Are Sex Drive and Hypersexuality Associated with Pedophilic Interest and Child Sexual Abuse in a Male Community Sample? (2015)

COMMENTS: Study suggests that general escalation through porn genres may lead to using child porn (or at least underage porn such as teen porn). From the discussion:

“Hence, for all three constructs there seem to be substantial independent links to child pornography consumption. In the literature several explanations for child pornography consumption are discussed. The underlying motivational aspects to engage in the consumption of child pornography seem to be sexual interests in children and/or thrill-seeking behavior as a result of habituation to mainstream pornography [27, 28]. Pornography dependence is a common sexual behavioral pattern in hypersexual men [1, 2]. Therefore, the association between sex drive and child pornography consumption may be explained by the fact that frequent pornography consumption and a wide-spread interest in all sorts of (a)typical pornography can be seen as indications of increased sex drive. Accordingly, in a sample of male juveniles, Svedin et al [29] identified a relationship between frequent pornography use and child pornography consumption. Hence, it seems possible that persons who frequently engage in pornography consumption have an increased risk of getting in contact with child pornography [30]. Similarly, in a further study the variable frequent sexual lust predicted child pornography use [28]. Still, it remains unclear whether sex drive leads to pornography use, or vice versa. Also a circular reinforcing process in which the availability of pornography on the internet serves as a strong reinforcer for hypersexual behavior seems possible. Therefore, a non-correlative (i.e., ideally longitudinal) examination of the causal extent to which hypersexual behavior/sex drive impacts the use of atypical pornography use should be aimed for in future research.”


Published: July 6, 2015

Verena Klein, Alexander F. Schmidt, Daniel Turner, Peer Briken

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129730

Correction

28 Sep 2015: The PLOS ONE Staff (2015) Correction: Are Sex Drive and Hypersexuality Associated with Pedophilic Interest and Child Sexual Abuse in a Male Community Sample? PLoS ONE 10(9): e0139533. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139533 View correction

Abstract

Although much is currently known about hypersexuality (in the form of excessive sexual behavior) among sexual offenders, the degree to which hypersexual behavior is linked to paraphilic and especially pedophilic interests in non-forensic populations has not been established. The purpose of the present study was to elucidate the associations between total sexual outlets (TSO) and other sex drive indicators, antisocial behavior, pedophilic interests, and sexual offending behavior in a large population-based community sample of males. The sample included 8,718 German men who participated in an online study. Hypersexual behavior as measured by self-reported TSO, self-reported sex drive, criminal history, and pedophilic interests were assessed. In moderated hierarchical logistic regression analyses self-reported contact sexual offending against children was linked to sexual fantasizing about children and antisociality. There was no association between aggregated sex drive, and sexual abusive behaviour in the multivariate analyses. In contrast, self-reported child pornography consumption was associated with sex drive, sexual fantasies involving children, and antisociality. Nevertheless, in clinical practice an assessment of criminal history and pedophilic interests in hypersexual individuals and vice versa hypersexuality in antisocial or pedophilic men should be considered as particularly antisociality and pedophilic interest are important predictors of sexual offending against prepubescent children.

Citation: Klein V, Schmidt AF, Turner D, Briken P (2015) Are Sex Drive and Hypersexuality Associated with Pedophilic Interest and Child Sexual Abuse in a Male Community Sample? PLoS ONE 10(7): e0129730. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0129730

Editor: Ulrich S. Tran, University of Vienna, School of Psychology, AUSTRIA

Received: January 9, 2015; Accepted: May 12, 2015; Published: July 6, 2015

Copyright: © 2015 Klein et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited

Data Availability: All relevant data are within the paper.

