The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model for addictive behaviors: Update, generalization to addictive behaviors beyond Internet-use disorders, and specification of the process character of addictive behaviors (2019)

Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019 Jun 24. pii: S0149-7634(19)30370-7. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032.

Brand M1, Wegmann E2, Stark R3, Müller A4, Wölfling K5, Robbins TW6, Potenza MN7.

Highlights

  • Addictive behaviors are linked to cue-reactivity and craving
  • Addictive behaviors are associated with diminished inhibitory control
  • Habitual behaviors are developed in the process of addictive behaviors
  • An imbalance between fronto-striatal circuits contributes to addictive behaviors

Abstract

We propose an updated version of the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model, which we argue to be valid for several types of addictive behaviors, such as gambling, gaming, buying-shopping, and compulsive sexual behavior disorders. Based on recent empirical findings and theoretical considerations, we argue that addictive behaviors develop as a consequence of the interactions between predisposing variables, affective and cognitive responses to specific stimuli, and executive functions, such as inhibitory control and decision-making. In the process of addictive behaviors, the associations between cue-reactivity/craving and diminished inhibitory control contribute to the development of habitual behaviors. An imbalance between structures of fronto-striatal circuits, particularly between ventral striatum, amygdala, and dorsolateral prefrontal areas, may be particularly relevant to early stages and the dorsal striatum to later stages of addictive processes. The I-PACE model may provide a theoretical foundation for future studies on addictive behaviors and clinical practice. Future studies should investigate common and unique mechanisms involved in addictive, obsessive-compulsive-related, impulse-control, and substance-use disorders.

KEYWORDS: Behavioral addictions; buying-shopping disorder; cue-reactivity; gambling disorder; gaming disorder; inhibitory control; problematic pornography use

PMID: 31247240

DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.032

1. Introduction

The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model of specific Internet-use disorders was published more than two years ago (Brand et al., 2016b). One aim was to describe the psychological and neurobiological processes underlying the development and maintenance of an addictive use of specific Internet applications, such as those used when gaming, gambling, viewing pornography, buying-shopping, and social-networking. Since the publication of the I-PACE model, it has been cited relatively frequently by researchers globally not only for gaming disorder (e.g., Deleuze et al., 2017; Dieter et al., 2017; Dong et al., 2019; Kaess et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2018a; Lee et al., 2018b; Li et al., 2018; Paulus et al., 2018; Sariyska et al., 2017), but also for gambling disorder (e.g., Ioannidis et al., 2019b; Starcke et al., 2018), compulsive sexual behavior disorder including problematic pornography use (e.g., Carnes & Love, 2017; Strahler et al., 2018; Wéry et al., 2018), buying-shopping disorder (e.g., Lam & Lam, 2017; Vogel et al., 2018), excessive use of communication applications (e.g., Dempsey et al., 2019; Elhai et al., 2018; Kircaburun & Griffiths, 2018; Montag et al., 2018; Rothen et al., 2018), unspecified Internet-use disorder (e.g., Carbonell et al., 2018; Emelin et al., 2017; Ioannidis et al., 2019a; Lachmann et al., 2018; Vargas et al., 2019; Zhou et al., 2018b), and for other addictive behaviors including substance-use disorders (Zhou et al., 2018a). The eleventh edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11), as recently being released (World-Health-Organization, 2019), focuses on the disorder itself (e.g. gambling disorder) without referring to the medium of the disorder, for example gaming disorder instead of Internet-gaming disorder in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5) (APA, 2013). In the ICD-11, the environment of the behavior may subsequently be specified as predominantly offline or predominantly online for gambling and gaming disorders. Consequently, a model explaining the underlying processes of the problematic behavior should be valid for both online and offline environment and for a combination of offline and online behaviors as well. We continue proposing that the behavior itself is the core element to consider and the environment (online versus offline) may be typically secondary but may contribute importantly to the expression of specific addictive behaviors and common variance across these behaviors (Baggio et al., 2018). We suggest an updated version of the I-PACE model, which we hypothesize will be valid not only for specific Internet-use disorders, but also for other types of addictive behaviors. This updated I-PACE model concentrates on the individual psychological and neurobiological mechanisms of addictive behaviors. Media-specific aspects and other environmental factors related to the behavior that likely accelerate or decrease the development of an addictive behavior could then be defined and described for specific versions of the model. Fig. 1 summarizes the proposed differentiation between medium/ environmental aspects, individuals’ reactions, and behavioral and neurobiological factors involved in addictive behaviors.

Fig. 1

Fig. 1. Differentiating environmental aspects, individual reactions of the person, and consequences of repeated specific behaviors over time. The revised I-PACE model concentrates on the person’s reactions and consequences involved in developing addictive behaviors.

In addition, we aim at specifying more explicitly the process character of the model by differentiating two sub-models, one for the mechanisms involved in the early stages and one for the mechanisms involved in the later stages of the addiction process. We do not repeat the detailed discussion of all the components included in the I-PACE model (cf. Brand et al., 2016b). Instead, we mainly concentrate on the most recent articles, in particular meta-analyses and systematic reviews that inspired the I-PACE update.

2. The updated I-PACE model of addictive behaviors

The revision of the I-PACE model includes three main steps. First, we focus on predisposing variables, which have been considered as being involved in different types of addictive behaviors (gambling disorders, gaming disorder, and others) and differentiate them from more behavior-specific predisposing variables. Second, we define more precisely the inner circle of the addiction process in the I-PACE model with respect to recent findings. Third, we differentiate between early and later stages of the process in order to illustrate explicitly potentially different roles of moderating and mediating variables depending upon the stage of addiction. The revised I-PACE model of addictive behaviors is shown in Fig. 2. Fig. 2A shows the interactions between variables that are considered particularly important in early stages of addictive behaviors. Fig. 2B illustrates the interactions of variables in later stages of addictive processes.

Fig. 2

Fig. 2. The revised I-PACE model for addictive behaviors. Figure A shows early stages of the development of addictive behaviors. Figure B illustrates later stages of the process and factors contributing to the maintenance of addictive behaviors. Bolder arrows indicate stronger connections/accelerated mechanisms.

2.1. The P-component of the I-PACE model

The P-component represents person’s core characteristics likely involved in the addiction process as predisposing variables (see discussion in Brand et al., 2016b). The general predisposing variables (left side in the upper box of the model) may contribute importantly to all types of addictive behaviors (e.g., gambling disorder, gaming disorder, buying-shopping disorder, pornography-viewing disorder/hypersexual behavior). The list of these potentially predisposing variables is not exhaustive. It only summarizes variables for which relatively broad evidence exists, including from meta-analyses, even though the evidence may differ in strength for the different types of addictive behaviors. Data suggest a significant genetic contribution to gambling disorder (Lobo, 2016; Potenza, 2017, 2018; Xuan et al., 2017) and unspecified internet-use disorder (Hahn et al., 2017). Furthermore, negative early childhood experiences have been reported as vulnerability factors for gambling disorder (Roberts et al., 2017) and gaming disorder (Schneider et al., 2017), findings consistent with recent theoretical considerations of the role of attachment in addictive behaviors (Alvarez-Monjaras et al., 2018). Psychopathological correlates, in particular depression and social anxiety, have been reported repeatedly for gambling (Dowling et al., 2017), gaming (Männikkö et al., 2017), unspecified internet-use (Ho et al., 2014), and buying-shopping (Müller et al., 2019) disorders, and other behavioral addictions (Starcevic & Khazaal, 2017). Temperamental features, such as high impulsivity, have also been associated with gambling (Dowling et al., 2017), gaming (Gervasi et al., 2017; Kuss et al., 2018; Ryu et al., 2018), and unspecified internet-use (Kayiş et al., 2016) disorders, as has dysfunctional coping styles with gaming disorder (Schneider et al., 2018). In the I-PACE model, we use general terms (e.g., psychopathology, temperamental features including, for example, impulsivity) that may be specified further with respect to specific addictive behaviors. The behavior-specific predisposing variables (right side of the upper box in the model, Fig. 2A and B) are considered characteristic for the different specific addictive behaviors. For example, individuals with higher novelty-seeking may be more likely to develop gambling disorder (Del Pino-Gutiérrez et al., 2017). Individuals with higher aggressiveness and narcissistic personality traits may be more prone to develop gaming disorder (Gervasi et al., 2017). Individuals with high trait sexual motivation may be more likely to develop hypersexual behavior or pornography-use disorder (Stark et al., 2017), and individuals with high materialistic values may be particularly prone to developing buying-shopping disorder (Claes et al., 2016; Müller et al., 2014).

