (2006) 494:528–48. 10.1002/cne.20823 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
98. Abizaid A, Liu Z-W, Andrews ZB, Shanabrough M, Borok E, Elsworth JD, et al. . Ghrelin modulates the activity and synaptic input organization of midbrain dopamine neurons while promoting appetite. J Clin Invest. (2006) 116:3229–39. 10.1172/JCI29867 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
99. Overduin J, Figlewicz DP, Bennett-Jay J, Kittleson S, Cummings DE. Ghrelin increases the motivation to eat, but does not alter food palatability. Am J Physiol Integr Comp Physiol. (2012) 303:R259–69. 10.1152/ajpregu.00488.2011 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
100. Perello M, Dickson SL. Ghrelin signalling on food reward: a salient link between the gut and the mesolimbic system. J Neuroendocrinol. (2015) 27:424–34. 10.1111/jne.12236 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
101. Pal GK, Thombre DP. Modulation of feeding and drinking by dopamine in caudate and accumbens nuclei in rats. Indian J Exp Biol. (1993) 31:750–4. [PubMed]
102. Swanson CJ, Heath S, Stratford TR, Kelley AE. Differential behavioral responses to dopaminergic stimulation of nucleus accumbens subregions in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1997) 58:933–45. 10.1016/S0091-3057(97)00043-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
103. Bakshi VP, Kelley AE. Sensitization and conditioning of feeding following multiple morphine microinjections into the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res. (1994) 648:342–6. 10.1016/0006-8993(94)91139-8 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
104. Baldo BA, Sadeghian K, Basso AM, Kelley AE. Effects of selective dopamine D1 or D2 receptor blockade within nucleus accumbens subregions on ingestive behavior and associated motor activity. Behav Brain Res. (2002) 137:165–77. 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00293-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
105. Boekhoudt L, Roelofs TJM, de Jong JW, de Leeuw AE, Luijendijk MCM, Wolterink-Donselaar IG, et al. Does activation of midbrain dopamine neurons promote or reduce feeding? Int J Obes. (2017) 41:1131–40. 10.1038/ijo.2017.74 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
106. Dourmashkin JT, Chang G-Q, Hill JO, Gayles EC, Fried SK, Leibowitz SF. Model for predicting and phenotyping at normal weight the long-term propensity for obesity in Sprague–Dawley rats. Physiol Behav. (2006) 87:666–78. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.008 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
107. Geiger BM, Behr GG, Frank LE, Caldera-Siu AD, Beinfeld MC, Kokkotou EG, et al. . Evidence for defective mesolimbic dopamine exocytosis in obesity-prone rats. FASEB J. (2008) 22:2740–6. 10.1096/fj.08-110759 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
108. Rada P, Bocarsly ME, Barson JR, Hoebel BG, Leibowitz SF. Reduced accumbens dopamine in Sprague-Dawley rats prone to overeating a fat-rich diet. Physiol Behav. (2010) 101:394–400. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.07.005 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
109. Geiger BM, Haburcak M, Avena NM, Moyer MC, Hoebel BG, Pothos EN. Deficits of mesolimbic dopamine neurotransmission in rat dietary obesity. Neuroscience (2009) 159:1193–9. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.02.007 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
110. Stice E, Spoor S, Bohon C, Small DM. Relation between obesity and blunted striatal response to food is moderated by TaqIA A1 allele. Science (2008) 322:449–52. 10.1126/science.1161550 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
111. Kessler RM, Zald DH, Ansari MS, Li R, Cowan RL. Changes in dopamine release and dopamine D2/3 receptor levels with the development of mild obesity. Synapse (2014) 68:317–20. 10.1002/syn.21738 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
112. Volkow ND, Wang G, Fowler JS, Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity : evidence of systems pathology. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2008) 363:3191–200. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0107 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
113. Blum K, Sheridan PJ, Wood RC, Braverman ER, Chen TJ, Cull JG, et al. . The D2 dopamine receptor gene as a determinant of reward deficiency syndrome. J R Soc Med. (1996) 89:396–400. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
114. Blum K, Oscar-Berman M, Giordano J, Downs B, Simpatico T, Han D, et al. . Neurogenetic impairments of brain reward circuitry links to Reward Deficiency Syndrome (RDS): potential nutrigenomic induced dopaminergic activation. J Genet Syndr Gene Ther. (2012) 3:1000e115. 10.4172/2157-7412.1000e115 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
115. Blum K, Oscar-Berman M, Barh D, Giordano J, Gold M. Dopamine genetics and function in food and substance abuse. J Genet Syndr Gene Ther. (2013) 4:1000121. 10.4172/2157-7412.1000121 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
116. Kawamura Y, Takahashi T, Liu X, Nishida N, Noda Y, Yoshikawa A, et al. Variation in the DRD2 gene affects impulsivity in intertemporal choice. Open J Psychiatry (2013) 3:26–31. 10.4236/ojpsych.2013.31005 [CrossRef]
117. Mikhailova MA, Bass CE, Grinevich VP, Chappell AM, Deal AL, Bonin KD, et al. . Optogenetically-induced tonic dopamine release from VTA-nucleus accumbens projections inhibits reward consummatory behaviors. Neuroscience (2016) 333:54–64. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.07.006 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
118. Grace AA. The tonic/phasic model of dopamine system regulation and its implications for understanding alcohol and psychostimulant craving. Addiction (2000) 95:119–28. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.95.8s2.1.x [PubMed] [CrossRef]