Funding: This research was supported by funding from the German Federal Ministry of Family Affairs, Senior Citizens, Women, and Youth.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

In recent years, there has been an increasing amount of literature providing empirical evidence for a link between hypersexuality and paraphilic interests in sexual offenders [1, 2]. Hypersexuality is used as an umbrella term to describe excessive sexual behavioral pattern in research and clinical practice. Kinsey et al. [3] coined the term “total sexual outlets/week” (TSO) in order to assess the frequency of sexual behavior. TSO was defined as “the sum of the orgasms derived from the various types of sexual activity in which that individual had engaged” ([3], pp. 510–511) during a week including sexual behaviors such as sexual intercourse and masturbation. Typically, TSO is characterized by a left skewed distribution and shows a peak for the age group between 15 and 25 years. Furthermore, TSO seems to be testosterone dependent and relatively independent from the individual relationship status [4]. In earlier research, Kafka [5] proposed seven orgasms per week over a period of six months as a criterion for hypersexual behavior. Further part of the definition was spending at least 1–2 hours per day with sexual behavior.

Possible prevalence estimations used the TSO/week ≥ 7 cut-off as a behavioral indicator for hypersexual behavior in non-clinical community samples of males. In the Kinsey et al. [3] study 7.6% out of 5,300 men reported a TSO/week ≥7 over the last five years. Masturbation was the most frequently reported sexual practice in the sample. Atwood and Gagnon [6] found that 5% of male high-school students and 3% of male college students masturbated once a day (N = 1,077). A large survey on sexual behavior in the United States identified daily masturbation in 3.1% of the male sample (N = 3,159). Furthermore, 7.6% of the men reported sexual intercourse at least four times a week [7]. In a population-based Swedish community sample (N = 2,450), 12.1% of male participants were identified as being hypersexual [8]. In the latter study high rates of impersonal sexual activity were associated with health problems such as substance abuse and gambling as well as with paraphilic interests in terms of voyeurism, exhibtionism, sadism, and masochism.

In sexual offender populations paraphilic interest has been meta-analytically established as the most important risk factor [9, 10]. Hypersexuality (or sexual preoccupation, high sex drive) is also found among the most important risk factors for sexual offending [11] and was identified as a possible contributing risk factor for sexual and violent reoffending in sexual offenders [12]. Furthermore, hypersexual behavior patterns seem to be more likely in sexual offenders than in community controls [13, 14]. In addition, high sex drive has been reported to be associated with sexual coercive behavior against women [15]. Pornography consumption understood as a behavioral pattern possibly related to hypersexual behavior was associated with recidivism in a sample of 341 high-risk sexual offenders against children. Additionally, sexual deviant content of pornography was a risk factor for reoffending in this sample [16]. One obvious limitation to most of these studies is the fact that they were based on exclusive samples of sexual offenders. However, in a young Swedish community-sample sexual preoccupation (defined as sexual lust almost all time) was identified as risk factor for self-reported sexual coercive behavior [17]. Notably, research on sexual aggression and hypersexual behavior is not altogether consistent. In a study by Malamuth et al [18] sexually aggressive men against females reported a higher preference for impersonal sexual activity (e.g. masturbation frequency, attitudes toward causal sex) but did not indicate higher frequency of orgasms per week and sexual intercourse. Hence, high sex drive did not contribute to sexual aggression in their sample. To the best of our knowledge, the study by Långström and Hanson [9] is the only study demonstrating an association between hypersexuality and paraphilic interests in a community sample.

Present Research

Our research question connects the findings about hypersexuality among sexual offenders to the link between hypersexual behavior and paraphilic, especially pedophilic interests, in non-forensic populations. Therefore, the first aim is to explore the association between pedophilic sexual interests/ sexual offending behavior and TSO/sex drive indicators in a large population-based community sample of males. Furthermore, due to a lack of research in community samples, only little is known about possible criminological factors and their putative associations with hypersexual behavior in males. Therefore, the second purpose of the present study was to elucidate the association between TSO, other sex drive indicators and antisocial behaviour including sexual offending against children. Moreover, most studies on TSO in community samples have neglected the amount of time spent with sexual fantasies and urges [1]. Thus, the present study also aimed to examine the relationship between TSO and the amount of time spent with sexual fantasizing and pornography consumption.