2.2. The inner circle: The affect (A-), cognition (C-), and execution (E-) components of the I-PACE model

One main idea of the inner circle of the I-PACE model is that the development of a problematic and addictive behavior occurs only in interactions between individuals’ predisposing variables and certain aspects that specific situations deliver. The interactions result in experiences of gratification and compensation that are associated with specific behaviors. In the early stages (Fig. 2A), individuals may perceive external (e.g., confrontation with behavior-related stimuli) or internal triggers (e.g., negative or very positive moods) in specific situations. The perceptions may result in affective and cognitive responses, such as increased attention to these stimuli and urges to behave in specific ways; e.g. urges to play online games or view pornography (Starcke et al., 2018).

The affective and cognitive responses lead to decisions to behave in specific ways. The decision to engage in a specific behavior may be guided by two interactive systems: an impulsive/ reactive system, which is mainly based on associative learning (classical and operant conditioning), and a reflective/ deliberative system, which is mainly linked to reasoning and executive functions (Kahneman, 2003; Schiebener & Brand, 2015; Strack & Deutsch, 2004). In individuals with addictions, behavior is considered to depend increasingly on impulsive/ reactive neural systems, including limbic structures (Noël et al., 2006). Prefrontal-cortex-related inhibitory control over urges and desires may decrease during the addiction process (Bechara, 2005; Volkow & Morales, 2015). Combining these theoretical perspectives, we propose that relationships between affective and cognitive responses to external or internal triggers and decisions to engage in specific behaviors are moderated by the level of general inhibitory control (in contrast to mood-specific or stimulus-specific inhibitory control) and self-regulation/ self-directedness (Hahn et al., 2017), at least in the early stages of addictive behaviors. The meta-analysis by Meng, Deng, Wang, Guo, and Li (2015) illustrates that prefrontal dysfunctions are associated with gaming disorder, indicating the potential conflict between reward-anticipation and self-regulation systems, including conflicts involved in the delaying of gratification (Volkow & Baler, 2015). Regarding a general inhibitory control, Yao et al. (2017) report functional and structural brain alterations in gaming disorder that are linked to reductions in executive functioning. Specific behaviors (e.g. playing an online game, gambling in a casino, buying items) may lead to feelings of gratification or relief from negative moods (Laier & Brand, 2017). These experiences subsequently change the subjective reward expectancies that are associated with the specific behaviors. They may also modify the individual coping style. For example, if individuals learn that playing online games is effective in generating good feelings or avoiding negative emotional states, they may generalize the expectancy that playing online games is helpful for dealing with emotions in daily life (Kuss et al., 2018; Laier et al., 2018). The changes of expectancies and coping styles may increase the likelihood of responding with urge feelings or feelings of desire in subsequent situations when being confronted with external or internal triggers. This interaction of craving experiences and expectancies has been demonstrated by individuals with higher symptom severity of an addictive use of internet-communication services (Wegmann et al., 2018b). Over time, these associations between affective and cognitive responses, decisions to behave in specific ways, experiences of gratification and compensation and behavior-specific expectancies may become stronger. Consequently, control over the behaviors by general inhibitory mechanisms may become more difficult, and decisions to behave in specific ways may be guided more dominantly by impulsive/ reactive responses to triggers. Mechanisms proposed to be involved in later stages of addictive behaviors are summarized in Fig. 2B.

In later stages of the addiction process, although the shift may be gradual, the aforementioned associations may become increasingly strong, resulting in habitual behaviors that may feel automatic in certain situations. Cue-reactivity and craving may evolve from affective and cognitive responses over time as a consequence of conditioning processes (Starcke et al., 2018). Previous research highlights the important role of sensitivities towards addiction-related stimuli and activations in neural reward systems involving the ventral and dorsal striatum and other limbic structures in addictive behaviors (Fauth-Bühler & Mann, 2017; Fauth-Bühler et al., 2017; Luijten et al., 2017; Palaus et al., 2017). Subjective expectancies may evolve into affective and cognitive biases, which may include biased or seemingly automatic attention to the respective behavior-related stimuli and triggers (Jeromin et al., 2016). We propose that compensatory effects become stronger over gratifying effects in later stages of the addiction process (cf. Brand et al., 2016b). In addition to the moderating effects of general inhibitory control on relationships between cue-reactivity/craving and the habitual behaviors, we propose that stimuli-specific inhibitory control may act as a mediator in the later stages of addictive processes (Everitt & Robbins, 2016). Several researchers have emphasized impairments in inhibitory control and executive functions in gambling disorder (Ioannidis et al., 2019b; van Timmeren et al., 2018), gaming disorder (Argyriou et al., 2017; Kuss et al., 2018; Yao et al., 2017), and unspecified internet-use disorders (Ioannidis et al., 2019a). We propose, however, that although the general inhibitory control may also decrease during addictive processes, the development of diminished specific stimulus-related inhibitory control is crucially involved in habitual behaviors in later stages of addictive behaviors. We propose that if cue-reactivity and craving have been developed as responses to external or internal triggers, this may lead to reductions in controlling desire when being confronted with addictive stimuli, which may then increase the likelihood of behaving habitually (Piazza & Deroche-Gamonet, 2013).

3. Neurobiological mechanisms

3.1. Neuroscientific theories of addiction integrated in the inner circle of the I-PACE model

Several neuroscientific theories and models explaining addictive behaviors have been integrated in the theoretical framework of the inner circle of the I-PACE model (Brand et al., 2016b). Direct links can be seen to the Impaired Response Inhibition and Salience Attribution (I-RISA) model (Goldstein & Volkow, 2011), Incentive-Sensitization (Robinson & Berridge, 2008), Reward Deficiency Syndrome (Blum et al., 1996) models and theories, and in dual-process approaches of addiction (Bechara, 2005; Everitt & Robbins, 2005, 2016) and ideas of imbalance between goal-directed behaviors and habits (Robbins et al., 2019). We also refer to aspects of more specific theoretical models integrating neuroscientific considerations of gambling disorder (Blaszczynski & Nower, 2002; Goudriaan et al., 2004) and gaming disorder (Dong & Potenza, 2014; Wei et al., 2017). Combining these theories, we consider the progression of an imbalance between increasing incentive-oriented urges and desires on the one hand and decreasing situation-specific inhibitory control over these urges and desires on the other hand as important for the development and maintenance of addictive behaviors. Increasing incentive sensitization, as a result of conditioning processes (Berridge et al., 2009), may associate with attentional bias and cue-reactivity in later stages of addiction processes. Individuals with reward deficiencies may be particularly prone to developing incentive sensitization (Blum et al., 2012). Incentive salience may promote cue-reactivity and craving, which may contribute to engagement in addictive behaviors.

Reductions in executive functions have considered both as vulnerability factors and consequences of addictive behaviors including substance-use disorders (Volkow et al., 2012). In behavioral addictions, such as gambling and gaming disorders, one may argue that executive reductions constitute vulnerability factors and do not develop as a consequence of the addictive behavior, because no direct substance-related neurotoxic effects on the brain are involved. Consistent with this notion, we propose that a diminished level of general inhibitory control is a vulnerability factor for the addictive behavior and acts as a moderating variable of the relationship between affective responses to certain triggering stimuli (e.g., stress or negative moods) and decisions to engage in specific behaviors (see Fig. 2A). In addition, however, we argue that beyond this moderating effect of executive functions as a vulnerability factor of addictions, situation-specific inhibitory control (when being confronted with addiction-related stimuli) may decrease over time as a consequence of addictive behaviors, although – in contrast to substance-use disorders – no direct neurotoxic effects on the brain are involved in behavioral addictions. Reductions in stimuli-specific inhibitory control may develop based on cue-reactivity and craving and be accompanied by functional brain changes in addiction-related circuits (Ersche et al., 2012; Koob & Volkow, 2010; Volkow & Morales, 2015; Volkow et al., 2012). Thus, in later stages of addictive behaviors (Fig. 2B), stimuli-specific inhibitory control processes may be influenced by craving and urges related to encountering addiction-related stimuli, which may then make it more likely that an individual will behave habitually or seemingly automatically (Everitt & Robbins, 2005, 2013, 2016).