119. Wightman RM, Robinson DL. Transient changes in mesolimbic dopamine and their association with “reward.” J Neurochem. (2002) 82:721–35. 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2002.01005.x [PubMed] [CrossRef]
120. Di Chiara G, Imperato A. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (1988) 85:5274–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
121. Mifsud JC, Hernandez L, Hoebel BG. Nicotine infused into the nucleus accumbens increases synaptic dopamine as measured by in vivo microdialysis. Brain Res. (1989) 478:365–7. [PubMed]
122. Nisell M, Nomikos GG, Svensson TH. Systemic nicotine-induced dopamine release in the rat nucleus accumbens is regulated by nicotinic receptors in the ventral tegmental area. Synapse (1994) 16:36–44. 10.1002/syn.890160105 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
123. Bozarth MA, Wise RA. Intracranial self-administration of morphine into the ventral tegmental area in rats. Life Sci. (1981) 28:551–5. [PubMed]
124. Glimcher PW, Giovino AA, Margolin DH, Hoebel BG. Endogenous opiate reward induced by an enkephalinase inhibitor, thiorphan, injected into the ventral midbrain. Behav Neurosci. (1984) 98:262–8. [PubMed]
125. McBride WJ, Murphy JM, Ikemoto S. Localization of brain reinforcement mechanisms: intracranial self-administration and intracranial place-conditioning studies. Behav Brain Res. (1999) 101:129–52. [PubMed]
126. McKinzie DL, Rodd-Henricks ZA, Dagon CT, Murphy JM, McBride WJ. Cocaine is self-administered into the shell region of the nucleus accumbens in Wistar rats. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1999) 877:788–91. [PubMed]
127. Trifilieff P, Ducrocq F, van der Veldt S, Martinez D. Blunted dopamine transmission in addiction: potential mechanisms and implications for behavior. Semin Nucl Med. (2017) 47:64–74. 10.1053/j.semnuclmed.2016.09.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
128. Volkow ND, Wise RA. How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat Neurosci. (2005) 8:555–60. 10.1038/nn1452 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
129. Bocarsly ME, Barson JR, Hauca JM, Hoebel BG, Leibowitz SF, Avena NM. Effects of perinatal exposure to palatable diets on body weight and sensitivity to drugs of abuse in rats. Physiol Behav. (2012) 107:568–75. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.04.024 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
130. Nicolas C, Lafay-Chebassier C, Solinas M. Exposure to sucrose during periods of withdrawal does not reduce cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:23272. 10.1038/srep23272 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
131. Meredith GE, Blank B, Groenewegen HJ. The distribution and compartmental organization of the cholinergic neurons in nucleus accumbens of the rat. Neuroscience (1989) 31:327–45. [PubMed]
132. Bolam JP, Wainer BH, Smith AD. Characterization of cholinergic neurons in the rat neostriatum. A combination of choline acetyltransferase immunocytochemistry, Golgi-impregnation and electron microscopy. Neuroscience (1984) 12:711–8. [PubMed]
133. Phelps PE, Vaughn JE. Immunocytochemical localization of choline acetyltransferase in rat ventral striatum: a light and electron microscopic study. J Neurocytol. (1986) 15:595–617. [PubMed]
134. Katzenschlager R, Sampaio C, Costa J, Lees A. Anticholinergics for symptomatic management of Parkinson’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2002) 2002:CD003735 10.1002/14651858.CD003735 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
135. Xiang Z, Thompson AD, Jones CK, Lindsley CW, Conn PJ. Roles of the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor subtype in the regulation of basal ganglia function and implications for the treatment of Parkinson’s Disease. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. (2012) 340:595–603. 10.1124/jpet.111.187856 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
136. DeBoer P, Abercrombie ED, Heeringa M, Westerink BHC. Differential effect of systemic administration of bromocriptine and l-DOPA on the release of acetylcholine from striatum of intact and 6-OHDA-treated rats. Brain Res. (1993) 608:198–203. 10.1016/0006-8993(93)91459-6 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
137. Hagino Y, Kasai S, Fujita M, Setogawa S, Yamaura H, Yanagihara D, et al. . Involvement of cholinergic system in hyperactivity in dopamine-deficient mice. Neuropsychopharmacology (2015) 40:1141–50. 10.1038/npp.2014.295 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
138. Affaticati A, Lidia Gerra M, Amerio A, Inglese M, Antonioni MC, Marchesi C. The controversial case of biperiden from prescription drug to drug of abuse. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2015) 35:749–50. 10.1097/JCP.0000000000000421 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
139. Modell JG, Tandon R, Beresford TP. Dopaminergic activity of the antimuscarinic antiparkinsonian agents. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (1989) 9:347–51. [PubMed]
140. Hoebel BG, Avena NM, Rada P. Accumbens dopamine-acetylcholine balance in approach and avoidance. Curr Opin Pharmacol. (2007) 7:617–27. 10.1016/j.coph.2007.10.014 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
141. Avena NM, Rada P, Moise N, Hoebel BG. Sucrose sham feeding on a binge schedule releases accumbens dopamine repeatedly and eliminates the acetylcholine satiety response. Neuroscience (2006) 139:813–820. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.12.037 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
142. Mark GP, Shabani S, Dobbs LK, Hansen ST, Health O. Cholinergic modulation of mesolimbic dopamine function and reward. Physiol Behav. (2011) 104:76–81. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.052 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