Materials and Methods

The reported data are part of a large population-based online study on German males’ sexual interest in prepubescent children [19]. The study was part of a research project funded by the German Federal Ministry of Family Affairs, Senior Citizens, Women, and Youth. A German market research institution was authorised to collect data via an online panel. Participants were informed beforehand in an email about the topic of the study. They provided an online consent form at the beginning of the survey by clicking the online consent form “accept” button. In addition, it was possible to withdraw from the study at any point simply by leaving the survey´s web page. At the end of the survey an option was offered that prevented individual data from being included into the analyses. Complete anonymity and confidentiality was assured to potential participants. Therefore, a university server was used to store the collected data whereas a separate server coded participation status in order to ensure participant compensation via the market research institution. Furthermore, due to this procedure, it was impossible to identify individuals in case legal authorities intended to prosecute men who admitted criminal behavior. The participants were informed about this procedure so that they might answer honestly and received a monetary reward of 20 €. The ethics committee of the German Psychological Society approved the study protocol and the consent procedure.

In total, 17,917 men (≥ 18 years of age) were contacted by the market research institution in order to be representative of the German male population in terms of age and education levels. In consequence, the link has been accessed 10,538 times and data were collected for 10,045 participants. Because of missing data in the individual surveys the effective sample was reduced to 8,718 participants (48.7% of the initially contacted men; 82.7% of participants actually accessing the link). Participants’ mean age was 43.5 years (SD = 13.7, range 18–89). Concerning their professional status most of the participants were employed (71.5%, n = 6,179) or retired (13.1%, n = 1,143), 5.6% (n = 488) of the participants were unemployed and 9.7% (n = 836) were in professional training at the time of data collection. The majority of participants (56.4%, n = 4,874) had a school leaving examination taken at the end of the 13th year, 30.3% (n = 2,618) finished school with a high-school diploma, 12.7% (n = 1,104) with a secondary modern school qualification, 0.3% (n = 24) had no graduation, and 0.3% (n = 28) were still in school. Participants differed from the German male population in terms of age and education levels as there was an overrepresentation of higher education and the age range of 30–49 whereas lower education and men over 65 were underrepresented [19]. For detailed results concerning self-reported prevalences of sexual interest in prepubescent children refer to [19].

Measures

TSO was measured with the following question: “Please think of a typical week in the last year: How many orgasms did you have on average no matter how the orgasm was achieved (e.g., masturbation, sexual encounters, wet dreams)?”. In addition, sex drive (“Please think of a typical week in the last year: How strong was your desire for sexual activity?”) and the amount of time spent with sexual fantasies, sexual urges, and sexual behavior (“Please think of a typical day in the last year: Please estimate the amount of time you spend with sexual fantasies, sexual urges, and sexual behavior.”) as well as with pornography consumption (“Please think of a typical day in the last year: Please estimate the amount of time you spend viewing pornography (e.g., naked genitals) in order to be sexually aroused?”) were assessed. Sex drive was rated on a 100-point slider scale. The amount of time spent with sexual fantasies, sexual urges, and sexual behavior as well as with pornography was assessed using an open answer format (hours and minutes per day). Sexual fantasies and behaviors directed at prepubescent children were assessed with a shortened 12-item version of the Explicit Sexual Interest Questionnaire (ESIQ) [20]. The ESIQ has been shown to be a reliable and valid measure of adult and pedophilic sexual interests [2022]. The items of the shortened version referred to four sexual target categories (prepubescent boys or girls ≤ 12 years and women or men) and consisted of each three items describing sexual fantasies (“I find it erotic to see a …’s body through the clothes”, “I get excited when I imagine that a … stimulates me”, “I find it erotic to imagine having sex with a …”) and sexual behaviors (“I have sexually caressed a…”, “I have tongue kissed a …”, “I have enjoyed getting my private parts touched by a …”). Participants had to indicate on a dichotomous scale (true/false) whether they had experienced the corresponding sexual fantasies and behaviors as adults (> 18 years). The reliability (internal consistency) of the aggregated ESIQ subscales was good: sexual fantasies involving girls (α = .81), boys (α = .86), women (α = .90), and men (α = .92). Child sexual fantasy items were used as indicators of pedophilic interest whereas child sexual behavior items were used as indicating sexual offenses against children. Child pornography consumption was assessed with the following item: “Have you ever watched pornographic depictions of children, e.g., the nude genitals of children, to get sexually aroused after you were 18 years of age?” [true/false]). Again, children were anchored to represent prepubescent stages of sexual maturity. In order to examine participants’ antisocial behavior and criminal history they were asked to answer the following three forced-choice questions: 1. Have you ever been convicted of an offense against property (etc. larceny, burglary)?; 2. Have you ever been convicted of a violent offense (etc. bodily injury)?; 3. Have you ever been convicted of a sexual offense (etc. sexual coercion, rape, sexual abuse)?