3.2. Neural correlates of the main processes within the inner circle of the I-PACE model

The aforementioned proposed imbalance between limbic/ reward-oriented brain circuits and prefrontal control in behavioral addictions has been reviewed relatively extensively for gambling disorder (Clark et al., 2013; Goudriaan et al., 2014; Potenza, 2013; van Holst et al., 2010) and gaming disorder (Kuss et al., 2018; Weinstein, 2017; Weinstein et al., 2017), including in meta-analyses (Meng et al., 2015). Although less extensively, there also exist neuroimaging studies of compulsive sexual behaviors, including problematic pornography use (e.g., Brand et al., 2016a; Gola et al., 2017; Klucken et al., 2016; Schmidt et al., 2017; Voon et al., 2014), which have been examined in recent reviews (Kraus et al., 2016; Stark et al., 2018). Scientific studies of neural correlates of buying-shopping disorder are relatively scarce. There are, however, some studies from consumer psychology perspectives (e.g., Raab et al., 2011) and studies using electrophysiological measures to investigate neurobiological mechanisms of buying-shopping disorder (Trotzke et al., 2014) which have been reviewed recently (Kyrios et al., 2018; Trotzke et al., 2017). Although not yet recognized as a clinical condition, there are also recent publications on structural and functional neuroimaging findings of poorly controlled and problematic use of social networks sites and other internet-communication applications (e.g., Dieter et al., 2017; He et al., 2017; Lemenager et al., 2016; Montag et al., 2017; Montag et al., 2018; Turel & Qahri-Saremi, 2016), which have been reviewed by Wegmann et al. (2018a).

There is significant variability across neuroimaging studies of addictive behaviors regarding the types of behavioral addictions, the techniques used (e.g., structural/functional magnetic resonance imaging [s/fMRI], positron emission tomography [PET]), the psychological constructs or processes of interest, the experimental tasks used to measure specific functions, the samples included (convenient samples with individuals showing different degrees of symptoms versus clinically diagnosed individuals or treatment-seeking patients), and the diagnostic procedures used. Nevertheless, when drawing conclusions from the studies, meta-analyses and reviews (see, for example, citations above), there is first evidence for hyperactive involvement of limbic structures, including the amygdala and the ventral striatum, and hypoactive prefrontal-striatal circuits involved in cognitive control over the behaviors. There are, however, some caveats, for example hypoactive engagement of reward circuitry during the anticipatory phase of monetary processing (Balodis & Potenza, 2015), with some researchers proposing differences relating to the processing of addictive cues (hyperactive reward responding) and non-addictive rewarding cues (relatively hypoactive reward responding) (Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2013). The insula may be a mediator between the two systems (limbic and prefrontal-striatal), representing the somatic states linked to craving and the desire to behave specifically (see discussion in Namkung et al., 2017; Wei et al., 2017). The main structures that have been identified as potential brain correlates of addictive behaviors are summarized in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Fig. 3. Brain circuits potentially underlying addictive behaviors. Orange arrows represent main circuitry proposed to be involved in early stages of addictive processes. Blue arrows indicate the additional involvement of the dorsal striatum and related structures in later stages of addiction processes, when behaviors become more habitual. ACC = anterior cingulate cortex, AM = amygdala, DLPFC = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, DS = dorsal striatum, GP = globus pallidus, Hipp = hippocampus, Ins = insula, Motor = motor cortex and associated regions for executing behaviors, OFC = orbitofrontal cortex, Rap = serotonergic raphé nuclei, SN = substantia nigra, Thal = thalamus, VMPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex, VS = ventral striatum, VTA = dopaminergic ventral tegmental area.

In the recent meta-analysis of cue-reactivity-related brain activity in fMRI-studies with samples of patients with behavioral addictions compared to control subjects (Starcke et al., 2018), the dorsal striatum (caudate nucleus) was more active in individuals with addictions compared to those without and in individuals with addictions when contrasting the addiction-related condition with the neutral condition in the cue-reactivity tasks. The findings may reflect shifts from involvement of the ventral striatum in early stages of behavioral addictions when being confronted with addiction-related stimuli to involvement of the dorsal striatum in later stages of the disorder, when the behavior becomes more habitual (Everitt & Robbins, 2013, 2016; Zhou et al., 2019). The brain structures and circuits that likely underlie addictive behaviors and shifts from early to later stages of addiction processes are illustrated schematically in Fig. 3.

Widespread connections among fronto-striatal structures have been examined using resting-state fMRI investigations with healthy subjects and have been shown to be involved in behavioral flexibility (Morris et al., 2016). These circuits are also broadly consistent with functional networks involved in emotion regulation (Öner, 2018). Changes in connectivity between specific structures involved in fronto-striatal circuits (e.g., connectivity between amygdala and medial PFC) appear important in explaining emotion dysregulation in substance-use disorders (Koob, 2015; Wilcox et al., 2016). The connectivity of networks involved in cognitive control (fronto-parietal circuits and medial frontal areas) and in reward processing (including subcortical and limbic structures) has also been shown to predict abstinence in cocaine-use disorder after treatment (Yip et al., 2019). A stronger separation of the two networks involved in executive control and in reward sensitivity has been proposed to underlie behavioral flexibility and decreased compulsivity, which may explain better therapeutic outcomes (Yip et al., 2019).

In summary, we propose that an imbalance in circuits underlying behavioral flexibility and emotion/urge regulation relate to main aspects of addictive behaviors. The pathways include dopaminergic projections from the ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra to prefrontal areas, the ventral striatum, and anterior cingulate gyrus as well as serotonergic projections from raphé nuclei to prefrontal areas (mainly orbitofrontal regions) (Everitt & Robbins, 2005; Volkow et al., 2012; Volkow et al., 2013). The interconnections between striatal structures, thalamus, and prefrontal areas depend largely upon glutamate and gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) (Naaijen et al., 2015), and the neurochemical systems involved in fronto-striatal loops act in concerted and cross-regulatory fashions (Gleich et al., 2015). Neurochemical correlates of addictions have been discussed intensively elsewhere, and many studies emphasize the important role of dopamine in substance-use disorders (Herman & Roberto, 2015; Pascoli et al., 2018; Volkow et al., 2016). The findings concerning dopamine in behavioral addictions are, however, less robust (Potenza, 2018).

Although a meaningful number of studies on neural correlates of behavioral addictions have been published in recent years, there remain limitations that should be mentioned. First, most studies have been published on gambling disorder and gaming disorder (see comments above). Less evidence exists for other behavioral addictions including compulsive sexual behaviors including problematic pornography use, buying-shopping disorder, and other potential phenomena that are not yet recognized as clinical conditions, such as problematic use of social-network sites. In particular, studies that systematically investigate neural correlates of specific psychological functions (e.g., craving, inhibitory control) across specific types of behavioral addictions are missing. Studies investigating stages of addiction processes or symptom severity as predictors or moderating variables of neural activity and potential structural brain abnormalities are important to better understand mechanisms underlying the progression of addictive behaviors. Consistent with this, longitudinal studies on brain correlates of addictive behaviors that test specific hypotheses are missing. Investigating the potential shift from activity of the ventral to the dorsal striatum as response to addiction-related cues across types of behavioral addictions and across different stages of addiction, using both cross-sectional and longitudinal designs, would help to understand better the nature of addictive behaviors. Such studies are necessary to disentangle potential shifts from craving to compulsion and from expecting gratification to expecting relief from negative states when encountering addiction-related stimuli in different stages of behavioral addictions, which in turn should help in optimizing treatments. Studies comparing different types of addictive behaviors and different stages of addiction processes including prospective longitudinal studies could also investigate hypothesized involvement of reductions in inhibitory control as a vulnerability factor and/ or as a consequence of the addictive behaviors, and one that may mediate links between affective responses and habitual/ compulsive behaviors (see discussion in Everitt & Robbins, 2016).

4. Conclusion and future directions

The updated I-PACE model is a theoretical approach for describing the process of addictive behaviors by combining psychological and neuroscientific theories of substance-use disorders and behavioral addictions. We consider disorders due to addictive behaviors being the consequence of interactions between core characteristics of a person and several moderating and mediating variables, which may be dynamic and develop over time as a consequence of engagement in specific behaviors. We propose that the I-PACE model of addictive behaviors may be useful for psychological and neuroscientific research because it permits formation and testing of clear hypotheses regarding interaction effects of specific variables in explaining variance in symptom severity of behavioral addictions. The model may also inspire clinical practice (cf. King et al., 2017; Potenza, 2017) by defining and investigating possible mediating variables that may represent important targets for treatment (e.g., expectancies, affective and cognitive responses to triggers). The updated I-PACE model also offers the possibility of deriving hypotheses on the stages of addictive processes (both during progression and recovery), for example by arguing that reductions of specific inhibitory-control accelerate in later stages in the progression of addiction processes. It is important to note, however, that we consider theoretical models as being dynamic. The validity of specific hypotheses, combined in a theoretical framework, should be evaluated empirically, and theoretical models should be updated by considering recent scientific findings from different perspectives.

