Contribution of sexual desire and motives to the compulsive use of cybersex (2019)

Cybersex includes a wide range of sex-related Internet activities, such as pornography, webcam, sex chat, online sex games, and dating (Döring, 2009; Wéry & Billieux, 2017). In addition to virtual encounters, cybersex can facilitate meeting real romantic and sexual partners. Other positive impacts of cybersex have been noted (Grov, Gillespie, Royce, & Lever, 2011). For instance, it can enhance sexual arousal and facilitate engagement in sexual practices (Allen, Kannis-Dymand, & Katsikitis, 2017). It can also help couples with sexual stimulation or with the exploration of new sexual practices (Albright, 2008; Philaretou, Mahfouz, & Allen, 2005).

Compulsive cybersex use seems to affect a small proportion of cybersex consumers (Dufour et al., 2016; Frangos, Frangos, & Sotiropoulos, 2010; Kafka, 2010) and is possibly associated with psychosocial distress, disturbances in fulfilling daily life responsibilities, and sleep-related problems (Grubbs, Volk, Exline, & Pargament, 2015; Karila et al., 2014). In several studies involving males and females, compulsive cybersex users, compared to controls, showed greater arousal and cue reactivity from watching porn (Brand et al., 2011; Laier & Brand, 2014; Laier, Pawlikowski, Pekal, Schulte, & Brand, 2013; Laier, Pekal, & Brand, 2014). However, despite the importance of the topic, it has been little studied (Brand et al., 2011). In particular, little is known about the possible psychological determinants of compulsive cybersex use (Franc et al., 2018).

Negative emotions combined with negative urgency (the tendency to act impulsively in negative emotional situations) have been found to contribute to cybersex use (Wéry, Deleuze, Canale, & Billieux, 2018), probably as a way to cope with negative affect. Motives, especially those related to coping (i.e., to escape from real-life problems), are known to influence substance use (Benschop et al., 2015), behavioral addictions (Billieux et al., 2011; Király et al., 2015; Zanetta Dauriat et al., 2011), and compulsive cybersex (Brand, Laier, & Young, 2014). Cybersex, with or without masturbation (Putnam & Maheu, 2000; Wéry, Karila, Sutter, & Billieux, 2014), is used by some people as a coping strategy to manage negative emotions (Barrault, Hegbe, Bertsch, & Courtois, 2016; Southern, 2008). Enhancement and social motives (Franc et al., 2018) may also play a role in compulsive cybersex. Studies on Internet dating have underlined the role of social motives (Sumter, Vandenbosch, & Ligtenberg, 2017) and the expectation of arousal when watching porn (Young, 2008).

Sexual desire is a driving component of sexual fantasy life and activity (Levine, 2003; Pfaus, 2009). Moderate associations have been found between sexual desire and cybersex enhancement motives (Commission of the European Communities, 2002; Franc et al., 2018; Mark, Toland, Rosenkrantz, Brown, & Hong, 2018; Spector, Carey, & Steinberg, 1996), congruent with the enhancing and arousing effects of cybersex (Beutel et al., 2017; Reid, Li, Gilliland, Stein, & Fong, 2011). A recent study found weaker correlations between cybersex coping motives and sexual desire, and no association between solitary sexual desire and cybersex social motives (Franc et al., 2018).

The aim of this study was therefore to assess the influence of cybersex-related motives and sexual desire on compulsive cybersex use in men and women.

Procedure

The study was conducted online by posting ads on sexually related sites and forums with the agreement of the webmasters. It was aimed at anyone aged 18 years or over participating in online sexual activity. No payment was made for participation in the study.

Participants

Following the recruitment procedure, 761 people clicked on the link and 605 agreed to participate in the study. The completion rate progressively decreased, and 358 of the 605 subjects who began the questionnaire continued past the demographic data section. After missing values were removed, the final sample consisted of 306 subjects, i.e., 150 men (49%) and 156 women (51%). The age range was 18–69 years, average 32.63 (±10.83) years. The average age of males was 33.44 (±11.84) years and females 31.86 (±9.73) years. Participants were mainly from Switzerland (68%), France (25%), Belgium (2%), Canada (1%), USA (1%), and other countries. About 73% of participants were in a relationship. People with a heterosexual orientation represented about 84% of the sample, those with a bisexual orientation about 12%, and about 4% stated that they were homosexual.

Measures

The first part of the questionnaire included questions that explored the participants’ sociodemographic characteristics. The remainder of the questionnaire included three tools: (a) the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS), (b) the Cybersex Motives Questionnaire (CMQ), and (c) the Sexual Desire Inventory-2 (SDI-2).

The CIUS was designed to evaluate addictive Internet use (Meerkerk, Van Den Eijnden, Vermulst, & Garretsen, 2009) and has been validated as such. It consists of 14 items with a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 56 (very often). The CIUS has constantly been found to have a unified structure in various studies and samples (Khazaal et al., 2011, 2012; Meerkerk et al., 2009; Wartberg, Petersen, Kammerl, Rosenkranz, & Thomasius, 2014). High scores indicate respondents’ inability to restrict their use of the Internet. As reported in other studies assessing Internet-related addictive behaviors (Khazaal et al., 2015), the CIUS has been adapted to assess compulsive cybersex use.