Statistical Analyses

In order to robustly identify outliers the median absolute deviation (MAD) [23] was calculated for TSO, amount of time spent with sexual fantasies, urges, and behavior as well as amount of time watching pornography. The MAD analyses yielded cut-offs for outliers of TSO ≥ 10, ≥ 165 minutes for daily sexual fantasies, urges, and behaviors, as well as ≥ 95 minutes for daily pornography consumption. Correlational analyses were conducted to verify the association between absolute TSO (i.e., as a dimensional construct), subjective sex drive, amount of time spent with sexual fantasies and viewing pornography. Further correlations were calculated to examine the relationship between TSO or sex drive indicators and pedophilic interests, sexually offending behavior, and criminal history. In order to elucidate the impact of the categorical cut-off for hypersexuality, participants were divided in two groups: low and high self-reported hypersexuality as based on the proposed cut-off value TSO ≥ 7 by Kafka [5]. Because frequency of sexual activity and sex drive on average declines with age in so far as younger individuals report more sexual outlets per week [7] we conducted additional analyses utilizing partial correlations controlled for age. Finally, we conducted moderated hierarchical binary logistic regression analyses [24] to test for possible interaction effects of sex drive indicators, antisociality, and child-related sexual fantasies on contact child sexual abuse and child pornography use.

Results

Overall, the mean TSO/week was 3.46 (SD = 2.29). On average the participants spent 45.2 minutes/day (SD = 38.1) with sexual fantasies and urges. The mean score of sex drive was 59.7 (SD = 21.4) and the reported daily duration for consuming pornography was 13.1 minutes (SD = 19.3). The non-hypersexual group consisted of 7,339 males (87.9%), whereas 1,011 males (12.1%) were classified into the hypersexual group according to the classical cut-off value TSO ≥ 7. Sex drive and TSO positively correlated with time consuming sexual fantasies and urges. Furthermore, a significant positive correlation occurred between TSO and sex drive with the amount of time spent with pornography consumption. Partial correlations corrected for possible age and education effects showed a very similar pattern of results (see Table 1). As all measures indicating sex drive were positively intercorrelated we calculated an aggregated sex drive index consisting of z-standardized TSO/week, subjective sex drive ratings, as well as amount of time spent viewing pornography and fantasizing about sexual content (α = .66). In addition, an antisociality index was determined by aggregating self-reported preconvictions (violent, property, sexual). As sexual preconvictions could overlap with self-reported sexual victimization of children we also calculated an aggregated antisociality index leaving out sexual preconvictions.

thumbnail    

 
Table 1. Overview of sex drive intercorrelations (above diagonal zero-order correlations, below diagonal partial correlations corrected for age and education).

 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129730.t001

Zero-order correlations

In order to verify the relation between TSO and pedophilic interests, correlation analyses were conducted. Sex drive, TSO, and TSO ≥ 7 were positively associated with sexual fantasies involving children and child pornography consumption. Additionally, aggregated sex drive positively correlated with self-reported sexual offending behavior in the past. Concerning antisocial behavior, TSO and TSO ≥ 7 were positively related to a history of property and violent offences in the past, whereas no association with sexual offending was found. Aggregated sex drive showed a positive correlation with all offending categories. However, effect sizes were small (Table 2).

thumbnail    

 
Table 2. Overview of zero-order child sexual abuse risk factor intercorrelations.