What is important to keep in mind is that the theoretical model proposed is based on different degrees of scientific evidence with respect to addictive behaviors. As mentioned in the previous sections, the involvement of specific psychological mechanisms and neurobiological processes is relatively well studied in gambling disorder and gaming disorder and less intensively investigated in other types of behaviors that potentially become addictive, for example pornography use, buying-shopping and social-networking. Furthermore, for some aspects and mechanisms proposed in the updated I-PACE model, different levels of evidence exist. For executive functions and inhibitory control, a relatively large number of studies has used experimental paradigms and has examined particular aspects of executive functioning in different types of addictive behaviors. On the other hand, for cue-reactivity and craving in specific addictive behaviors, some studies applied a correlational design, which makes interpretations of causality and the time of development of cue-reactivity and craving in the addiction process difficult to define (Zilberman et al., 2019). Considering these limitations it is important to emphasize that the model proposed is a theoretical model that summarizes the current state-of-the-art of behavioral addiction research and aims at inspiring theory-based future studies.

Another issue that is important to consider is that personality and temperament features are rather vague predictors for specific addictive behaviors, given that these variables are involved in many psychopathologies and often explain only a mild-to-moderate proportion of symptoms across different disorders (Zilberman et al., 2018).

We would also like to comment on the current debate with respect to classifying buying-shopping disorder and pornography-use disorder as impulse-control disorders or as behavioral addictions. The ICD-11 includes problematic pornography use as one facet of compulsive sexual behavior disorder within the category of impulse-control disorders. Buying-shopping disorder is listed as an example for other specified impulse-control disorders in the ICD-11 coding tool (World-Health-Organization, 2019). Many researchers, however, argue that both types of disorders would be better classified as addictive behaviors (Potenza et al., 2018).

One challenge for future research and theory building involves identifying and disentangling potential commonalities and differences between disorders due to addictive behaviors and other mental disorders, such as obsessive-compulsive-related and impulse-control disorders, which may relate to behavioral addictions on psychological and neurobiological levels (Chamberlain et al., 2016; Fineberg et al., 2013; Fineberg et al., 2018; Robbins et al., 2019). For example, inhibitory control and reward processing have also been proposed to be important in obsessive-compulsive-related and impulse-control disorders, such as discussed in skin-picking disorder and trichotillomania, which have also been related to functioning of fronto-striatal brain circuits (Chamberlain et al., 2008). Dysfunctions of fronto-striatal loops may, however, be involved in multiple other mental health disorders as well (Mitelman, 2019). Nonetheless, the fact that fronto-striatal loops are involved in different mental disorders does not necessarily mean that the psychological processes related to the clinical phenotypes of disorders are the same. First, fronto-striatal loops are differently defined and analyzed across studies. Future studies should examine more closely specific contributions of structures currently broadly defined within fronto-striatal loops to certain specific psychological processes underlying specific problematic behaviors. Second, the general involvement of inhibitory control and reward processing does not mean that the psychological processes are comparable across disorders, although there may be some overlap of impulsivity/compulsivity and addictive behaviors (e.g., Chamberlain et al., 2018). It is important to define more precisely the factors underlying the temporal progression of motivations for people to engage excessively in specific behaviors. For example, in addictive behaviors, it may be that core motivations to engage in gaming or gambling, at least at early stages, involve reward anticipation. In later stages, avoidance of negative feelings is likely additionally involved. In obsessive-compulsive-related disorders, it may be that the core drive at early stages involves avoidance of negative feelings or anxiety. Later, the behavior itself may potentially be experienced as rewarding because it may help relieve stress. In other words, the general involvement of specific neurocognitive functions may not entirely explain a disorder. The same idea potentially applies to neural mechanisms. It may be the case that in disorders due to addictive behaviors, the ventral striatum contributes importantly at early stages of the disorder, with respect to cue-reactivity and craving. In later stages, the dorsal striatum may become more involved and relate to habitual and compulsive aspects of addictive disorders. In contrast, the dorsal striatum is likely involved in obsessive-compulsive-related disorders and impulse-control disorders, such as trichotillomania, from early stages (Isobe et al., 2018; van den Heuvel et al., 2016).

In future studies, it seems important to investigate processes and interactions of different neurocognitive functions in different types of addictive behaviors in order to achieve a better understanding of the underlying natures of behavioral phenomena. The I-PACE model may be used for defining and clarifying specific hypotheses in researching these phenomena. It is important to examine hypothesized processes in addictive behaviors and compare them with other mental disorders, such as obsessive-compulsive-related and impulse-control disorders to understand whether the underlying processes involved are different or similar. In this process, data generated should help clarify the extent to which different terms may be currently employed for describing similar mechanisms across disorders. In this manner, the updated version of the I-PACE model provides a theoretical framework that should help address major questions relating to addictive, obsessive-compulsive, impulse-control and other disorders, including those relating to internet use, which may become increasingly relevant over time given changes in the digital technology environment.

Declaration of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Dr. Brand has received (to University of Duisburg-Essen) grants from the German Research Foundation (DFG), the German Federal Ministry for Research and Education, the German Federal Ministry for Health, and the European Union. Dr. Brand has performed grant reviews for several agencies; has edited journal sections and articles; has given academic lectures in clinical or scientific venues; and has generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts. Dr. Potenza receives support from NIH (R01 DA039136, R01 DA042911, R01 DA026437, R03 DA045289, R21 DA042911, and P50 DA09241), the Connecticut Department of Mental Health and Addiction Services, the Connecticut Council on Problem Gambling and the National Center for Responsible Gaming. Dr. Potenza has consulted for and advised Rivermend Health, Opiant/Lakelight Therapeutics and Jazz Pharmaceuticals; received research support (to Yale) from the Mohegan Sun Casino and the National Center for Responsible Gaming; consulted for or advised legal and gambling entities on issues related to impulse control and addictive behaviors; provided clinical care related to impulse control and addictive behaviors; performed grant reviews; edited journals/journal sections; given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events and other clinical/scientific venues; and generated books or chapters for publishers of mental health texts.

Acknowledgements

We thankfully acknowledge the intellectual contributions of Dr. Kimberly S. Young to the earlier version of the I-PACE model, which were inspiring for the updated model. Dr. Young has passed away in February 2019. In memory of Dr. Kimberly S. Young, we dedicate this article to her.

References

 

Baggio et al., 2018

S. Baggio, V. Starcevic, J. Studer, O. Simon, S.M. Gainsbury, G. Gmel, J. BillieuxTechnology-mediated addictive behaviors constitute a spectrum of related yet distinct conditions: A network perspective
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 32 (2018), pp. 564-572, 10.1037/adb0000379

Balodis and Potenza, 2015

I.M. Balodis, M.N. PotenzaAnticipatory reward processing in addicted populations: A focus on the monetary incentive delay task
Biological Psychiatry, 77 (2015), pp. 434-444, 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.020

Bechara, 2005

A. BecharaDecision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: A neurocognitive perspective
Nature Neuroscience, 8 (2005), pp. 1458-1463, 10.1038/nn1584

Berridge et al., 2009

K.C. Berridge, T.E. Robinson, J.W. AldridgeDissecting components of reward: ‘Liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning
Current Opinions in Pharmacology, 9 (2009), pp. 65-73, 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014

Blaszczynski and Nower, 2002

A. Blaszczynski, L. NowerApathways model of problem and pathological gambling
Addiction, 97 (2002), pp. 487-499

Blum et al., 2012

K. Blum, E. Gardner, M. Oscar-Berman, M. Gold“Liking” and “wanting” linked to Reward Deficiency Syndrome (RDS): Hypothesizing differential responsivity in brain reward circuitry
Current Pharmaceutical Design, 18 (2012), pp. 113-138, 10.2174/138161212798919110