To ensure that the CIUS targeted only cybersex behaviors, we drew the participants’ attention to the fact that the word Internet in the scale referred solely to cybersex. The CIUS and other instruments designed to assess Internet addiction have been successfully adapted to study gaming and gambling (Khazaal et al., 2015), and cybersex (Downing, Antebi, & Schrimshaw, 2014; Varfi et al., 2019; Wéry et al., 2018), without modifying their psychometric properties.

The CIUS items target the typical symptoms of compulsive Internet use mentioned in the literature, such as loss of control, preoccupation, withdrawal symptoms, coping, and mood changes. It has good stability with good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .90; Meerkerk et al., 2009).

The CMQ identifies the motives associated with the use of sexual sites (Franc et al., 2018). It comprises 14 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale from never to always or almost always, structured around three factors: Enhancement, Social, and Coping. The enhancement motive corresponds to the excitement and enjoyment of sexual activity online (i.e., “Because it is exciting” and “Because I like the feeling”). The social motive refers to the affiliation that can be felt by the user through the virtual world that encourages and accepts one’s deepest fantasies (i.e., “To be sociable and liked by others” and “To meet somebody”). The third motive refers to coping strategies that reflect the use of cybersex as a way of escaping from reality and disengaging from real-life concerns (i.e., “It comforts me when I’m in a bad mood” and “In order to forget my problems or worries”). The CMQ has satisfactory psychometric qualities. Cronbach’s α is .84 for the first factor, .73 for the second factor, and .79 for the third factor (Franc et al., 2018), which is acceptable.

The SDI-2 is used to assess sexual desire, defined as interest in sexual behavior (Spector et al., 1996). It is one of the most frequently used instruments for the assessment of sexual desire (Mark et al., 2018). It consists of 14 items measuring the intensity and importance of desire on a 9-point Likert scale ranging from no desire to strong desire or from not at all important to extremely important. Items measuring the frequency of sexual activities are rated on an 8-point Likert scale ranging from never to more than once a day. The items can be summed to produce an overall score or to produce scores for its two component dimensions: (a) dyadic sexual desire (desire to engage in sexual activity with a partner) and (b) solitary sexual desire (desire to engage in solo sexual activity). Dyadic sexual desire corresponds to Items 1–9, with a total score ranging from 0 to 62, and solitary sexual desire to the sum of Items 10–13, with a total score ranging from 0 to 23. Item 14 assesses to what extent participants can do without sexual activity and does not belong to either the solitary or the dyadic desire dimension. Solitary desire concerns the rate of solitary sexual behavior and dyadic desire concerns the frequency of sexual behaviors with a companion (Spector et al., 1996). The SDI-2 has good psychometric qualities with good internal consistency of the two factors (.86 and .96, respectively; Spector et al., 1996). Previous studies reported stable psychometric characteristics across samples with different languages and sexual orientations, such as lesbians and gay males (Dosch, Rochat, Ghisletta, Favez, & Van der Linden, 2016; Mark et al., 2018).

Statistical analysis

After a descriptive analysis, we used Student’s t-test to provide gender comparisons and Pearson’s correlation analyses to assess links between variables. Where data were missing, scores were replaced by the average of the scores obtained by the individual for the items of the subscale, or for the total score if the questionnaire had no subscale (person-mean imputation). Respondents with a rate of missing responses exceeding 10% were excluded.

Structural equation modeling (SEM) was performed using the maximum likelihood estimation. Fits were considered to be good if the values of the comparative fit index (CFI) were close to .90, the χ2/df ratio close to 2, and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.08 (Arbuckle & Wothke, 2003; Byrne, 2010; Hu & Bentler, 1999). Statistical analyses were performed using software TIBCO Statistica™ 13.3.0 (TIBCO Software Inc., Palo Alto, CA, USA) and IBM® SPSS® Amos™ 23.00 (IBM SPSS Software Inc., Wexford, PA, USA).

Ethics

The study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The ethical committee of the Geneva University Hospitals approved the study protocol. Participants were given a full description of the study aims and methods online. They were then asked to give their informed consent online, which allowed them to answer the questionnaires anonymously via SurveyMonkey.

Descriptive results are presented in Table 1. All distributions can be considered normal. Table 1 also provides the Cronbach’s α coefficients used to test the reliability of the scales. These are considered satisfactory when the α coefficient is >.70, which was the case here. Table 2 shows the differences in means by gender. Men scored higher than women for cybersex enhancement motives, and also for dyadic and solitary sexual desire (with large effect sizes). When single participants were compared with those living with a partner, there was a significant difference for dyadic sexual desire (41.64 vs. 46.23, t = −2.73, p < .01, with a medium effect size). The comparison by sexual orientation did not reveal any significant differences, although there was a tendency for bisexual participants to report more sexual desire and to have higher CIUS scores.