 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129730.t002

Logistic regression analyses

Hierarchical logistic regression analyses revealed that self-reported contact sexual offending against prepubescent children was associated with child sexual fantasies and antisociality (without sexual preconvictions). In addition, a significant interaction between antisociality and sexual fantasies involving children emerged corroborating a moderation effect (Fig 1): For men who reported no preconvictions in the sample no link between sexual fantasizing about children and contact sexual offending against children emerged. However, the likelihood to report contact sexual abuse of children significantly increased for men who had reported prior convictions for criminal offences from two different categories (violence, property). Notably, aggregated sex drive showed no association with contact sexual abuse and no further interaction effects emerged (Table 3). Similar logistic regression analyses with self-reported child pornography consumption as criterion identified three independent links to sex drive, sexual fantasies involving children, and antisociality excluding sexual preconvictions. No further interactions were revealed.

thumbnail    

 
Fig 1. Probability of contact child sexual abuse as a function of self-reported amount of child sexual fantasies (+ 1 SD vs. – 1 SD) and antisociality (aggregated non-sexual preconvictions; average of the sample [low] vs. two different preconvictions [high]).

 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129730.g001

thumbnail    

 
Table 3. Summary of hierarchical logistic regression analyses for child sexual abuse as a function of sex drive, antisociality, and sexual fantasies involving children.

 

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129730.t003

Although, the moderation explained statistically significant shares of criterion variance the net increments were practically irrelevant as they accounted for only a 1% increase in explained variance. However, the independent multivariate main effects ranged between odds ratios from 1.1 to 2.0 (Table 3) for antisociality, sexual fantasies involving prepubescent children, and sex drive (the latter only in case of self-reported child pornography use).

Discussion

The present study provides clinically relevant insight into the actual paraphilic and criminological correlates of hypersexual behavior in a large non-clinical male community sample. Etiological models and theories on sexual offending against children consider paraphilic sexual interest and antisociality as import contributing risk factors for sexual abusive behavior [9, 25]. The present results are in line with this notion. In multivariate statistical analyses antisocial behavior and sexual fantasies involving children, an indicator for paraphilic interests, were associated with contact child sexual abuse. Moreover, the significant interaction between antisociality and sexual fantasies involving children in logistic regression analyses may indicate that the probability of contact sexual abuse markedly increases in men with particularly high rates of self-reported amount of sexual fantasies involving children and antisocial behavior in the past. In contrast, neither sex drive as such nor in combination with pedophilic fantasies showed an association with contact sexual abuse. Hence, the present results indicate that the effects of sex drive in general and specifically hypersexual behavior as measured by the TSO on self-reported contact sexual abusive behaviour with children are rather small on the level of zero-order correlations and completely vanish once entered into multivariate analyses.

Current research provides evidence for child pornography consumption as predictor for sexual interest in children [26]. Among pornography users, Ray et al [27] found that child pornography users were also more likely to report interest in sexual contact with children. Furthermore, an association between child pornography consumption and sexual coercive behavior was identified in a sample of young Scandinavian men [28]. Consistent with previous results, in the present sample child pornography consumption was positively related to contact sexual offending against children and sexual fantasies involving children. In addition, aggregated sex drive, antisocial behavior, and sexual fantasies involving children were identified as risk factors for child pornography consumption. Hence, for all three constructs there seem to be substantial independent links to child pornography consumption. In the literature several explanations for child pornography consumption are discussed. The underlying motivational aspects to engage in the consumption of child pornography seem to be sexual interests in children and/or thrill-seeking behavior as a result of habituation to mainstream pornography [27, 28]. Pornography dependence is a common sexual behavioral pattern in hypersexual men [1, 2]. Therefore, the association between sex drive and child pornography consumption may be explained by the fact that frequent pornography consumption and a wide-spread interest in all sorts of (a)typical pornography can be seen as indications of increased sex drive. Accordingly, in a sample of male juveniles, Svedin et al [29] identified a relationship between frequent pornography use and child pornography consumption. Hence, it seems possible that persons who frequently engage in pornography consumption have an increased risk of getting in contact with child pornography [30]. Similarly, in a further study the variable frequent sexual lust predicted child pornography use [28]. Still, it remains unclear whether sex drive leads to pornography use, or vice versa. Also a circular reinforcing process in which the availability of pornography on the internet serves as a strong reinforcer for hypersexual behavior seems possible. Therefore, a non-correlative (i.e., ideally longitudinal) examination of the causal extent to which hypersexual behavior/sex drive impacts the use of atypical pornography use should be aimed for in future research.