Blum et al., 1996

K. Blum, P.J. Sheridan, R.C. Wood, E.R. Braverman, T.J. Chen, J.G. Cull, D.E. ComingsThe D2 dopamine receptor gene as a determinant of reward deficiency syndrome
Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 89 (1996), pp. 396-400

Brand et al., 2016a

M. Brand, J. Snagowski, C. Laier, S. MaderwaldVentral striatum activity when watching preferred pornographic pictures is correlated with symptoms of Internet pornography addiction
Neuroimage, 129 (2016), pp. 224-232, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.033

Brand et al., 2016b

M. Brand, K.S. Young, C. Laier, K. Wölfling, M.N. PotenzaIntegrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: An Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 71 (2016), pp. 252-266, 10.16/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033

Carbonell et al., 2018

X. Carbonell, A. Chamarro, U. Oberst, B. Rodrigo, M. PradesProblematic use of the internet and smartphones in university students: 2006–2017
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15 (2018), p. E475, 10.3390/ijerph15030475

Carnes and Love, 2017

S. Carnes, T. LoveSeparating models obscures the scientific underpinnings of sex addiction as a disorder
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46 (2017), pp. 2253-2256, 10.1007/s10508-017-1072-8

Chamberlain et al., 2018

S.R. Chamberlain, K. Ioannidis, J.E. GrantThe impact of comorbid impulsive/compulsive disorders in problematic Internet use
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7 (2018), pp. 269-275, 10.1556/2006.7.2018.30

Chamberlain et al., 2016

S.R. Chamberlain, C. Lochner, D.J. Stein, A.E. Goudriaan, R.J. van Holst, J. Zohar, J.E. GrantBehavioural addiction – A rising tide?
European Neuropsychopharmacology, 26 (2016), pp. 841-855, 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.08.013

Chamberlain et al., 2008

S.R. Chamberlain, L. Menzies, A. Hampshire, J. Suckling, N.A. Fineberg, N. del Campo, et al.Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives
Science, 321 (2008), pp. 421-422, 10.1126/science.1154433

Claes et al., 2016

L. Claes, A. Müller, K. LuyckxCompulsive buying and hoarding as identity substitutes: The role of materialistic value endorsement and depression
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 68 (2016), pp. 65-71, 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.04.005

Clark et al., 2013

L. Clark, B. Averbeck, D. Payer, G. Sescousse, C.A. Winstanley, G. XuePathological choice: The neuroscience of gambling and gambling addiction
Journal of Neuroscience, 33 (2013), pp. 17617-17623, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3231-13.2013

Del Pino-Gutiérrez et al., 2017

A. Del Pino-Gutiérrez, S. Jiménez-Murcia, F. Fernández-Aranda, Z. Agüera, R. Granero, A. Hakansson, et al.The relevance of personality traits in impulsivity-related disorders: From substance use disorders and gambling disorder to bulimia nervosa
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6 (2017), pp. 396-405, 10.1556/2006.6.2017.051

Deleuze et al., 2017

J. Deleuze, F. Nuyens, L. Rochat, S. Rothen, P. Maurage, J. BillieuxEstablished risk factors for addiction fail to discriminate between healthy gamers and gamers endorsing DSM-5 Internet gaming disorder
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6 (2017), pp. 516-524, 10.1556/2006.6.2017.074

Dempsey et al., 2019

A.E. Dempsey, K.D. O’Brien, M.F. Tiamiyu, J.D. ElhaiFear of missing out (FoMO) and rumination mediate relations between social anxiety and problematic Facebook use
Addictive Behaviors Reports, 9 (2019), Article 100150, 10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100150

Dieter et al., 2017

J. Dieter, S. Hoffmann, D. Mier, I. Reinhard, M. Beutel, S. Vollstädt-Klein, et al.The role of emotional inhibitory control in specific internet addiction – an fMRI study
Behavioural Brain Research, 324 (2017), pp. 1-14, 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.01.046

Dong and Potenza, 2014

G. Dong, M.N. PotenzaA cognitive-behavioral model of Internet gaming disorder: Theoretical underpinnings and clinical implications
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 58 (2014), pp. 7-11, 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.07.005

Dong et al., 2019

G. Dong, Z. Wang, Y. Wang, X. Du, M.N. PotenzaGender-related functional connectivity and craving during gaming and immediate abstinence during a mandatory break: Implications for development and progression of internet gaming disorder
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 88 (2019), pp. 1-10, 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.04.009

Dowling et al., 2017

N.A. Dowling, S.S. Merkouris, C.J. Greenwood, E. Oldenhof, J.W. Toumbourou, G.J. YoussefEarly risk and protective factors for problem gambling: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies
Clinical Psychology Review, 51 (2017), pp. 109-124, 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.008

Elhai et al., 2018

J.D. Elhai, M. Tiamiyu, J. WeeksDepression and social anxiety in relation to problematic smartphone use: The prominent role of rumination
Internet Research, 28 (2018), pp. 315-332, 10.1108/IntR-01-2017-0019

Emelin et al., 2017

V.A. Emelin, E.I. Rasskazova, A.S. TkhostovTechnology-related transformations of imaginary body boundaries: Psychopathology of the everyday excessive Internet and mobile phone use
Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 10 (2017), pp. 177-189, 10.11621/pir.2017.0312

Ersche et al., 2012

K.D. Ersche, P.S. Jones, G.B. Williams, A.J. Turton, T.W. Robbins, E.T. BullmoreAbnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction
Science, 335 (2012), pp. 601-604, 10.1126/science.1214463

Everitt and Robbins, 2005

B.J. Everitt, T.W. RobbinsNeural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion
Nature Neuroscience, 8 (2005), pp. 1481-1489, 10.1038/nn1579

Everitt and Robbins, 2013

B.J. Everitt, T.W. RobbinsFrom the ventral to the dorsal striatum: Devolving views of their roles in drug addiction
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37 (2013), pp. 1946-1954, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.010

Everitt and Robbins, 2016

B.J. Everitt, T.W. RobbinsDrug addiction: Updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on
Annual Review of Psychology, 67 (2016), pp. 23-50, 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457

Fauth-Bühler and Mann, 2017

M. Fauth-Bühler, K. MannNeurobiological correlates of internet gaming disorder: Similarities to pathological gambling
Addictive Behaviors, 64 (2017), pp. 349-356, 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.004

Fauth-Bühler et al., 2017

M. Fauth-Bühler, K. Mann, M.N. PotenzaPathological gambling: A review of the neurobiological evidence relevant for its classification as an addictive disorder
Addiction Biology, 22 (2017), pp. 885-897, 10.1111/adb.12378

Fineberg et al., 2013

N.A. Fineberg, D.S. Baldwin, J.M. Menchon, D. Denys, E. Grünblatt, S. Pallanti, et al.Manifesto for a European research network into obsessive-compulsive and related disorders
European Neuropsychopharmacology, 23 (2013), pp. 561-568, 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.06.006

Fineberg et al., 2018

N.A. Fineberg, Z. Demetrovics, D.J. Stein, K. Ioannidis, M.N. Potenza, E. Grünblatt, et al.Manifesto for a European research network into Problematic Usage of the Internet
European Neuropsychopharmacology, 11 (2018), pp. 1232-1246, 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.08.004

Gervasi et al., 2017

A.M. Gervasi, L. La Marca, A. Costanzo, U. Pace, F. Guglielmucci, A. SchimmentiPersonality and Internet gaming disorder: A systematic review of recent literature
Current Addiction Reports, 4 (2017), pp. 293-307, 10.1007/s40429-017-0159-6

Gleich et al., 2015

T. Gleich, L. Deserno, R.C. Lorenz, R. Boehme, A. Pankow, R. Buchert, et al.Prefrontal and striatal glutamate differently relate to striatal dopamine: Potential regulatory mechanisms of striatal presynaptic dopamine function?
Journal of Neuroscience, 35 (2015), pp. 9615-9621, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0329-15.2015

Gola et al., 2017

M. Gola, M. Wordecha, G. Sescousse, M. Lew-Starowicz, B. Kossowski, M. Wypych, et al.Can pornography be addictive? An fMRI study of men seeking treatment for problematic pornography use
Neuropsychopharmacology, 42 (2017), pp. 2021-2031, 10.1038/npp.2017.78

Goldstein and Volkow, 2011

R.Z. Goldstein, N.D. VolkowDysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: Neuroimaging findings and clinical implications
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12 (2011), pp. 652-669, 10.1038/nrn3119