143. Rada PV, Hoebel BG. Supraadditive effect of d-fenfluramine plus phentermine on extracellular acetylcholine in the nucleus accumbens: possible mechanism for inhibition of excessive feeding and drug abuse. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2000) 65:369–73. 10.1016/S0091-3057(99)00219-1 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
144. Aitta-aho T, Phillips BU, Pappa E, Hay YA, Harnischfeger F, Heath CJ, et al. . Accumbal cholinergic interneurons differentially influence motivation related to satiety signaling. Eneuro (2017) 4:ENEURO.0328-16.2017. 10.1523/ENEURO.0328-16.2017 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
145. Mark GP, Weinberg JB, Rada PV, Hoebel BG. Extracellular acetylcholine is increased in the nucleus accumbens following the presentation of an aversively conditioned taste stimulus. Brain Res. (1995) 688:184–8. [PubMed]
146. Taylor KM, Mark GP, Hoebel BG. Conditioned taste aversion from neostigmine or methyl-naloxonium in the nucleus accumbens. Physiol Behav. (2011) 104:82–6. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.050 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
147. Rada P, Pothos E, Mark GP, Hoebel BG. Microdialysis evidence that acetylcholine in the nucleus accumbens is involved in morphine withdrawal and its treatment with clonidine. Brain Res. (1991) 561:354–6. [PubMed]
148. Rada P, Mark GP, Pothos E, Hoebel BG. Systemic morphine simultaneously decreases extracellular acetylcholine increases dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. Neuropharmacology (1991) 30:1133–36. [PubMed]
149. Rada PV, Mark GP, Taylor KM, Hoebel BG. Morphine and naloxone, i.p. or locally, affect extracellular acetylcholine in the accumbens and prefrontal cortex. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1996) 53:809–16. [PubMed]
150. Rada P, Hoebel BG. Acetylcholine in the accumbens is decreased by diazepam and increased by benzodiazepine withdrawal: a possible mechanism for dependency. Eur J Pharmacol. (2005) 508:131–8. [PubMed]
151. Rada P, Jensen K, Hoebel BG. Effects of nicotine and mecamylamine-induced withdrawal on extracellular dopamine and acetylcholine in the rat nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology (2001) 157:105–10. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2004.12.016 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
152. Hurd YL, Weiss F, Koob G, Ungerstedt U. The influence of cocaine self-administration on in vivo dopamine and acetylcholine neurotransmission in rat caudate-putamen. Neurosci Lett. (1990) 109:227–33. [PubMed]
153. Consolo S, Caltavuturo C, Colli E, Recchia M, Di Chiara G. Different sensitivity of in vivo acetylcholine transmission to D1 receptor stimulation in shell and core of nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience (1999) 89:1209–17. [PubMed]
154. Hikida T, Kaneko S, Isobe T, Kitabatake Y, Watanabe D, Pastan I, et al. . Increased sensitivity to cocaine by cholinergic cell ablation in nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2001) 98:13351–4. 10.1073/pnas.231488998 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
155. Rada P, Johnson DF, Lewis MJ, Hoebel BG. In alcohol-treated rats, naloxone decreases extracellular dopamine and increases acetylcholine in the nucleus accumbens: evidence of opioid withdrawal. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2004) 79:599–605. 10.1016/j.pbb.2004.09.011 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
156. Hikida T, Kitabatake Y, Pastan I, Nakanishi S. Acetylcholine enhancement in the nucleus accumbens prevents addictive behaviors of cocaine and morphine. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2003) 100:6169–73. 10.1073/pnas.0631749100 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
157. Pothos EN, Rada P, Mark GP, Hoebel BG. Dopamine microdlalysis in the nucleus accumbens during acute and chronic morphine, naloxone-precipitated withdrawal and clonidine treatment. Brain Res. (1991) 566:348–50. [PubMed]
158. Zombeck JA, Chen G-T, Johnson ZV, Rosenberg DM, Craig AB, Rhodes JS. Neuroanatomical specificity of conditioned responses to cocaine versus food in mice. Physiol Behav. (2008) 93:637–50. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.11.004 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
159. Pressman P, Clemens R, Rodriguez H. Food addiction: clinical reality or mythology. Am J Med. (2015) 128:1165–6. 10.1016/j.amjmed.2015.05.046 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
160. Rogers P. Food and drug addiction: similarities and differences. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2017) 153:182–90. 10.1016/j.pbb.2017.01.001 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
161. Hebebrand J, Albayrak O, Adan R, Antel J, Dieguez C, de Jong J, et al. . “Eating addiction” rather than “food addiction”, better captures addictive-like behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 47:295–306. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.016 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
162. Naneix F, Darlot F, Coutureau E, Cador M. Long-lasting deficits in hedonic and nucleus accumbens reactivity to sweet rewards by sugar overconsumption during adolescence. Eur J Neurosci. (2016) 43:671–80. 10.1111/ejn.13149 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
163. Vendruscolo LF, Gueye AB, Vendruscolo JCM, Clemens KJ, Mormède P, Darnaudéry M, et al. . Reduced alcohol drinking in adult rats exposed to sucrose during adolescence. Neuropharmacology (2010) 59:388–94. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2010.05.015 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
164. Vendruscolo LF, Gueye AB, Darnaudéry M, Ahmed SH, Cador M. Sugar overconsumption during adolescence selectively alters motivation and reward function in adult rats. PLoS ONE (2010) 5:e9296. 10.1371/journal.pone.0009296 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
165. Wei Z, Zhang X. Similarities and differences in diagnostic criterion. In Substance and Non-substance Addiction. Zhang X, editor. Singapore: Springer Nature; (2017). p. 105–132.