The known association between antisocial behavior and testosterone [31] has not been investigated in its relationship with sexuality related aspects. In the present study a relation between high TSO and indicators for antisociality were found. However, these findings are limited by small effect sizes. Future research should therefore examine more closely the interplay between the level of testosterone, TSO, and antisociality.

Limitations and Outlook

There is a lack of research on the association between the amount of time spent with sexual fantasies and urges with TSO in community samples [1]. In the present study, TSO and sex drive were associated with higher rates of time consuming sexual fantasies and pornography use. This finding was expected and suggests that the amount of time spent with sexual activities may be important for the definition of hypersexual behavior [5]. Nevertheless, for the definition of a clinical disorder not only symptomatic behavior but psychological distress and/or maybe also the criterion of causing harm to non-consenting others should be taken into concern. In the consequence, the present study is limited by the lack of information on clinically relevant characteristics caused by hypersexual behavior or high sex drive, even if the potential criterion causing harm to non-consenting others was considered. Further research on hypersexual behavior should address clinical distress as criterion beside the amount of orgasms and time spending with sexuality-related issues.

Several limitations of this study need to be acknowledged. First of all the data are based on self-report and results are limited to the German population. Moreover, the effect sizes in statistical analyses particularly in the interaction terms were rather small. This study was also limited by its cross-sectional correlational design. In addition, it is important to note that in the present study hypersexual behavior and sex drive were based on self-report and should not be confused neither with constructs which are used in sexual offender risk assessments such as sexual preoccupation in the Stable-2007 [32] nor with the diagnostic criteria of hypersexual disorder [1]. Furthermore, questions about total sexual outlets, sex drive, and sexual fantasies/urges were asked in the form of “a typical week or typical day in the past year”. This kind of formulation could be more vulnerable to recall bias than asking about the past week, where the week is “randomly selected” and thus might be construed as more representative of the past year. Also the operationalization of antisocial behavior as preconvictions can be deemed a rather conservative criterion. Further studies could measure antisocial behavior by asking if the person had ever stolen, committed assault, or other antisocial acts. Another aspect to be added to future research is the distinction between intercourse/sexual activity within relationships and impersonal sexual activity. This can be hypothesized to be particularly important, because sexual activity in a stable relationship on average is associated with positive mood whereas high rates of impersonal sexual activity are often related to negative mood states [7, 8]. In order to examine possible intimacy problems that may correlate with hypersexuality, further studies, which take the proposed distinction between sexual outlets into account, will need to be undertaken [2].

The results of the present study suggest that the association between hypersexual behaviour as measured by the TSO, sex drive, and contact sexual abusive behaviour in our community sample of men was lower than expected. In contrast, an association occurred between sex drive indicators and child pornography consumption. An implication of these findings is that in the assessment of hypersexual individuals atypical pornography consumption should be taken into account. Nonetheless, in clinical practice (and particularly in forensic populations) an assessment of criminal history and pedophilic interests in hypersexual individuals and vice versa hypersexuality in antisocial or pedophilic men should still be considered.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: PB AFS VK DT. Analyzed the data: AFS VK. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: VK AFS DT PB. Wrote the paper: VK AFS DT PB.