 

Table

Table 1. Descriptive analyses of CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2

 

Table 1. Descriptive analyses of CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2

Mean [95% CI]MedianSDRangeSkewnessKurtosisd (K-S)Lilliefors (p)Cronbach’s α
CIUS19.54 [18.16–20.91]1912.200–510.19−0.920.08<.01.93
CMQ enhancement23.85 [23.04–24.66]2512.208–40−0.36−0.230.08<.01.88
CMQ social10.33 [9.91–10.74]1112.204–20−0.15−0.660.10<.01.72
CMQ coping12.70 [12.15–13.25]1312.205–250.06−0.850.08<.01.81
SDI-264.25 [61.96–66.54]6720.340–109−0.620.390.07<.01.91
SDI-2 dyadic44.97 [43.48–46.47]4712.200–70−0.881.070.08<.01.87
SDI-2 solitary15.60 [14.61–16.59]1712.200–31−0.32−0.890.10<.01.93

Note. CIUS: Compulsive Internet Use Scale; CMQ: Cybersex Motives Questionnaire; SDI-2: Sexual Desire Inventory-2; CI: confidence interval; SD: standard deviation; d (K-S): Kolmogorov–Smirnov test.

 

Table

Table 2. Comparisons of CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2 scores by gender

 

Table 2. Comparisons of CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2 scores by gender

MeasureMenWomenEffect sizetp
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)(Cohen’s d)
CIUS19.30 (11.18)19.76 (13.14)−0.04−0.33.740
CMQ enhancement26.25 (6.66)21.55 (7.01)0.656.01.001
CMQ social10.18 (3.47)10.47 (3.90)−0.08−0.67.510
CMQ coping12.82 (4.81)12.59 (4.93)0.050.40.690
SDI-271.89 (17.88)56.90 (19.90)0.746.92.001
SDI-2 dyadic48.35 (12.30)41.73 (13.37)0.504.51.001
SDI-2 solitary19.02 (7.64)12.31 (8.64)0.767.18.001

Note. CIUS: Compulsive Internet Use Scale; CMQ: Cybersex Motives Questionnaire; SDI-2: Sexual Desire Inventory-2; SD: standard deviation.

Because we found no difference in CIUS scores between men and women, we explored how they were distributed according to the intensity of cybersex use (low, moderate, and high). The ranking of all participants in the three groups (by tertiles) showed that women were mostly in the low and the high compulsive use groups, whereas men were mainly in the moderate-use group (Table 3).

 

Table

Table 3. Characteristics of the three groups of cybersex users with low, moderate, and high CIUS scores

 

Table 3. Characteristics of the three groups of cybersex users with low, moderate, and high CIUS scores

Low CIUS score (n = 105)Medium CIUS score (n = 102)High CIUS score (n = 99)
CIUS [mean (SD)]6.05 (3.84)19.48 (4.05)33.89 (5.28)
CIUS (range)0–1213–2627–51
Age [mean (SD)]32.68 (11.17)33.15 (11.90)32.06 (9.27)
In a relationship67.62% (n = 71)75.49% (n = 77)74.75% (n = 74)
Men46.67% (n = 49)56.87% (n = 58)43.43% (n = 43)
Women53.33% (n = 56)43.14% (n = 44)56.57% (n = 56)
Heterosexual orientation86.67% (n = 91)84.31% (n = 86)79.80% (n = 79)
Homosexual orientation3.81% (n = 4)6.86% (n = 7)3.03% (n = 3)
Bisexual orientation9.52% (n = 10)8.82% (n = 9)17.17% (n = 17)

Note. CIUS: Compulsive Internet Use Scale; SD: standard deviation.

Table 4 presents correlations between CIUS scores and CMQ and SDI-2 subscores. The results highlight significant and positive correlations with all of the subscales. The strongest relationships were observed between the CIUS score and scores for the CMQ coping motives (r = .52, p < .001) and the CMQ social motives (r = .39, p < .001), and to a lesser extent between the CIUS score and the enhancement motives score (r = .28, p < .001).

 

Table

Table 4. Correlations between CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2 scores

 

Table 4. Correlations between CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2 scores

CIUSCMQ enhancementCMQ socialCMQ copingSDI-2SDI-2 dyadicSDI-2 solitary
CIUS1.28***.39***.52***.16**.15**.14*
CMQ enhancement1.28***.55***.56***.44***.55***
CMQ social1.58***.16**.17**.10
CMQ coping1.22***.20***.19**
SDI-21.91***.79***
SDI-2 dyadic1.48**
SDI-2 solitary1

Note. CIUS: Compulsive Internet Use Scale; CMQ: Cybersex Motives Questionnaire; SDI-2: Sexual Desire Inventory-2.

*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Table 5 presents the correlations by gender. For women, there was a significant relationship between the CIUS score and scores for the CMQ coping motives (r = .51, p < .001) and the CMQ social motives (r = .49, p < .001), and to a lesser extent between the CIUS score and the CMQ enhancement motives score (r = .34, p < .001). Notably, no statistically significant correlations were found between the CIUS score and the sexual desire subscales.