Goudriaan et al., 2004

A.E. Goudriaan, J. Oosterlaan, E. Beurs, W. van den BrinkPathological gambling: A comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28 (2004), pp. 123-141, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.001

Goudriaan et al., 2014

A.E. Goudriaan, M. Yücel, R.J. van HolstGetting a grip on problem gambling: What can neuroscience tell us?
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8 (2014), p. 141, 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00141

Hahn et al., 2017

E. Hahn, M. Reuter, F.M. Spinath, C. MontagInternet addiction and its facets: The role of genetics and the relation to self-directedness
Addictive Behaviors, 65 (2017), pp. 137-146, 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.10.018

He et al., 2017

Q. He, O. Turel, A. BecharaBrain anatomy alterations associated with Social Networking Site (SNS) addiction
Scientific Reports, 23 (7) (2017), p. 45064, 10.1038/srep45064

Herman and Roberto, 2015

M.A. Herman, M. RobertoThe addicted brain: Understanding the neurophysiological mechanisms of addictive disorders
Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 9 (2015), p. 18, 10.3389/fnint.2015.00018

Ho et al., 2014

R.C. Ho, M.W.B. Zhang, T.Y. Tsang, A.H. Toh, F. Pan, Y. Lu, K.-K. MakThe association between internet addiction and psychiatric co-morbidity: A meta-analysis
BMC Psychiatry, 14 (2014), p. 183, 10.1186/1471-244X-14-183

Ioannidis et al., 2019a

K. Ioannidis, R. Hook, A.E. Goudriaan, S. Vlies, N.A. Fineberg, J.E. Grant, S.R. ChamberlainCognitive deficits in problematic Internet use: A meta-analysis of 40 studies
The British Journal of Psychiatry (2019), 10.1192/bjp.2019.3
[EPub ahead of print]

Ioannidis et al., 2019b

K. Ioannidis, R. Hook, K. Wickham, J.E. Grant, S.R. ChamberlainImpulsivity in gambling disorder and problem gambling: A meta-analysis
Neuropsychopharmacology (2019), 10.1038/s41386-019-0393-9
[EPub ahead of print]

Isobe et al., 2018

M. Isobe, S.A. Redden, N.J. Keuthen, D.J. Stein, C. Lochner, J.E. Grant, S.R. ChamberlainStriatal abnormalities in trichotillomania: A multi-site MRI analysis
Neuroimmage: Clinical, 17 (2018), pp. 893-898, 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.12.031

Jeromin et al., 2016

F. Jeromin, N. Nyenhuis, A. BarkeAttentional bias in excessive Internet gamers: Experimental investigations using an addiction Stroop and a visual probe
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5 (2016), pp. 32-40

Kaess et al., 2017

M. Kaess, P. Parzer, L. Mehl, L. Weil, E. Strittmatter, F. Resch, J. KoenigStress vulnerability in male youth with Internet Gaming Disorder
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77 (2017), pp. 244-251, 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.01.008

Kahneman, 2003

D. KahnemanA perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality
American Psychologist, 58 (2003), pp. 697-720, 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697

Kayiş et al., 2016

A.R. Kayiş, S.A. Satici, M.F. Yilmaz, D. Şimşek, E. Ceyhan, F. BakioğluBig five-personality trait and internet addiction: A meta-analytic review
Computers in Human Behavior, 63 (2016), pp. 35-40, 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.012

King et al., 2017

D.L. King, P.H. Delfabbro, A.M.S. Wu, Y.Y. Doh, D.J. Kuss, S. Pallesen, et al.Treatment of Internet gaming disorder: An international systematic review and CONSORT evaluation
Clinical Psychology Review, 54 (2017), pp. 123-133, 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.04.002

Kircaburun and Griffiths, 2018

K. Kircaburun, M.D. GriffithsInstagram addiction and the Big Five of personality: The mediating role of self-liking
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7 (2018), pp. 158-170, 10.1556/2006.7.2018.15

Klucken et al., 2016

T. Klucken, S. Wehrum-Osinsky, J. Schweckendiek, O. Kruse, R. StarkAltered appetitive conditioning and neural connectivity in subjects with compulsive sexual behavior
Journal of Sexual Medicine, 13 (2016), pp. 627-636, 10.1016/j.jsxm.2016.01.013

Koob, 2015

G.F. KoobThe dark side of emotion: The addiction perspective
European Journal of Pharmacology, 753 (2015), pp. 73-87, 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.11.044

Koob and Volkow, 2010

G.F. Koob, N.D. VolkowNeurocircuitry of addiction
Neuropsychopharmacology, 35 (2010), pp. 217-238

Kraus et al., 2016

S.W. Kraus, V. Voon, M.N. PotenzaShould compulsive sexual behavior be considered an addiction?
Addiction, 111 (2016), pp. 2097-2106, 10.1111/add.13297

Kuss et al., 2018

D.J. Kuss, H.M. Pontes, M.D. GriffithsNeurobiological correlates in Internet gaming disorder: A systematic literature review
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9 (2018), p. 166, 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00166

Kyrios et al., 2018

M. Kyrios, P. Trotzke, L. Lawrence, D. Fassnacht, K. Ali, N.M. Laskowski, A. MüllerBehavioral Neuroscience of Buying-Shopping Disorder: A Review
Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 5 (2018), pp. 263-270, 10.1007/s40473-018-0165-6

Lachmann et al., 2018

B. Lachmann, C. Sindermann, R.Y. Sariyska, R. Luo, M.C. Melchers, B. Becker, et al.The role of empathy and life satisfaction in internet and smartphone use disorder
Frontiers in Psychology, 9 (2018), p. 398, 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00398

Laier and Brand, 2017

C. Laier, M. BrandMood changes after watching pornography on the Internet are linked to tendencies towards Internet-pornography-viewing disorder
Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5 (2017), pp. 9-13, 10.1016/j.abrep.2016.11.003

Laier et al., 2018

C. Laier, E. Wegmann, M. BrandPersonality and cognition in gamers: Avoidance expectancies mediate the relationship between maladaptive personality traits and symptoms of Internet-gaming disorder
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9 (2018), pp. 1-8, 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00304

Lam and Lam, 2017

L.T. Lam, M.K. LamThe association between financial literacy and problematic Internet shopping in a multinational sample
Addictive Behaviors Reports, 6 (2017), pp. 123-127, 10.1016/j.abrep.2017.10.002

Lee et al., 2018a

D. Lee, J. Lee, K. Namkoong, Y.C. JungSubregions of the anterior cingulate cortex form distinct functional connectivity patterns in young males with internet gaming disorder with comorbid depression
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9 (2018), pp. 1-9, 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00380

Lee et al., 2018b

D. Lee, K. Namkoong, J. Lee, Y.C. JungAbnormal gray matter volume and impulsivity in young adults with Internet gaming disorder
Addiction Biology, 23 (2018), pp. 1160-1167, 10.1111/adb.12552

Lemenager et al., 2016

T. Lemenager, J. Dieter, H. Hill, S. Hoffmann, I. Reinhard, M. Beutel, et al.Exploring the neural basis of avatar identification in pathological Internet gamers and of self-reflection in pathological social network users
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 5 (2016), pp. 485-499, 10.1556/2006.5.2016.048

Li et al., 2018

W. Li, E.L. Garland, J.E. O’Brien, C. Tronnier, P. McGovern, B. Anthony, M.O. HowardMindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for video game addiction in emerging adults: Preliminary findings from case reports
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 16 (2018), pp. 928-945, 10.1007/s11469-017-9765-8

Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2013

E.H. Limbrick-Oldfield, R.J. van Holst, L. ClarkFronto-striatal dysregulation in drug addiction and pathological gambling: Consistent inconsistencies?
Neuroimage Clinical, 2 (2013), pp. 385-393, 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.02.005

Lobo, 2016

D.S.S. LoboGenetic aspects of gambling disorders: Recent developments and future directions
Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 3 (2016), pp. 58-66, 10.1007/s40473-016-0064-7

Luijten et al., 2017

M. Luijten, A.F. Schellekens, S. Kuhn, M.W. Machielse, G. SescousseDisruption of reward processing in addiction: An image-based meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies
JAMA Psychiatry, 74 (4) (2017), pp. 387-398, 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3084

Männikkö et al., 2017

N. Männikkö, H. Ruotsalainen, J. Miettunen, H.M. Pontes, M. KääriäinenProblematic gaming behaviour and health-related outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis
Journal of Health Psychology (2017), 10.1177/1359105317740414