References

  1. 1. Kafka MP Hypersexual disorder: A proposed diagnosis for DSM-V (2010) Arch Sex Behav 39: 940–949. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9483-9. pmid:19308715
  2. 2. Kingston DA, Firestone P (2008) Problematic hypersexuality: A review of conceptualization and diagnosis. Sex Addict Compul 15: 284–310. doi: 10.1080/10720160802289249
  3. View Article
  4. PubMed/NCBI
  5. Google Scholar
  6. View Article
  7. PubMed/NCBI
  8. Google Scholar
  9. View Article
  10. PubMed/NCBI
  11. Google Scholar
  12. View Article
  13. PubMed/NCBI
  14. Google Scholar
  15. View Article
  16. PubMed/NCBI
  17. Google Scholar
  18. View Article
  19. PubMed/NCBI
  20. Google Scholar
  21. View Article
  22. PubMed/NCBI
  23. Google Scholar
  24. View Article
  25. PubMed/NCBI
  26. Google Scholar
  27. View Article
  28. PubMed/NCBI
  29. Google Scholar
  30. View Article
  31. PubMed/NCBI
  32. Google Scholar
  33. View Article
  34. PubMed/NCBI
  35. Google Scholar
  36. View Article
  37. PubMed/NCBI
  38. Google Scholar
  39. View Article
  40. PubMed/NCBI
  41. Google Scholar
  42. View Article
  43. PubMed/NCBI
  44. Google Scholar
  45. View Article
  46. PubMed/NCBI
  47. Google Scholar
  48. View Article
  49. PubMed/NCBI
  50. Google Scholar
  51. View Article
  52. PubMed/NCBI
  53. Google Scholar
  54. View Article
  55. PubMed/NCBI
  56. Google Scholar
  57. View Article
  58. PubMed/NCBI
  59. Google Scholar
  60. View Article
  61. PubMed/NCBI
  62. Google Scholar
  63. View Article
  64. PubMed/NCBI
  65. Google Scholar
  66. View Article
  67. PubMed/NCBI
  68. Google Scholar
  69. View Article
  70. PubMed/NCBI
  71. Google Scholar
  72. View Article
  73. PubMed/NCBI
  74. Google Scholar
  75. View Article
  76. PubMed/NCBI
  77. Google Scholar
  78. 3. Kinsey AC, Pomeroy WB, Martin CE (1948) Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company.
  79. 4. Kafka MP (2012) Hypersexual Disorder: DSM-5 current status and contemporary controversies. Paper presented at the 12th International Association for the Treatment of Sexual Offenders. Berlin: Germany.
  80. 5. Kafka MP (1997) Hypersexual desire in males: An operational definition and clinical implications for males with paraphilias and paraphilia-related disorders. Arch Sex Behav 26: 505–526. pmid:9343636
  81. 6. Atwood JD, Gagnon J. Masturbatory behavior in college youth (1987) J Sex Educ Thera 13: 35–42.
  82. 7. Laumann EO, Gagnon JH, Michael RT, Michaels S (1994) The social organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  83. 8. Långström N, Hanson RK (2006) High rates of sexual behavior in the general population: correlates and predictors. Arch Sex Behav 35: 37–52. pmid:16502152 doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-8993-y
  84. 9. Hanson RK, Morton-Bourgon KE (2005) The characteristics of persistent sexual offenders: A meta-analysis of recidivism studies. J Consult Clin Psychol 73: 1154–1163. pmid:16392988 doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.73.6.1154
  85. 10. Mann RE, Hanson KR, Thornton D (2010) Assessing risk for sexual recidivism: Some proposals on the nature of psychologically meaningful risk factors. Sex Abuse 22: 191–217. doi: 10.1177/1079063210366039. pmid:20363981
  86. 11. Hanson RK, Harris AJR (2000) Where should we intervene? Dynamic predictors of sex offense recidivism. Crim Just Behav 27: 6–35. doi: 10.1177/0093854800027001002
  87. 12. Kingston DA, Bradford JM (2013) Hypersexuality and recidivism among sexual offenders. Sex Addict Compul 20: 91–105.
  88. 13. Marshall LE, Marshall WL (2008) Sexual addiction in incarcerated sexual offenders. Sex Addict Compul 13: 377–390. doi: 10.1080/10720160601011281
  89. 14. Marshall LE, Marshall WL, Moulden HM, Serran GA (2008) Prevalence of sexual addiction in incarcerated sexual offenders and matched community non offenders. Sex Addict Compul 15: 271–283. doi: 10.1080/10720160802516328