166. Avena N, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Sugar bingeing in rats. Curr Protoc Neurosci. (2006) Chapter 9:Unit9.23C. 10.1002/0471142301.ns0923cs36 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
167. Colantuoni C, Schwenker J, McCarthy J, Rada P, Ladenheim B, Cadet JL, et al. . Excessive sugar intake alters binding to dopamine and mu-opioid receptors in the brain. Neuroreport (2001) 12:3549–52. [PubMed]
168. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2008) 32:20–39. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
169. Gerber GJ, Wise RA. Pharmacological regulation of intravenous cocaine and heroin self-administration in rats: a variable dose paradigm. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1989) 32:527–31. [PubMed]
170. Mutschler NH, Miczek KA. Withdrawal from a self-administration or non-contingent cocaine binge: differences in ultrasonic distress vocalizations in rats. Psychopharmacology (1998) 136:402–8. [PubMed]
171. O‘Brien CP, Childress AR, Ehrman R, Robbins SJ. Conditioning factors in drug abuse: can they explain compulsion? J Psychopharmacol. (1998) 12:15–22. [PubMed]
172. Klenowski PM, Shariff MR, Belmer A, Fogarty MJ, Mu EWH, Bellingham MC, et al. . Prolonged consumption of sucrose in a binge-like manner, alters the morphology of medium spiny neurons in the nucleus accumbens shell. Front Behav Neurosci. (2016) 10:54. 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00054 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
173. Skibicka KP, Hansson C, Egecioglu E, Dickson SL. Role of ghrelin in food reward: impact of ghrelin on sucrose self-administration and mesolimbic dopamine and acetylcholine receptor gene expression. (2011) 17:95–107 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00294.x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
174. Jerlhag E, Egecioglu E, Landgren S, Salome N, Heilig M, Moechars D, et al. . Requirement of central ghrelin signaling for alcohol reward. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2009) 106:11318–23. 10.1073/pnas.0812809106 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
175. Leggio L, Ferrulli A, Cardone S, Nesci A, Miceli A, Malandrino N, et al. . Ghrelin system in alcohol-dependent subjects: role of plasma ghrelin levels in alcohol drinking and craving. Addict Biol. (2012) 17:452–64. 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00308.x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
176. Dickson SL, Egecioglu E, Landgren S, Skibicka KP, Engel JA, Jerlhag E. The role of the central ghrelin system in reward from food and chemical drugs. Mol Cell Endocrinol. (2011) 340:80–7. 10.1016/j.mce.2011.02.017 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
177. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Neurobiology of Addiction. San Diego: Academic Press; (2005).
178. Crombag HS, Bossert JM, Koya E, Shaham Y. Context-induced relapse to drug seeking: a review. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2008) 363:3233–43. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0090 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
179. Bienkowski P, Rogowski A, Korkosz A, Mierzejewski P, Radwanska K, Kaczmarek L, et al. . Time-dependent changes in alcohol-seeking behaviour during abstinence. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2004) 14:355–60. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2003.10.005 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
180. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Shaham Y. Neuroadaptation. Incubation of cocaine craiving after withdrawal. Nature (2001) 412:141–2. 10.1038/35084134 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
181. Le AD, Shaham Y. Neurobiology of relapse to alcohol in rats. Pharmacol Ther. (2002) 94:137–56. 10.1016/S0163-7258(02)00200-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
182. Lu L, Grimm JW, Hope BT, Shaham Y. Incubation of cocaine craving after withdrawal: a review of preclinical data. Neuropharmacology (2004) 47:214–26. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.06.027 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
183. Sinclair JD, Senter RJ. Development of an alcohol-deprivation effect in rats. Q J Stud Alcohol. (1968) 29:863–67. [PubMed]
184. Grimm JW, Fyall AM, Osincup DP, Wells B. Incubation of sucrose craving: effects of reduced training and sucrose pre-loading. Physiol Behav. (2005) 84:73–9. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.10.011 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
185. Grimm JW, Manaois M, Osincup D, Wells B, Buse C. Naloxone attenuates incubated sucrose craving in rats. Psychopharmacology (2007) 194:537–44. 10.1007/s00213-007-0868-y [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
186. Aoyama K, Barnes J, Grimm JW. Incubation of saccharin craving and within-session changes in responding for a cue previously associated with saccharin. Appetite (2014) 72:114–22. 10.1016/j.appet.2013.10.003 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
187. Avena NM, Long KA, T BGH. Sugar-dependent rats show enhanced responding for sugar after abstinence : Evidence of a sugar deprivation effect. Physiol Behav. (2005) 84:359–62. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.12.016 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
188. Vanderschuren LJMJ, Everitt BJ. Drug seeking becomes compulsive after prolonged cocaine self-admnistration. Science (2004) 305:1017–20. 10.1126/science.1098975 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
189. Patrono E, Segni M Di, Patella L, Andolina D, Pompili A, Gasbarri A, et al. . Compulsion : gene-environment interplay. Plos ONE (2015)10:e0120191. 10.1371/journal.pone.0120191 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