 

Table

Table 5. Correlations between CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2 scores in men and women

 

Table 5. Correlations between CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2 scores in men and women

Men/womenCIUSCMQ enhancementCMQ socialCMQ copingSDI-2SDI-2 dyadicSDI-2 solitary
CIUS.26***.25***.54***.30***.25***.28***
CMQ enhancement.34***.08.47***.44***.29***.49***
CMQ social.49***.50***.41***.05.09−.03
CMQ coping.51***.65***.74***.15.09.18**
SDI-2.09.55***.29***.30***.91***.76***
SDI-2 dyadic.09.48***.27**.29***.91***.42***
SDI-2 solitary.07.48***.23***.21***.76***.44***

Note. The lower part of the diagonal refers to correlations for men and the upper part for women. CIUS: Compulsive Internet Use Scale; CMQ: Cybersex Motives Questionnaire; SDI-2: Sexual Desire Inventory-2.

**p < 0.01. ***p < .001.

For men, we observed a significant relationship between the CIUS score and the CMQ coping motives score (r = .54, p < .001), and also, albeit to a lesser extent, between the CIUS score and scores for the other CMQ motive subscales. In contrast to women, we found an association between the men’s CIUS scores and both solitary (r = .28, p < .001) and dyadic sexual desire (r = .25, p < .001).

Finally, we conducted SEM to investigate the relationships between the measured variables (CIUS, CMQ, and SDI-2) and their interdependences (Figure 1). The values of the fits are acceptable (χ2/df = 3.01, CFI = 0.80, and RMSEA = 0.08). We proceeded in a similar way for men and women separately (see Figures 2 and 3). The CFI value for men was low (0.74). Figure 1 shows the association between the CIUS scores and the CMQ coping motives and CMQ social motives. It also highlights the importance of the links between CMQ enhancement motives and SDI-2 sexual desire. For men, Figure 2 shows the association between the CMQ coping motives and the CIUS, with links to SDI-2 sexual desire. Figure 3 for women highlights the association of CMQ social and coping motives with the CIUS.

figure parent remove

Figure 1. Relationships between the compulsive use of cybersex (Compulsive Internet Use Scale score), cybersex motives (Cybersex Motives Questionnaire subscales), and sexual desire (Sexual Desire Inventory-2 subscales) for the whole sample (men and women). *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. χ2 = 2,295.60, df = 764, χ2/df = 3.01, p < .001, CFI = 0.80, and RMSEA = 0.08 [0.08–0.09]. For readability, only latent variables are presented

figure parent remove

Figure 2. Relationships between compulsive use of cybersex (Compulsive Internet Use Scale score), cybersex motives (Cybersex Motives Questionnaire subscales), and sexual desire (Sexual Desire Inventory-2 subscales) for men. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. χ2 = 1,617.37, df = 764, χ2/df = 2.12, p < .001, CFI = 0.74, and RMSEA = 0.09 [0.08–0.09]. For readability, only latent variables are presented

figure parent remove

Figure 3. Relationships between compulsive use of cybersex (Compulsive Internet Use Scale score), cybersex motives (Cybersex Motives Questionnaire subscales), and sexual desire (Sexual Desire Inventory-2 subscales) for women. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. χ2 = 1,650.29, df = 766, χ2/df = 2.15, p < .001, CFI = 0.80, and RMSEA = 0.09 [0.08–0.09]. For readability, only latent variables are presented

The results of the study did not reveal significant differences between men and women in the CIUS scores, but they did show that the women participants were mostly in either the low-use or the high-use group. The lack of gender difference is not consistent with previous work (e.g., Kafka, 2010). The presence of subgroups of women in the high-risk group is, however, consistent with other studies on behavioral addictions (Khazaal et al., 2017), showing that subsamples of women are possibly at increased risk of behavioral addictions.

Because we recruited participants through sex-related websites and forums, the study was possibly subject to a self-selection bias (Khazaal et al., 2014). Overinclusion of people with a high level of cybersex use cannot be excluded. Epidemiological conclusions thus cannot be drawn from the study. Nevertheless, the results highlight an association between a number of variables and the CIUS scores of men and women in this sample.

The results suggest the role of CMQ coping, in both genders, and to some extent social motives, especially for women, in compulsive cybersex. The CMQ enhancement motives showed a small association with the CIUS scores, especially for men.

Coping motives refer to escape strategies that the individual sets up in the face of aversive situations (e.g., Barrault et al., 2016; Miner, Coleman, Center, Ross, & Rosser, 2007). They may help people find transient relief during painful moments (Coleman-Kennedy & Pendley, 2002; Leiblum, 1997). The study results suggest that such motives are associated with compulsive cybersex in both men and women. This finding is consistent with other studies on substance use (Blevins, Banes, Stephens, Walker, & Roffman, 2016; Grazioli et al., 2018) and other behavioral addictions (Brand et al., 2014; Clarke et al., 2007; Khazaal et al., 2018), as well as with studies showing interactions between negative affect and impulsivity in compulsive cybersex (Wéry et al., 2018). In this study, the CMQ coping motives also had the strongest impact on the CIUS in both genders. At a clinical level, these results suggest the importance of interventions that focus on emotional regulation to help people with compulsive cybersex. Further studies may benefit from including coping skills and cognitive expectancies as possible mediators between the compulsive use of cybersex and coping motives (Brand et al., 2014; Laier, Wegmann, & Brand, 2018).