  90. 15. Knight RA, Sims Knight JE (2003) The developmental antecedents of sexual coercion against women: Testing alternative hypotheses with structural equation modeling. Ann N Y Acad Sci 989: 72–85. pmid:12839887 doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07294.x
  91. 16. Kingston DA, Fedoroff P, Firestone P, Curry S, Bradford JM (2008) Pornography use and sexual aggression: The impact of frequency and type of pornography use on recidivism among sexual offenders. Aggress Behav 34: 341–351. doi: 10.1002/ab.20250. pmid:18307171
  92. 17. Kjellgren C, Priebe G, Svedin CG, Långström N (2010) Sexually coercive behavior in male youth: population survey of general and specific risk factors. Arch Sex Behav 39: 1161–1169. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9572-9. pmid:19888644
  93. 18. Malamuth NM, Linz D, Heavey CL, Barnes G, Acker M (1995) Using the confluence model of sexual aggression to predict men’s conflict with women: A 10-year follow-up study. J Pers Soc Psychol 69: 353–369. pmid:7643309 doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.2.353
  94. 19. Dombert B, Schmidt AF, Banse R, Briken P, Hoyer J, Neutze J, et al. (2015) How common is males’ self-reported sexual interest in prepubescent children? J Sex Res in press.
  95. 20. Banse R, Schmidt AF, Clabour J (2010) Indirect measures of sexual interest in child sex offenders: A multimethod approach. Crim Just Behav 37: 319–335. doi: 10.1177/0093854809357598
  96. 21. Schmidt AF, Gykiere K, Vanhoeck K, Mann RE, Banse R (2014) Direct and indirect measures of sexual maturity preferences differentiate subtypes of child sexual abusers. Sex Abuse 26: 107–128 doi: 10.1177/1079063213480817. pmid:23524323
  97. 22. Schmidt AF, Mokros A, Banse R (2013) Is pedophilic sexual preference continuous? A taxometric analysis based on direct and indirect measures. Psychol Assess 25: 1146–1153. doi: 10.1037/a0033326. pmid:23815115
  98. 23. Leys C, Ley C, Klein O, Bernard P, Licata L (2013) Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. J Exp Soc Psychol 49: 764–766. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013
  99. 24. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS (2003) Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  100. 25. Seto MC (2008) Pedophilia and sexual offending against children: Theory, assessment, and intervention. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  101. 26. Seto MC, Cantor JM, Blanchard R (2006) Child pornography offenses are a valid diagnostic indicator of pedophilia. J Abnorm Psychol 115: 610. pmid:16866601 doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.115.3.610
  102. 27. Ray JV, Kimonis ER, Seto MC (2014) Correlates and moderators of child pornography consumption in a community sample. Sex Abuse 26: 523–45. doi: 10.1177/1079063213502678. pmid:24088812
  103. 28. Seto MC, Hermann CA, Kjellgren C, Priebe G, Svedin CG, Långström N (2014) Viewing child pornography: Prevalence and correlates in a representative community sample of young Swedish men. Arch Sex Behav. doi: 10.1007/s10508-013-0244-4
  104. 29. Svedin CG, Åkerman I, Priebe G (2011) Frequent users of pornography A population based epidemiological study of Swedish male adolescents. Journal Adolesc 34: 779–788. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.04.010
  105. 30. Seigfried-Spellar KC, Rogers MK (2013) Does deviant pornography use follow a Guttman-like progression? Comput Hum Behav 29: 1997–2003. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.04.018
  106. 31. Yildirim BO, Derksen JJ (2012) A review on the relationship between testosterone and life-course persistent antisocial behavior. Psychiatry Res 200: 984–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.07.044. pmid:22925371
  107. 32. Hanson RK, Harris AJR, Scott TL, Helmus L (2007) Assessing the risk of sexual offenders on community supervision: The Dynamic Supervision Project (Report No. 2007–05). Ottawa: ON: Public Safety and Emergency Preparedness Canada.