190. Latagliata EC, Patrono E, Puglisi-allegra S, Ventura R. Food seeking in spite of harmful consequences is under prefrontal cortical noradrenergic control. BioMed Cent Neurosci. (2010) 11:15–29. 10.1186/1471-2202-11-15 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
191. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nat Neurosci. (2010) 13:635–41. 10.1038/nn.2519 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
192. Teegarden SL, Bale TL. Decreases in dietary preference produce increased emotionality and risk for dietary relapse. Biol Psychiatry (2007) 61:1021–9. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.032 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
193. Mcsweeney FK, Murphy ES, Kowal BP. Regulation of drug taking by sensitization and habituation. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2005) 13:163–84. 10.1037/1064-1297.13.3.163 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
194. File SE, Lippa AS, Beer B, Lippa MT. Animal tests of anxiety. Curr Protoc Neurosci. (2004)unit 8.4. 10.1002/0471142301.ns0803s26 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
195. Schulteis G, Yackey M, Risbrough V, Koob GF. Anxiogenic-like effects of spontaneous and naloxone-precipitated opiate withdrawal in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (1998) 60:727–31. [PubMed]
196. Porsolt RD, Anton G, Blavet N, Jalfre M. Behavioural despair in rats: a new model sensitive to antidepressant tretaments. Eur J Pharmacol. (1978) 47:379–91. [PubMed]
197. Anraku T, Ikegaya Y, Matsuki N, Nishiyama N. Withdrawal from chronic morphine administration causes prolonged enhancement of immobility in rat forced swimming test. Psychopharmacology (2001) 157:217–20. 10.1007/s002130100793 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
198. de Freitas RL, Kübler JML, Elias-Filho DH, Coimbra NC. Antinociception induced by acute oral administration of sweet substance in young and adult rodents: The role of endogenous opioid peptides chemical mediators and μ1-opioid receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2012) 101:265–70. 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.12.005 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
199. Le Magnen J. A role for opiates in food reward and food addiction. In: Capaldi PT, editor. Taste, Experience and Feeding. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; (1990), 241–252.
200. Kim S, Shou J, Abera S, Ziff EB. Neuropharmacology Sucrose withdrawal induces depression and anxiety-like behavior by Kir2. 1 upregulation in the nucleus accumbens. Neuropharmacology (2018) 130:10–7. 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2017.11.041 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
201. Colantuoni C, Rada P, McCarthy J, Patten C, Avena NM, Chadeayne A, et al. . Evidence that intermittent, excessive sugar intake causes endogenous opioid dependence. Obes Res. (2002) 10:478–88. 10.1038/oby.2002.66 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
202. Avena NM, Bocarsly ME, Rada P, Kim A, Hoebel BG. After daily bingeing on a sucrose solution, food deprivation induces anxiety and accumbens dopamine/acetylcholine imbalance. Physiol Behav. (2008) 94:309–15. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.01.008 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
203. De Ridder D, Manning P, Leong SL, Ross S, Vanneste S. Allostasis in health and food addiction. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:37126. 10.1038/srep37126 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
204. Kalivas PW, Striplin CD, Steketee JD, Kljtenick MA. Cellular mechanisms of behavioral sensitization to drugs of abuse. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (1992) 654:128–35. [PubMed]
205. Landa L, Machalova A, Sulcova A. Implication of NMDA receptors in behavioural sensitization to psychostimulants : a short review. Eur J Pharmacol. (2014) 730:77–81. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.02.028 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
206. Robinson TE, Kent C. reviews The neural basis of drug craving : an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev. (1993) 18:165–73. [PubMed]
207. Steketee JD, Kalivas PW. Drug wanting : behavioral sensitization and relapse to drug-seeking behavior. Pharmacol Rev. (2011) 63:348–65. 10.1124/pr.109.001933.remains [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
208. Cadoni C, Valentini V, Di Chiara G. Behavioral sensitization to delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cross-sensitization with morphine: differential changes in accumbal shell and core dopamine transmission. J Neurochem. (2008) 106:1586–93. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05503.x [PubMed] [CrossRef]
209. Itzhak Y, Martin JL. Effects of cocaine, nicotine, dizocipline and alcohol on mice locomotor activity : cocaine—alcohol cross-sensitization involves upregulation of striatal dopamine transporter binding sites. Brain Res. (1999) 818:204–11. [PubMed]
210. Avena NM, Hoebel BG. A diet promoting sugar dependency causes behavioral cross-sensitization to a low dose of amphetamine. Neuroscience (2003) 122:17–20. 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00502-5 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
211. Avena NM, Hoebel BG. Amphetamine-sensitized rats show sugar-induced hyperactivity (cross-sensitization) and sugar hyperphagia. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2003) 74:635–639. [PubMed]
212. Gosnell BA. Sucrose intake enhances behavioral sensitization produced by cocaine. Brain Res. (2005) 1031:194–201. 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.10.037 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