As in other studies on behavioral addictions (Müller et al., 2017) and addictive cybersex (Weinstein, Zolek, Babkin, Cohen, & Lejoyeux, 2015), the study showed some important gender differences. More specifically, social motives were more strongly associated with CIUS scores for women than for men. This is consistent with other studies showing the higher involvement of women in social networks (Dufour et al., 2016). In addition, sexual desire was not found to be associated with CIUS scores in women, whereas an association, albeit small, was found for men.

Although the strongest association between the CIUS score and the other variables was most importantly shown in both genders for the CMQ coping motives, a smaller association was observed for sexual desire among men and for social motives among women. These findings are possibly due to dissimilarities between women and men in sexual desire (Carvalho & Nobre, 2011). The finding that sexual desire was only a factor in the male participants’ compulsive use of cybersex may be linked to gender differences in the way relational and social dimensions of sexuality act on sexual desire (Carvalho & Nobre, 2011).

In addition to such gender differences, our results suggest that sexual desire plays only a small role (in men), or even no role (in women) in compulsive cybersex use. Furthermore, the CMQ enhancement subscale does not seem to contribute to the CIUS score. This suggests that cybersex addiction is not driven by sex or only to a small extent in men. This finding is consistent with other studies showing that liking sexually explicit videos (Voon et al., 2014) and sexual activities (i.e., number of sexual contacts, satisfaction with sexual contacts, and use of interactive cybersex) is not associated with compulsive cybersex (Laier et al., 2014; Laier, Pekal, & Brand, 2015). As suggested in other studies on addictive behaviors, the “liking” dimension (hedonic drive) seems to play a smaller role than the “wanting” (incentive salience) and “learning” dimensions (predictive associations and cognitions, e.g., learning about negative emotion relief when using cybersex; Berridge, Robinson, & Aldridge, 2009; Robinson & Berridge, 2008).

At first glance, the small role of sexual desire and enhancement motives in compulsive cybersex seems counterintuitive. It appears that the sexual nature of the gratification is not a major drive of the behavior. This observation could be explained by the fact that the CIUS is not a measure of sexual activity or of cybersex use, but an assessment of compulsive cybersex use. The findings are consistent with the process related to maintenance of addictive behaviors. It has been postulated that addictions are maintained by a shift from gratification (i.e., looking for direct sexual rewards) to compensation (i.e., looking for escape from negative moods; Young & Brand, 2017). To further investigate this question, future studies should include concomitant assessments of cybersex use, sexual behaviors, and compulsive cybersex, together with the nature of the rewards obtained during cybersex use. Ecological momentary assessment could be used to explore these questions (Benarous et al., 2016; Ferreri, Bourla, Mouchabac, & Karila, 2018; Jones, Tiplady, Houben, Nederkoorn, & Field, 2018).

This work has several limitations, mainly related to the cross-sectional design, use of self-assessment questionnaires, self-selection biases, and the convenience sample size. The results should be confirmed by future studies, possibly based on the present results, including power analyses and sample size planning as well as detailed assessments of cybersex use (i.e., porn, dating, webcam, and chat) or focusing on a specific activity. The CIUS adapted for cybersex in this study showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = .93). It is a measure of compulsive sexuality, but not an assessment of cybersex use itself, and specific sexual activities were not reported. Further studies should include a description of such activities, perceived rewards linked to cybersex use, as well as an assessment of non-Internet based sexual involvement (i.e., sexual intercourse, etc.) and emotional relationship with partner.

Further psychological variables, such as self-esteem, mood (Park, Hong, Park, Ha, & Yoo, 2012), impulsivity (Wéry et al., 2018), loneliness (Khazaal et al., 2017; Yong, Inoue, & Kawakami, 2017), attachment (Favez, Tissot, Ghisletta, Golay, & Cairo Notari, 2016), and psychiatric comorbidities (Starcevic & Khazaal, 2017), may play an important role in compulsive cybersex, in addition to emotional and sexual satisfaction in real life.

YK, FB-D, and SR contributed in study concept and design. FBB, RC, SR, and YK contributed in statistical analysis and interpretation of data. YK contributed in recruitment. FBB, SR, FB-D, RC, and YK contributed in drafting of the manuscript.

None.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Barbara Every, ELS, of BioMedical Editor, and Elizabeth Yates for English language editing. They would also like to thank the study participants.