Meng et al., 2015

Y. Meng, W. Deng, H. Wang, W. Guo, T. LiThe prefrontal dysfunction in individuals with Internet gaming disorder: A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies
Addiction Biology, 20 (2015), pp. 799-808, 10.1111/adb.12154

Mitelman, 2019

S.A. MitelmanTransdiagnostic neuroimaging in psychiatry: A review
Psychiatry Research (2019), 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.01.026

Montag et al., 2017

C. Montag, A. Markowetz, K. Blaszkiewicz, I. Andone, B. Lachmann, R. Sariyska, et al.Facebook usage on smartphones and gray matter volume of the nucleus accumbens
Behavioural Brain Research, 329 (2017), pp. 221-228, 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.04.035

Montag et al., 2018

C. Montag, Z. Zhao, C. Sindermann, L. Xu, M. Fu, J. Li, et al.Internet Communication Disorder and the structure of the human brain: Initial insights on WeChat addiction
Scientific Reports, 8 (2018), pp. 1-10, 10.1038/s41598-018-19904-y

Morris et al., 2016

L.S. Morris, P. Kundu, N. Dowell, D.J. Mechelmans, P. Favre, M.A. Irvine, et al.Fronto-striatal organization: Defining functional and microstructural substrates of behavioural flexibility
Cortex, 74 (2016), pp. 118-133, 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.004

Müller et al., 2019

A. Müller, M. Brand, L. Claes, Z. Demetrovics, M. de Zwaan, F. Fernández-Aranda, et al.Buying-shopping disorder – Is there enough evidence to support its inclusion in ICD-11?
CNS Spectrums (2019), pp. 1-6, 10.1017/S1092852918001323

Müller et al., 2014

A. Müller, L. Claes, E. Georgiadou, M. Möllenkamp, E.M. Voth, R.J. Faber, et al.Is compulsive buying related to materialism, depression or temperament? Findings from a sample of treatment-seeking patients with CB
Psychiatry Research, 216 (2014), pp. 103-107, 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.012

Naaijen et al., 2015

J. Naaijen, D.J. Lythgoe, H. Amiri, J.K. Buitelaar, J.C. GlennonFronto-striatal glutamatergic compounds in compulsive and impulsive syndromes: A review of magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 52 (2015), pp. 74-88, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.009

Namkung et al., 2017

H. Namkung, S.H. Kim, A. SawaThe insula: An underestimated brain area in clinical neuroscience, psychiatry, and neurology
Trends in Neurosciences, 40 (2017), pp. 200-207, 10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.002

Noël et al., 2006

X. Noël, M. van der Linden, A. BecharaThe neurocognitive mechanisms of decision-making, impulse control, and loss of willpower to resist drugs
Psychiatry (Edgmont), 3 (2006), pp. 30-41

Öner, 2018

S. ÖnerNeural substrates of cognitive emotion regulation: A brief review
Psychiatry and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 28 (2018), pp. 91-96, 10.1080/24750573.2017.1407563

Palaus et al., 2017

M. Palaus, E.M. Marron, R. Viejo-Sobera, D. Redolar-RipollNeural basis of video gaming: A systematic review
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11 (2017), p. 248, 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00248

Pascoli et al., 2018

V. Pascoli, A. Hiver, R. Van Zessen, M. Loureiro, R. Achargui, M. Harada, et al.Stochastic synaptic plasticity underlying compulsion in a model of addiction
Nature, 564 (2018), pp. 366-371, 10.1038/s41586-018-0789-4

Paulus et al., 2018

F.W. Paulus, S. Ohmann, A. von Gontard, C. PopowInternet gaming disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 60 (2018), pp. 645-659, 10.1111/dmcn.13754

Piazza and Deroche-Gamonet, 2013

P.V. Piazza, V. Deroche-GamonetA multistep general theory of transition to addiction
Psychopharmacology, 229 (2013), pp. 387-413

Potenza, 2013

M.N. PotenzaNeurobiology of gambling behaviors
Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23 (2013), pp. 660-667, 10.1016/j.conb.2013.03.004

Potenza, 2017

M.N. PotenzaClinical neuropsychiatric considerations regarding nonsubstance or behavioral addictions
Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 19 (2017), pp. 281-291

Potenza, 2018

M.N. PotenzaSearching for replicable dopamine-related findings in gambling disorder
Biological Psychiatry, 83 (2018), pp. 984-986, 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.011

Potenza et al., 2018

M.N. Potenza, S. Higuchi, M. BrandCall for research into a wider range of behavioural addictions
Nature, 555 (2018), p. 30, 10.1038/d41586-018-02568-z

Raab et al., 2011

G. Raab, C.E. Elger, M. Neuner, B. WeberA neurological study of compulsive buying behaviour
Journal of Consumer Policy, 34 (2011), pp. 401-413, 10.1007/s10603-011-9168-3

Robbins et al., 2019

T.W. Robbins, M.M. Vaghi, P. BancaObsessive-compulsive disorder: Puzzles and prospects
Neuron, 102 (2019), pp. 27-47, 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.046

Roberts et al., 2017

A. Roberts, S. Sharman, J. Coid, R. Murphy, H. Bowden-Jones, S. Cowlishaw, J. LandonGambling and negative life events in a nationally representative sample of UK men
Addictive Behaviors, 75 (2017), pp. 95-102, 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.07.002

Robinson and Berridge, 2008

T.E. Robinson, K.C. BerridgeThe incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363 (2008), pp. 3137-3146, 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093

Rothen et al., 2018

S. Rothen, J.F. Briefer, J. Deleuze, L. Karila, C.S. Andreassen, S. Achab, et al.Disentangling the role of users’ preferences and impulsivity traits in problematic Facebook use
PloS One, 13 (2018), pp. 1-13, 10.1371/journal.pone.0201971

Ryu et al., 2018

H. Ryu, J.Y. Lee, A. Choi, S. Park, D.J. Kim, J.S. ChoiThe relationship between impulsivity and Internet gaming disorder in young adults: Mediating effects of interpersonal relationships and depression
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15 (3) (2018), pp. 1-11, 10.3390/ijerph15030458

Sariyska et al., 2017

R. Sariyska, B. Lachmann, S. Markett, M. Reuter, C. MontagIndividual differences in implicit learning abilities and impulsive behavior in the context of Internet addiction and Internet Gaming Disorder under the consideration of gender
Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5 (2017), pp. 19-28, 10.1016/j.abrep.2017.02.002

Schiebener and Brand, 2015

J. Schiebener, M. BrandDecision making under objective risk conditions – a review of cognitive and emotional correlates, strategies, feedback processing, and external influences
Neuropsychology Review, 25 (2015), pp. 171-198

Schmidt et al., 2017

C. Schmidt, L.S. Morris, T.L. Kvamme, P. Hall, T. Birchard, V. VoonCompulsive sexual behavior: Prefrontal and limbic volume and interactions
Human Brain Mapping, 38 (2017), pp. 1182-1190, 10.1002/hbm.23447

Schneider et al., 2017

L.A. Schneider, D.L. King, P.H. DelfabbroFamily factors in adolescent problematic Internet gaming: A systematic review
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6 (3) (2017), pp. 321-333, 10.1556/2006.6.2017.035

Schneider et al., 2018

L.A. Schneider, D.L. King, P.H. DelfabbroMaladaptive coping styles in adolescents with Internet gaming disorder symptoms
International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 16 (4) (2018), pp. 905-916, 10.1007/s11469-017-9756-9

Starcevic and Khazaal, 2017

V. Starcevic, Y. KhazaalRelationship between behavioural addictions and psychiatric disorder: What is known and what is yet to be learned
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8 (53) (2017), 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00053

Starcke et al., 2018

K. Starcke, S. Antons, P. Trotzke, M. BrandCue-reactivity in behavioral addictions: A meta-analysis and methodological considerations
Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7 (2018), pp. 227-238, 10.1556/2006.7.2018.39

Stark et al., 2018

R. Stark, T. Klucken, M.N. Potenza, M. Brand, J. StrahlerA current understanding of the behavioral neuroscience of compulsive sexual behavior disorder and problematic pornography use
Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 5 (2018), pp. 218-231, 10.1007/s40473-018-0162-9