213. Pastor R, Kamens HM, Mckinnon CS, Ford MM, Phillips TJ. Repeated ethanol administration modifies the temporal structure of sucrose intake patterns in mice : effects associated with behavioral sensitization. Addict Biol. (2010) 15:324–35. 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2010.00229.x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
214. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2008) 363:3137–46. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
215. Blum K, Thanos PK, Gold MS. Dopamine and glucose, obesity, and reward deficiency syndrome. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:919. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00919 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
216. Val-Laillet D, Aarts E, Weber B, Ferrari M, Quaresima V, Stoeckel LE, et al. . Neuroimaging and neuromodulation approaches to study eating behavior and prevent and treat eating disorders and obesity. NeuroImage Clin. (2015) 8:1–31. 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.03.016 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
217. Markus C, Rogers P, Brouns F, Schepers R. Eating dependence and weight gain; no human evidence for a “sugar-addiction” model of overweight. Appetite (2017) 114:64–72. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.024 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
218. Kandel DB, Yamaguchi K, Chen K. Stages of profression in drug involvement from adolescence to adulthood: further evidence for the gateway theory. J Stud Alcohol. (1992) 53:447–57. [PubMed]
219. Ellgren M, Spano SM, Hurd YL. Adolescent cannabis exposure alters opiate intake and opioid limbic neuronal populations in adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology (2007) 32:607–15. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301127 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
220. Griffin EA, Jr, Melas PA, Zhou R, Li Y, Mercado P, Kempadoo KA, et al. . Prior alcohol use enhances vulnerability to compulsive cocaine self-administration by promoting degradation of HDAC4 and HDAC5. Sci Adv. (2017) 3:e1791682. 10.1126/sciadv.1701682 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
221. Mateos-Garcia A, Manzanedo C, Rodriguez-Arias M, Aguilar MA, Reig-Sanchis E, Navarro-Frances CI, et al. . Sex differences in the long-lasting consequences of adolescent ethanol exposure for the rewarding effects of cocaine in mice. Psychopharmacology (2015) 232:2995–3007. 10.1007/s00213-015-3937-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
222. Avena NM, Carrillo CA, Needham L, Leibowitz SF, Hoebel BG. Sugar-dependent rats show enhanced intake of unsweetened ethanol. Alcohol (2004) 34:203–9. 10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.09.006 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
223. Unterwald EM, Jeanne M, Cuntapay M. The frequency of cocaine administration impacts cocaine-induced receptor alterations. Brain Res. (2001) 900:103–9. 10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02269-7 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
224. Nader M, Daunais JB, Moore RJ, Smith HR, Friedman DP, Porrino LJ. Effects of cocaine self-administration on striatal dopamine systems in rhesus monkeys : initial and chronic exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology (2002) 27:35–46. 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00427-4 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
225. Keramati M, Durand A, Girardeau P, Gutkin B, Ahmed SH. Cocaine addiction as a homeostatic reinforcement learning disorder. Psychol Rev. (2017) 124:130–53. 10.1037/rev0000046 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
226. Volkow ND, Morales M. The brain on drugs : from reward to addiction. Cell (2015) 162:712–25. 10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.046 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
227. Park K, Volkow ND, Pan Y, Du C. Chronic cocaine dampens dopamine signaling during cocaine intoxication and unbalances D 1 over D 2 receptor signaling. J Neurosci. (2013) 33:15827–36. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1935-13.2013 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
228. Michaelides M, Thanos PK, Kim R, Cho J, Ananth M, Wang G, et al. NeuroImage PET imaging predicts future body weight and cocaine preference. Neuroimage (2012) 59:1508–13. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.028 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
229. Ashok AH, Mizuno Y, Volkow ND, Howes OD. Association of stimulant use with dopaminergic alterations in users of cocaine, amphetamine, or methamphetamine a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry (2017) 4:511–9. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0135 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
230. Bello NT, Lucas LR. Repeated sucrose access influences dopamine D2 receptor density in the striatum. Neuroreport (2007) 13:1575–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
231. Levy A, Marshall P, Zhou Y, Kreek MJ, Kent K, Daniels S, et al. . Fructose: glucose ratios- A study of sugar self-administration and associated neural and physiological responses in the rat. Nutrients (2015) 7:3869–90. 10.3390/nu7053869 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
232. Spangler R, Wittkowski KM, Goddard NL, Avena NM, Hoebel BG, Leibowitz SF. Opiate-like effects of sugar on gene expression in reward areas of the rat brain. Mol Brain Res. (2004) 124:134–42. 10.1016/j.molbrainres.2004.02.013 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
233. Wiss DA, Criscitelli K, Gold M, Avena N. Preclinical evidence for the addiction potential of highly palatable foods: current developments related to maternal influence. Appetite (2017) 115:19–27. 10.1016/j.appet.2016.12.019 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