Albright, J. M. (2008). Sex in America online: An exploration of sex, marital status, and sexual identity in Internet sex seeking and its impacts. The Journal of Sex Research, 45(2), 175186. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490801987481 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Allen, A., Kannis-Dymand, L., & Katsikitis, M. (2017). Problematic Internet pornography use: The role of craving, desire thinking, and metacognition. Addictive Behaviors, 70, 6571. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.02.001 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Arbuckle, J. L., & Wothke, W. (2003). AMOS (version 5.0) [Computer software]. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters Corporation. Google Scholar
Barrault, S., Hegbe, K., Bertsch, I., & Courtois, R. (2016). Relation entre les événements de vie traumatiques de l’enfance, le trouble de personnalité borderline et les conduites cybersexuelles problématiques [Relationship between traumatic life events of childhood, borderline personality disorder and problematic cybersexual behavior]. Psychotropes, 22(3), 6581. doi:https://doi.org/10.3917/psyt.223.006510.3917/psyt.223.0065 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Benarous, X., Edel, Y., Consoli, A., Brunelle, J., Etter, J.-F., Cohen, D., & Khazaal, Y. (2016). Ecological momentary assessment and smartphone application intervention in adolescents with substance use and comorbid severe psychiatric disorders: Study protocol. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7, 157. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00157 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Benschop, A., Liebregts, N., van der Pol, P., Schaap, R., Buisman, R., van Laar, M., van den Brink, W., de Graaf, R., & Korf, D. J. (2015). Reliability and validity of the Marijuana Motives Measure among young adult frequent cannabis users and associations with cannabis dependence. Addictive Behaviors, 40, 9195. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.09.003 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E., & Aldridge, J. W. (2009). Dissecting components of reward: ‘Liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 9(1), 6573. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Beutel, M. E., Giralt, S., Wölfling, K., Stöbel-Richter, Y., Subic-Wrana, C., Reiner, I., Tibubos, A. N., & Brähler, E. (2017). Prevalence and determinants of online-sex use in the German population. PLoS One, 12(6), e0176449. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0176449 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Billieux, J., Chanal, J., Khazaal, Y., Rochat, L., Gay, P., Zullino, D., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Psychological predictors of problematic involvement in massively multiplayer online role-playing games: Illustration in a sample of male cybercafé players. Psychopathology, 44(3), 165171. doi:https://doi.org/10.1159/000322525 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Blevins, C. E., Banes, K. E., Stephens, R. S., Walker, D. D., & Roffman, R. A. (2016). Motives for marijuana use among heavy-using high school students: An analysis of structure and utility of the Comprehensive Marijuana Motives Questionnaire. Addictive Behaviors, 57, 4247. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.02.005 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Brand, M., Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., Schächtle, U., Schöler, T., & Altstötter-Gleich, C. (2011). Watching pornographic pictures on the Internet: Role of sexual arousal ratings and psychological–psychiatric symptoms for using Internet sex sites excessively. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(6), 371377. doi:https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0222 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Brand, M., Laier, C., & Young, K. S. (2014). Internet addiction: Coping styles, expectancies, and treatment implications. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1256. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01256 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with Amos: Basic concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Routledge. Google Scholar
Carvalho, J., & Nobre, P. (2011). Gender differences in sexual desire: How do emotional and relationship factors determine sexual desire according to gender? Sexologies, 20(4), 207211. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sexol.2011.08.010 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, D., Tse, S., Abbott, M. W., Townsend, S., Kingi, P., & Manaia, W. (2007). Reasons for starting and continuing gambling in a mixed ethnic community sample of pathological and non-problem gamblers. International Gambling Studies, 7(3), 299313. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/14459790701601455 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Coleman-Kennedy, C., & Pendley, A. (2002). Assessment and diagnosis of sexual addiction. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association, 8(5), 143151. doi:https://doi.org/10.1067/mpn.2002.128827 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Commission of the European Communities. (2002). eEurope 2002: Quality criteria for health related websites. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 4(3), E15. doi:https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.4.3.e15 MedlineGoogle Scholar
Döring, N. M. (2009). The Internet’s impact on sexuality: A critical review of 15 years of research. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(5), 10891101. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2009.04.003 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Dosch, A., Rochat, L., Ghisletta, P., Favez, N., & Van der Linden, M. (2016). Psychological factors involved in sexual desire, sexual activity, and sexual satisfaction: A multi-factorial perspective. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45(8), 20292045. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0467-z Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Downing, M. J., Antebi, N., & Schrimshaw, E. W. (2014). Compulsive use of Internet-based sexually explicit media: Adaptation and validation of the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS). Addictive Behaviors, 39(6), 11261130. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.03.007 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Dufour, M., Brunelle, N., Tremblay, J., Leclerc, D., Cousineau, M.-M., Khazaal, Y., Légaré, A. A., Rousseau, M., & Berbiche, D. (2016). Gender difference in Internet use and Internet problems among Quebec high school students. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 61(10), 663668. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743716640755 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Favez, N., Tissot, H., Ghisletta, P., Golay, P., & Cairo Notari, S. (2016). Validation of the French version of the Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) Adult Romantic Attachment Questionnaire. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 75(3), 113121. doi:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000177 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Ferreri, F., Bourla, A., Mouchabac, S., & Karila, L. (2018). e-Addictology: An overview of new technologies for assessing and intervening in addictive behaviors. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 51. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00051 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Franc, E., Khazaal, Y., Jasiowka, K., Lepers, T., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., & Rothen, S. (2018). Factor structure of the Cybersex Motives Questionnaire. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 7(3), 601609. doi:https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.67 LinkGoogle Scholar