Stark et al., 2017

R. Stark, O. Kruse, S. Wehrum-Osinsky, J. Snagowski, M. Brand, B. Walter, T. KluckenPredictors for (problematic) use of Internet sexually explicit material: Role of trait sexual motivation and implicit approach tendencies towards sexual explicit material
Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 24 (2017), pp. 180-202

Strack and Deutsch, 2004

F. Strack, R. DeutschReflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior
Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8 (2004), pp. 220-247, 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1

Strahler et al., 2018

J. Strahler, O. Kruse, S. Wehrum-Osinsky, T. Klucken, R. StarkNeural correlates of gender differences in distractibility by sexual stimuli
Neuroimage, 176 (2018), pp. 499-509, 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.072

Trotzke et al., 2017

P. Trotzke, M. Brand, K. StarckeCue-reactivity, craving, and decision making in buying disorder: A review of the current knowledge and future directions
Current Addiction Reports, 4 (2017), pp. 246-253, 10.1007/s40429-017-0155-x

Trotzke et al., 2014

P. Trotzke, K. Starcke, A. Pedersen, M. BrandCue-induced craving in pathological buying: Empirical evidence and clinical implications
Psychosomatic Medicine, 76 (2014), pp. 694-700

Turel and Qahri-Saremi, 2016

O. Turel, H. Qahri-SaremiProblematic use of Social Networking Sites: Antecedents and consequence from a dual system theory perspective
Journal of Management Information Systems, 33 (2016), pp. 1087-1116

van den Heuvel et al., 2016

O.A. van den Heuvel, G. van Wingen, C. Soriano-Mas, P. Alonso, S.R. Chamberlain, T. Nakamae, et al.Brain circuitry of compulsivity
European Neuropsychopharmacology, 26 (2016), pp. 810-827, 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.005

van Holst et al., 2010

R.J. van Holst, W. van den Brink, D.J. Veltman, A.E. GoudriaanWhy gamblers fail to win: A review of cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34 (2010), pp. 87-107, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.007

van Timmeren et al., 2018

T. van Timmeren, J.G. Daams, R.J. van Holst, A.E. GoudriaanCompulsivity-related neurocognitive performance deficits in gambling disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 84 (2018), pp. 204-217, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.022

Vargas et al., 2019

T. Vargas, J. Maloney, T. Gupta, K.S.F. Damme, N.J. Kelley, V.A. MittalMeasuring facets of reward sensitivity, inhibition, and impulse control in individuals with problematic Internet use
Psychiatry Research, 275 (2019), pp. 351-358, 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.03.032

Vogel et al., 2018

V. Vogel, I. Kollei, T. Duka, J. Snagowski, M. Brand, A. Müller, S. LoeberPavlovian-to-instrumental transfer: A new paradigm to assess pathological mechanisms with regard to the use of Internet applications
Behavioural Brain Research, 347 (2018), pp. 8-16, 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.03.009

Volkow and Baler, 2015

N.D. Volkow, R.D. BalerNOW vs LATER brain circuits: Implications for obesity and addiction
Trends in Neurosciences, 38 (2015), pp. 345-352, 10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.002

Volkow et al., 2016

N.D. Volkow, G.F. Koob, A.T. McLellanNeurobiologic advances from the brain disease model of addiction
New England Journal of Medicine, 374 (2016), pp. 363-371, 10.1056/NEJMra1511480

Volkow and Morales, 2015

N.D. Volkow, M. MoralesThe brain on drugs: From reward to addiction
Cell, 162 (2015), pp. 712-725, 10.1016/j.cell.2015.07.046

Volkow et al., 2012

N.D. Volkow, G.J. Wang, J.S. Fowler, D. TomasiAddiction circuitry in the human brain
Annual Review of Pharmacology and Toxicology, 52 (2012), pp. 321-336, 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-010611-134625

Volkow et al., 2013

N.D. Volkow, G.J. Wang, D. Tomasi, R.D. BalerUnbalanced neuronal circuits in addiction
Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23 (2013), pp. 639-648, 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.002

Voon et al., 2014

V. Voon, T.B. Mole, P. Banca, L. Porter, L. Morris, S. Mitchell, et al.Neural correlates of sexual cue reactivity in individuals with and without compulsive sexual behaviours
PloS One, 9 (2014), Article e102419, 10.1371/journal.pone.0102419

Wegmann et al., 2018a

E. Wegmann, S. Mueller, S. Ostendorf, M. BrandHighlighting Internet-communication disorder as further Internet-use disorder when considering neuroimaging studies
Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports, 5 (2018), pp. 295-301, 10.1007/s40473-018-0164-7

Wegmann et al., 2018b

E. Wegmann, S. Ostendorf, M. BrandIs it beneficial to use Internet-communication for escaping from boredom? Boredom proneness interacts with cue-induced craving and avoidance expectancies in explaining symptoms of Internet-communication disorder
PloS One, 13 (4) (2018), Article e0195742, 10.1371/journal.pone.0195742

Wei et al., 2017

L. Wei, S. Zhang, O. Turel, A. Bechara, Q. HeA tripartite neurocognitive model of Internet Gaming Disorder
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8 (285) (2017), 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00285

Weinstein, 2017

A.M. WeinsteinAn update overview on brain imaging studies of Internet gaming disorder
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8 (2017), p. 185, 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00185

Weinstein et al., 2017

A.M. Weinstein, A. Livny, A. WeizmanNew developments in brain research of internet and gaming disorder
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 75 (2017), pp. 314-330, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.040

Wéry et al., 2018

A. Wéry, J. Deleuze, N. Canale, J. BillieuxEmotionally laden impulsivity interacts with affect in predicting addictive use of online sexual activity in men
Comprehensive Psychiatry, 80 (2018), pp. 192-201, 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.10.004

Wilcox et al., 2016

C.E. Wilcox, J.M. Pommy, B. AdinoffNeural circuitry of impaired emotion regulation in substance use disorders
American Journal of Psychiatry, 173 (2016), pp. 344-361, 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15060710

World-Health-Organization, 2019

World-Health-OrganizationICD-11 for Mortality and Morbidity Statistics
Retrieved from

Xuan et al., 2017

Y.-H. Xuan, S. Li, R. Tao, J. Chen, L.-L. Rao, X.T. Wang, R. ZhengGenetic and environmental influences on gambling: A meta-analysis of twin studies
Frontiers in Psychology, 8 (2017), 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02121
2121–2121

Yao et al., 2017

Y.W. Yao, L. Liu, S.S. Ma, X.H. Shi, N. Zhou, J.T. Zhang, M.N. PotenzaFunctional and structural neural alterations in Internet gaming disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 83 (2017), pp. 313-324, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.029

Yip et al., 2019

S.W. Yip, D. Scheinost, M.N. Potenza, K.M. CarrollConnectome-based prediction of cocaine abstinence
American Journal of Psychiatry (2019), 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17101147

Zhou et al., 2018a

F. Zhou, K. Zimmermann, F. Xin, D. Scheele, W. Dau, M. Banger, et al.Shifted balance of dorsal versus ventral striatal communication with frontal reward and regulatory regions in cannabis-dependent males
Human Brain Mapping, 39 (2018), pp. 5062-5073, 10.1002/hbm.24345

Zhou et al., 2018b

N. Zhou, H. Cao, X. Li, J. Zhang, Y. Yao, X. Geng, et al.Internet addiction, problematic internet use, nonproblematic internet use among Chinese adolescents: Individual, parental, peer, and sociodemographic correlates
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 32 (2018), pp. 365-372, 10.1037/adb0000358

Zhou et al., 2019

X. Zhou, K. Zimmermann, F. Xin, R. Derck, A. Sassmannshausen, D. Scheele, et al.Cue-reactivity in the ventral striatum characterizes heavy cannabis use, whereas reactivity in the dorsal striatum mediates dependent use
bioRxiv (2019), 10.1101/516385

Zilberman et al., 2019

N. Zilberman, M. Lavidor, G. Yadid, Y. RassovskyQualitative review and quantitative effect size meta-analyses in brain regions identified by cue-reactivity addiction studies
Neuropsychology, 33 (2019), pp. 319-334, 10.1037/neu0000526
Review. PubMed PMID: 30816782

Zilberman et al., 2018

N. Zilberman, G. Yadid, Y. Efratim, Y. Neumark, Y. RassovskyPersonality profiles of substance and behavioral addictions
Addictive Behaviors, 82 (2018), pp. 174-181, 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.03.007
Epub 2018 Mar 6. PubMed PMID: 29547799