234. Naef L, Moquin L, Dal Bo G, Giros B, Gratton A, Walker C-D. Maternal high-fat intake alters presynaptic regulation of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and increases motivation for fat rewards in the offspring. Neuroscience (2011) 176:225–36. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.12.037 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
235. Kendig MD, Ekayanti W, Stewart H, Boakes RA, Rooney K. Metabolic effects of access to sucrose drink in female rats and transmission of some effects to their offspring. PLoS ONE (2015) 10:e0131107. 10.1371/journal.pone.0131107 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
236. Carlin J, George R, Reyes TM. Methyl donor supplementation blocks the adverse effects of maternal high fat diet on offspring physiology. PLoS ONE (2013) 8:e63549. 10.1371/journal.pone.0063549 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
237. Grissom NM, Reyes TM. Gestational overgrowth and undergrowth affect neurodevelopment: similarities and differences from behavior to epigenetics. Int J Dev Neurosci. (2013) 31:406–14. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2012.11.006 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
238. Ponzio BF, Carvalho MHC, Fortes ZB, do Carmo Franco M. Implications of maternal nutrient restriction in transgenerational programming of hypertension and endothelial dysfunction across F1-F3 offspring. Life Sci. (2012) 90:571–7. 10.1016/j.lfs.2012.01.017 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
239. Jimenez-Chillaron JC, Isganaitis E, Charalambous M, Gesta S, Pentinat-Pelegrin T, Faucette RR, et al. . Intergenerational transmission of glucose intolerance and obesity by in utero undernutrition in mice. Diabetes (2009) 58:460–8. 10.2337/db08-0490 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
240. Vucetic Z, Kimmel J, Totoki K, Hollenbeck E, Reyes TM. Maternal high-fat diet alters methylation and gene expression of dopamine and opioid-related genes. Endocrinology (2010) 151:4756–64. 10.1210/en.2010-0505 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
241. Murray S, Tulloch A, Criscitelli K, Avena NM. Recent studies of the effects of sugars on brain systems involved in energy balance and reward: relevance to low calorie sweeteners. Physiol Behav. (2016) 164:504–8. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.04.004 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
242. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation of the yale food addiction scale. Appetite (2009) 52:430–6. 10.1016/j.appet.2008.12.003 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
243. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Development of the yale food addiction scale version 2.0. Psychol Addict Behav (2016) 30:113–121. 10.1037/adb0000136 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
244. de Vries S-K, Meule A. Food addiction and bulimia nervosa: new data based on the yale food addiction scale 2.0. Eur Eat Disord Rev. (2016) 24:518–22. 10.1002/erv.2470 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
245. Hauck C, Ellrott T, Schulte EM, Meule A. Prevalence of ‘food addiction’ as measured with the yale food addiction scale 2. 0 in a representative german sample and its association with sex, age and weight categories. Obes Facts (2017) 10:12–24. 10.1159/000456013 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
246. Pedram P, Wadden D, Amini P, Gulliver W, Randell E, Cahill F, et al. . Food addiction : its prevalence and significant association with obesity in the general population. Plos ONE (2013) 8:e0074832. 10.1371/journal.pone.0074832 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
247. Pursey KM, Stanwell P, Gearhardt AN, Collins CE, Burrows TL. The prevalence of food addiction as assessed by the yale food addiction scale: a systematic review. Nutrients (2014) 6:4552–90. 10.3390/nu6104552 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
248. Schulte EM, Gearhardt AN. Associations of food addiction in a sample recruited to be nationally representative of the United States. Eur Eat Disord Rev. (2017) 26:112–9. 10.1002/erv.2575 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
249. Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J. Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions III. Eur Eat Disord Rev. (2017) 72:757–66. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
250. Chou SP, Goldstein RB, Smith SM, Huang B, Ruan WJ, Zhang H. The epidemiology of DSM-5 nicotine use disorder: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. J Clin Psychiatry (2016) 77:1404–12. 10.4088/JCP.15m10114 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
251. Carter A, Hendrikse J, Lee N, Verdejo-garcia A, Andrews Z, Hall W. The neurobiology of “ food addiction ” and its implications for obesity treatment and policy. Annu Rev Nutr. (2016) 36:105–28. 10.1146/annurev-nutr-071715-050909 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
252. Ziauddeen H, Fletcher PC. Is food addiction a valid and useful concept ? Obes Rev. (2013) 14:19–28. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01046.x [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
253. Westwater ML, Fletcher PC, Ziauddeen H. Sugar addiction : the state of the science. Eur J Nutr. (2016) 55:55–69. 10.1007/s00394-016-1229-6 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
254. Ziauddeen H, Farooqi IS, Fletcher PC. Obesity and the brain: how convincing is the addiction model? Nat Rev Neurosci. (2012) 13:279–86. 10.1038/nrn3212 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
255. Wu M, Brockmeyer T, Hartmann M, Skunde M, Herzog W, Friederich H-C. Reward-related decision making in eating and weight disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the evidence from neuropsychological studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 61:177–96. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.017 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
256. Umberg EN, Shader RI, Hsu LKG, Greenblatt DJ. From disordered eating to addiction: “food drug” in bulimia nervosa. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2012) 32:376–89. 10.1097/JCP.0b013e318252464f [PubMed] [CrossRef]
257. Gearhardt AN, White MA, Masheb RM, Morgan PT, Crosby RD, Grilo CM. An examination of the food addiction construct in obese patients with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. (2012) 45:657–63. 10.1002/eat.20957 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
258. Schulte EM, Grilo CM, Gearhardt AN. Shared and unique mechanisms underlying binge eating disorder and addictive disorders. Clin Psychol Rev. (2016) 44:125–39. 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.02.001 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
259. Lacroix E, Tavares H, von Ranson K. Moving beyond the “eating addiction” versus “food addiction” debate. Comment on Schulte et al. (2017). Appetite (2018) 130:286–92. 10.1016/j.appet.2018.06.025 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
260. Meule A. Food Addiction and body-mass-index: a non-linear relationship. Med Hypothesis (2012) 79:508–11. 10.1016/j.mehy.2012.07.005 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
261. Meule A, von Rezori V, Blechert J. Food addiction and bulimia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. (2014) 22:331–7. 10.1002/erv.2306 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
262. Treasure J, Leslie M, Chami R, Fernández-Aranda F. Are trans diagnostic models of eating disorders fit for purpose? A consideration of the evidence for food addiction. Eur Eat Disord Rev. (2018) 26:83–91. 10.1002/erv.2578 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
263. Wiss DA, Brewerton TD. Incorporating food addiction into disordered eating: the disordered eating food addiction nutrition guide (DEFANG). Eat Weight Disord Stud Anorexia Bulim Obes. (2017) 22:49–59. 10.1007/s40519-016-0344-y [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
264. Hardy R, Fani N, Jovanovic T, Michopoulos V. Food addiction and substance addiction in women: Common clinical characteristics. Appetite (2018) 120:367–73. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.09.026 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
265. Canan F, Karaca S, Sogucak S, Gecici O, Kuloglu M. Eating disorders and food addiction in men with heroin use disorder: a controlled study. Eat Weight Disord Stud Anorexia Bulim Obes. (2017) 22:249–57. 10.1007/s40519-017-0378-9 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
266. Khan TA, Sievenpiper JL. Controversies about sugars : results from systematic reviews and meta – analyses on obesity, cardiometabolic disease and diabetes. Eur J Nutr. (2016) 55:25–43. 10.1007/s00394-016-1345-3 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
267. Rippe JM, Tappy L. Sweeteners and health : fi ndings from recent research and their impact on obesity and related metabolic conditions. Int J Obes. (2016) 40:S1–5. 10.1038/ijo.2016.7 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
268. Rippe JM, Angelopoulos TJ. Sucrose, high-fructose corn syrup, and fructose, their metabolism and potential health effects : what do we really know ? Advvances Nutr. (2013) 4:236–45. 10.3945/an.112.002824.236 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
269. Te Morenga L, Mallard S, Mann J. Dietary sugars and body weight : systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials and. BMJ (2013) 7492:1–25. 10.1136/bmj.e7492 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
270. Hu FB, Malik VS. Sugar-sweetened beverages and risk of obesity and type 2 diabetes : Epidemiologic evidence. Physiol Behav. (2010) 100:47–54. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.01.036 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
271. Baker P, Friel S. Processed foods and the nutrition transition : evidence from Asia. Obes Rev. (2014) 15:564–77. 10.1111/obr.12174 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
272. Swinburn B, Egger G. The runaway weight gain train: too many accelerators, not enough brakes. BMJ (2004) 329:736–9. 10.1136/bmj.329.7468.736 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
273. World Health Organization Globalization, Diets and Noncommunicable Diseases. Geneve: WHO IRIS; (2003).
274. Wiist WH. Public health and the anticorporate movement: rationale and recommendations. Am J Public Health (2006) 96:1370–5. 10.2105/AJPH.2005.072298 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
275. Gilmore AB, Savell E, Collin J. Public health, corporations and the New Responsibility Deal: promoting partnerships with vectors of disease? J Public Health (2011) 33:2–4. 10.1093/pubmed/fdr008 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
276. Moran A, Musicus A, Soo J, Gearhardt AN, Gollust SE, Roberto CA. Believing that certain foods are addictive is associated with support for obesity-related public policies. Prev Med. (2016) 90:39–46. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.06.018 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
277. Vella S-LC, Pai NB. A narrative review of potential treatment strategies for food addiction. Eat Weight Disord Stud Anorexia, Bulim Obes. (2017) 22:387–93. 10.1007/s40519-017-0400-2 [PubMed] [CrossRef]
278. Muele A. Are certain foods addictive? Front Psychiat. (2014) 5:38 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00038 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef]
279. Glass TA, McAtee MJ. Behavioral science at the crossroads in public health: Extending horizons, envisioning the future. Soc Sci Med. (2006) 62:1650–71. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.08.044 [PubMed] [CrossRef]