Frangos, C. C., Frangos, C. C., & Sotiropoulos, I. (2010). Problematic Internet use among Greek university students: An ordinal logistic regression with risk factors of negative psychological beliefs, pornographic sites, and online games. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(1–2), 5158. doi:https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0306 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Grazioli, V. S., Bagge, C. L., Studer, J., Bertholet, N., Rougemont-Bücking, A., Mohler-Kuo, M., Daeppen, J. B., & Gmel, G. (2018). Depressive symptoms, alcohol use and coping drinking motives: Examining various pathways to suicide attempts among young men. Journal of Affective Disorders, 232, 243251. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.02.028 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Grov, C., Gillespie, B. J., Royce, T., & Lever, J. (2011). Perceived consequences of casual online sexual activities on heterosexual relationships: A U. S. online survey. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(2), 429439. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-010-9598-z Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Grubbs, J. B., Volk, F., Exline, J. J., & Pargament, K. I. (2015). Internet pornography use: Perceived addiction, psychological distress, and the validation of a brief measure. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 41(1), 83106. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X.2013.842192 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Jones, A., Tiplady, B., Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., & Field, M. (2018). Do daily fluctuations in inhibitory control predict alcohol consumption? An ecological momentary assessment study. Psychopharmacology, 235(5), 14871496. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-018-4860-5 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Kafka, M. P. (2010). Hypersexual disorder: A proposed diagnosis for DSM-V. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 39(2), 377400. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-009-9574-7 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Karila, L., Wéry, A., Weinstein, A., Cottencin, O., Petit, A., Reynaud, M., & Billieux, J. (2014). Sexual addiction or hypersexual disorder: Different terms for the same problem? A review of the literature. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 40124020. doi:https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990619 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Khazaal, Y., Achab, S., Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Zullino, D., Dufour, M., & Rothen, S. (2015). Factor structure of the Internet Addiction Test in online gamers and poker players. JMIR Mental Health, 2(2), e12. doi:https://doi.org/10.2196/mental.3805 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Khazaal, Y., Breivik, K., Billieux, J., Zullino, D., Thorens, G., Achab, S., Gmel, G., & Chatton, A. (2018). Game Addiction Scale assessment through a nationally representative sample of young adult men: Item response theory graded-response modeling. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 20(8), e10058. doi:https://doi.org/10.2196/10058 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Khazaal, Y., Chatton, A., Achab, S., Monney, G., Thorens, G., Dufour, M., Zullino, D., & Rothen, S. (2017). Internet gamblers differ on social variables: A latent class analysis. Journal of Gambling Studies, 33(3), 881897. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-016-9664-0 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Khazaal, Y., Chatton, A., Atwi, K., Zullino, D., Khan, R., & Billieux, J. (2011). Arabic validation of the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS). Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 6(1), 32. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/1747-597X-6-32 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Khazaal, Y., Chatton, A., Horn, A., Achab, S., Thorens, G., Zullino, D., & Billieux, J. (2012). French validation of the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS). Psychiatric Quarterly, 83(4), 397405. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s11126-012-9210-x Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Khazaal, Y., van Singer, M., Chatton, A., Achab, S., Zullino, D., Rothen, S., Khan, R., Billieux, J., & Thorens, G. (2014). Does self-selection affect samples’ representativeness in online surveys? An investigation in online video game research. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 16(7), e164. doi:https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.2759 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Király, O., Urbán, R., Griffiths, M. D., Ágoston, C., Nagygyörgy, K., Kökönyei, G., & Demetrovics, Z. (2015). The mediating effect of gaming motivation between psychiatric symptoms and problematic online gaming: An online survey. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 17(4), e88. doi:https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.3515 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Laier, C., & Brand, M. (2014). Empirical evidence and theoretical considerations on factors contributing to cybersex addiction from a cognitive-behavioral view. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 21(4), 305321. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2014.970722 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Laier, C., Pawlikowski, M., Pekal, J., Schulte, F. P., & Brand, M. (2013). Cybersex addiction: Experienced sexual arousal when watching pornography and not real-life sexual contacts makes the difference. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 2(2), 100107. doi:https://doi.org/10.1556/JBA.2.2013.002 LinkGoogle Scholar
Laier, C., Pekal, J., & Brand, M. (2014). Cybersex addiction in heterosexual female users of Internet pornography can be explained by gratification hypothesis. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(8), 505511. doi:https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2013.0396 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Laier, C., Pekal, J., & Brand, M. (2015). Sexual excitability and dysfunctional coping determine cybersex addiction in homosexual males. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18(10), 575580. doi:https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2015.0152 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Laier, C., Wegmann, E., & Brand, M. (2018). Personality and cognition in gamers: Avoidance expectancies mediate the relationship between maladaptive personality traits and symptoms of Internet-gaming disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 304304. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00304 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Leiblum, S. R. (1997). Sex and the net: Clinical implications. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 22(1), 2127. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/01614576.1997.11074167 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Levine, S. B. (2003). The nature of sexual desire: A clinician’s perspective. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32(3), 279285. doi:https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023421819465 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Mark, K. P., Toland, M. D., Rosenkrantz, D. E., Brown, H. M., & Hong, S.-H. (2018). Validation of the Sexual Desire Inventory for lesbian, gay, bisexual, trans, and queer adults. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 5(1), 122128. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000260 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Meerkerk, G. J., Van Den Eijnden, R. J. J. M., Vermulst, A. A., & Garretsen, H. F. L. (2009). The Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS): Some psychometric properties. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12(1), 16. doi:https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0181 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Miner, M. H., Coleman, E., Center, B. A., Ross, M., & Rosser, B. R. S. (2007). The Compulsive Sexual Behavior Inventory: Psychometric properties. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(4), 579587. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9127-2 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Müller, M., Brand, M., Mies, J., Lachmann, B., Sariyska, R. Y., & Montag, C. (2017). The 2D:4D marker and different forms of Internet use disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 213. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00213 Google Scholar
Park, S., Hong, K.-E. M., Park, E. J., Ha, K. S., & Yoo, H. J. (2012). The association between problematic Internet use and depression, suicidal ideation and bipolar disorder symptoms in Korean adolescents. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 47(2), 153159. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867412463613 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Pfaus, J. G. (2009). Reviews: Pathways of sexual desire. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 6(6), 15061533. doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1743-6109.2009.01309.x Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Philaretou, A. G., Mahfouz, A. Y., & Allen, K. R. (2005). Use of Internet pornography and men’s well-being. International Journal of Men’s Health, 4(2), 149169. doi:https://doi.org/10.3149/jmh.0402.149 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, D. E., & Maheu, M. M. (2000). Online sexual addiction and compulsivity: Integrating web resources and behavioral telehealth in treatment. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 7(1–2), 91112. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/10720160008400209 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Reid, R. C., Li, D. S., Gilliland, R., Stein, J. A., & Fong, T. (2011). Reliability, validity, and psychometric development of the Pornography Consumption Inventory in a sample of hypersexual men. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 37(5), 359385. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/0092623X.2011.607047 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Robinson, T. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: Some current issues. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1507), 31373146. doi:https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0093 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Southern, S. (2008). Treatment of compulsive cybersex behavior. Psychiatric Clinics, 31(4), 697712. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2008.06.003 Google Scholar
Spector, I. P., Carey, M. P., & Steinberg, L. (1996). The Sexual Desire Inventory: Development, factor structure, and evidence of reliability. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 22(3), 175190. doi:https://doi.org/10.1080/00926239608414655 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Starcevic, V., & Khazaal, Y. (2017). Relationships between behavioural addictions and psychiatric disorders: What is known and what is yet to be learned? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 53. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00053 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Sumter, S. R., Vandenbosch, L., & Ligtenberg, L. (2017). Love me Tinder: Untangling emerging adults’ motivations for using the dating application Tinder. Telematics and Informatics, 34(1), 6778. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tele.2016.04.009 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Varfi, N., Rothen, S., Jasiowka, K., Lepers, T., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., & Khazaal, Y. (2019). Sexual desire, mood, attachment style, impulsivity, and self-esteem as predictive factors for addictive cybersex. JMIR Mental Health, 6(1), e9978. doi:https://doi.org/10.2196/mental.9978 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Voon, V., Mole, T. B., Banca, P., Porter, L., Morris, L., Mitchell, S., Lapa, T. R., Karr, J., Harrison, N. A., Potenza, M. N., & Irvine, M. (2014). Neural correlates of sexual cue reactivity in individuals with and without compulsive sexual behaviours. PLoS One, 9(7), e102419. doi:https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102419 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Wartberg, L., Petersen, K.-U., Kammerl, R., Rosenkranz, M., & Thomasius, R. (2014). Psychometric validation of a German version of the Compulsive Internet Use Scale. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17(2), 99103. doi:https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0689 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, A. M., Zolek, R., Babkin, A., Cohen, K., & Lejoyeux, M. (2015). Factors predicting cybersex use and difficulties in forming intimate relationships among male and female users of cybersex. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 54. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00054 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Wéry, A., & Billieux, J. (2017). Problematic cybersex: Conceptualization, assessment, and treatment. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 238246. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.007 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Wéry, A., Deleuze, J., Canale, N., & Billieux, J. (2018). Emotionally laden impulsivity interacts with affect in predicting addictive use of online sexual activity in men. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 80, 192201. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.10.004 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Wéry, A., Karila, L., Sutter, P. D., & Billieux, J. (2014). Conceptualisation, évaluation et traitement de la dépendance cybersexuelle: Une revue de la littérature [Conceptualization, assessment, and treatment of cybersexual addiction: A review of the literature]. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 55(4), 266281. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038103 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Yong, R. K. F., Inoue, A., & Kawakami, N. (2017). The validity and psychometric properties of the Japanese version of the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS). BMC Psychiatry, 17(1), 201. doi:https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1364-5 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Young, K. S. (2008). Internet sex addiction: Risk factors, stages of development, and treatment. American Behavioral Scientist, 52(1), 2137. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764208321339 CrossrefGoogle Scholar
Young, K. S., & Brand, M. (2017). Merging theoretical models and therapy approaches in the context of Internet gaming disorder: A personal perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1853. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01853 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar
Zanetta Dauriat, F., Zermatten, A., Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Bondolfi, G., Zullino, D., & Khazaal, Y. (2011). Motivations to play specifically predict excessive involvement in massively multiplayer online role-playing games: Evidence from an online survey. European Addiction Research, 17(4), 185189. doi:https://doi.org/10.1159/000326070 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar