Dopamine cross-sensitization between psychostimulant drugs and stress in healthy male volunteers (2016)

Citation: Translational Psychiatry (2016) 6, e740; doi:10.1038/tp.2016.6

Published online 23 February 2016

L Booij1,2,3,10, K Welfeld3,10, M Leyton3,4,5, A Dagher5, I Boileau6, I Sibon7, G B Baker8, M Diksic5, J-P Soucy5, J C Pruessner9, E Cawley-Fiset3, K F Casey2 and C Benkelfat3,5

  1. 1Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  2. 2CHU Sainte Justine Hospital Research Center, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  3. 3Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  4. 4Center for Studies in Behavioral Neurobiology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  5. 5McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  6. 6Center for Addiction and Mental Health, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  7. 7Pole de Neurosciences Cliniques, Hôpital Pellegrin, CHU Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
  8. 8Neurobiology Research Unit, Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
  9. 9Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada

Correspondence: Dr C Benkelfat, Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, 1033 Avenue des Pins West, Montreal, QC, Canada H3A1A1. E-mail: [email protected]

10These authors contributed equally to this work.

Received 15 September 2015; Revised 17 November 2015; Accepted 23 November 2015

Top of page

Abstract

Dysregulation of the stress response system is a potential etiological factor in the development of and relapse to multiple neuropsychiatric disorders. Previously we reported that repeated intermittent d-amphetamine administration can lead to progressively greater dopamine release, thereby providing evidence of drug-induced neurochemical sensitization. Here, we test the hypothesis that repeated exposure to d-amphetamine increases dopaminergic responses to stress; that is, produces cross-sensitization. Using positron emission tomography, we measured in 17 healthy male volunteers (mean±s.d.=22.1±3.4 years) [11C]raclopride binding responses to a validated psychosocial stress task before and 2 weeks after a regimen of repeated d-amphetamine (3 × 0.3 mg kg−1, by mouth; n=8) or placebo (3 × lactose, by mouth; n=9). Mood and physiological measurements were recorded throughout each session. Before the d-amphetamine regimen, exposure to the stress task increased behavioral and physiological indices of stress (anxiety, heart rate, cortisol, all Pless than or equal to0.05). Following the d-amphetamine regimen, the stress-induced cortisol responses were augmented (P<0.04), and voxel-based analyses showed larger stress-induced decreases in [11C]raclopride non-displaceable binding potential across the striatum. In the placebo group, re-exposure to stress led to smaller clusters of decreased [11C]raclopride binding, primarily in the sensorimotor striatum (P<0.05). Together, this study provides evidence for drug × stress cross-sensitization; moreover, random exposure to stimulants and/or stress cumulatively, while enhancing dopamine release in striatal areas, may contribute to a lowered set point for psychopathologies in which altered dopamine neurotransmission is invoked.

Top of page 

Introduction

Stress is a key contributing factor in the development and exacerbation of chronic relapsing neuropsychiatric disorders, including addiction and psychosis. One potentially involved process is ‘sensitization’; that is, following repeated exposure to stressors and/or psychostimulant drugs, some effects can become progressively greater.1, 2, 3 In susceptible individuals, these enhanced responses have been proposed to influence illness onset and relapse. 4, 5, 6, 7

In animals, ‘sensitization’ to psychostimulants is subject to cross-sensitization with stress.8, 9 For instance, in rodents, repeated exposure to psychostimulants increases the ability of stressors to precipitate motor activity, drug self-administration and dopamine (DA) release.10, 11 Conversely, exposure to experimental stress can increase the behavioral and DA response to psychostimulants.3, 12, 13, 14 Although the neurobiological substrate mediating cross-sensitization between stress and psychostimulants is not fully understood, there is evidence that it includes the interaction between the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and DA projections, in particular those arising from the mesencephalon.15 A number of studies showed that both stress and d-amphetamine activate the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, resulting in increased cortisol levels.16 Glucocorticoids, in turn, may facilitate DA release through several mechanisms, including affecting tyrosine hydroxylase, monoamine oxidase-A and DA reuptake.16 Conceivably, this could lead to a greater DA response on stress exposure. Indeed, removal of the major source of endogenous glucocorticoids, through surgery or pharmacological blockade (metyrapone), diminishes the development of drug-induced DA sensitization.17

We have previously reported a persistent increase in DA release on stimulant re-exposure in healthy humans who had undergone a subchronic regimen of d-amphetamine (three doses within 1 week) when tested at least 2 weeks following the last stimulant dose, an observation interpreted as evidence of neurochemical sensitization.18 The present study follows up on this initial observation18 to test the hypothesis that the same d-amphetamine regimen would lead to a greater DA response to a psychosocial stressor administered 2 weeks following the last stimulant dose; that is, evidence of cross-sensitization.

Top of page 

Materials and methods

Participants

Healthy males were recruited through on-line advertisements in the university network and local newspapers. Following a telephone interview to assess initial eligibility, participants underwent a full in-lab screening including: (1) a semi-structured psychiatric interview (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV: Patient Edition, SCID-NP),19 (2) a complete physical examination including laboratory testing and an electrocardiogram and (3) measures of self-esteem and trait anxiety, including a questionnaire of competence and control,20 the Rosenberg self-esteem scale21 and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory.22 Main exclusion criteria included: (1) major medical /neurological illness or the use of medication likely to affect brain function or confound positron emission tomography (PET) results; (2) a personal or family history of Axis I disorders; (3) regular past or present drug use (that is, exposure to stimulant drugs or hallucinogens/sedatives in the past 12 months); (4) lifetime use of stimulants, sedatives or hallucinogens exceeding four exposures total; (5) frequent use of tobacco (greater than or equal to5 cigarettes per day); (6) frequent use of cannabis (greter than two uses per week); (7) testing positive on a urine toxicology screen for illicit drug abuse on the days of study (Triage-TM); and (8) meeting other PET/magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) exclusion criteria (see Supplementary Information). The study was approved by the Montreal Neurological Institute Research Ethics Board. All the participants provided written informed consent.

 

Design overview

Eligible participants received either d-amphetamine (0.3 mg kg−1, by mouth) or placebo on three separate days, every 48 h, in the same environment (on the PET gantry), following similar procedures and assessments as in our initial study.18 All the participants underwent three 60-minute PET [11C]raclopride scan (~7 mCi) sessions, during which they were exposed to the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST; Figure 1). One PET [11C]raclopride scan was conducted with the control task (MIST control), whereas the two other PET [11C]raclopride scans were obtained with the MIST stress task to assess DA responses to stress before (MIST 1) and 14 days after the last drug (d-amphetamine or placebo) dose (MIST 2). All the participants underwent an anatomical high-resolution T1-weighted MRI scan for the purpose of PET registration. To minimize the influence of habituation to the MIST, participants performed the MIST task (pre MIST) once before the first PET session, as habituation of the stress response tends to be strongest between the first and second exposure to the same stress task.23, 24 Participants were asked to fast and abstain from caffeine and tobacco for a minimum of 4 h before each session. All seven sessions took place over a period of ~21 days, as described in Figure 1 (see Supplementary Information). They were instructed not to use any drugs throughout the entire study period. This was confirmed by a negative urine drug test at the beginning of each session.

 
Figure 1.

Figure 1 - Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative text for this. If you require assistance to access this image, please contact help@nature.com or the author

Experimental design of the study. PreMIST=practice session, before first PET [11C]raclopride scan. MIST control=PET [11C]raclopride scan in association with the low stress control task. MIST 1=PET [11C]raclopride scan with the stressful MIST task before the d-amphetamine or placebo regimen. MIST 2=PET [11C]raclopride scan with the stressful MIST task 14 days after the last drug (d-amphetamine or placebo) dose. PET, positron emission tomography.

Full figure and legend (56K)

 

 

Experimental stress task

The MIST is a validated stress task based on the Trier Mental Challenge task25 and adapted for use in an imaging environment.26 We used three 12-min blocks, each with four 3-min segments. During the task, arithmetic tasks are presented in the scanner via a computer screen. Participants answered using a computer mouse. Task difficulty and time constraint for each calculation are adjusted automatically by the computer algorithm in real time depending on the participant’s performance, so as to be slightly beyond each individual’s capability. After each trial, the computer screen displays feedback about the participant’s performance (correct, incorrect, timeout); following each segment, a negative feedback is provided in two complementary ways: by the program and by a confederate. Participants were led to believe that their performance was below expectations, and were asked to increase performance to meet requirements.

This task has been shown to elicit behavioral and hormonal responses to stress and has been associated with striatal DA release in healthy volunteers, including in its more ventral portion.26, 27 During the sensorimotor control condition (MIST control), participants performed simple arithmetic for 36 min, as described above, without time constraints, signs of visible progress, sound or negative feedback. Participants were debriefed at the end of the last PET stress session, and were told that the task was customized to be outside of their mental ability and was not meant to measure their arithmetic skills.

Subjective behavioral changes were assessed with the Profile of Mood States28 and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory,22 before and immediately after the end of each MIST exposure; as well as at the end of the test session when outside the scanner (data not shown). Blood samples for cortisol and heart rate measures (MP100-Biopac Systems) were collected at baseline and every 12 min throughout each session (Figure 1).

Voxel-wise parametric map and t-statistics

The PET images were corrected for between-frame motion artefacts29 and co-registered to each individual’s MRI. The MRI and PET images were linearly transformed into stereotactic space using the Montreal Neurological Institute-305 template.30 [11C]Raclopride non-displaceable binding potential (BPND=fNDBavail/KD) was estimated at each voxel, using a simplified reference tissue method, with the cerebellar cortex, excluding the vermis, as a reference region.31, 32 Voxel-wise t-maps comparing BPND during MIST 1 relative to MIST 2 were generated using residual paired t-tests with a threshold of t=3.76 equivalent to P=0.05 for a whole striatum search volume based on random field theory.33 An objective of this approach is to detect changes in BPND at the voxel level with no a priori anatomical hypothesis, hence circumventing some of the limitations of volume of interest (VOI) placement.27 Please see the Supplementary Information for a more detailed description on how the striatal search volume and the voxel-wise statistical threshold were defined.

 

VOI analysis

Three VOIs were selected bilaterally on each individual’s MRI, including limbic ventral striatum, associative striatum (pre-commissural dorsal putamen, pre-commissural dorsal caudate and the post-commissural caudate) and sensorimotor striatum (post-commissural putamen). VOI delineation into gross anatomical brain structures was first obtained by applying automatic segmentation procedures to each individual’s anatomical MRI.34 Each participant’s set of VOI was then manually refined.18 To align the VOI template on PET dynamic data and extract regional time activity curves, each individual’s dynamic radioactivity PET data were averaged along the time dimension and co-registered to the MRI.35 Estimates of average BPND within these VOIs were extracted in the three scanning conditions. A repeated-measures analysis of variance with experimental condition (MIST control, MIST 1, MIST 2) as within-subjects factor and one between-subjects factor subgroup (placebo, d-amphetamine) was conducted for each VOI, to investigate differences in BPND. Degrees of freedom were corrected using the Greenhouse–Geisser test in the case of nonsphericity, as determined by the Mauchly test (see also Supplementary Information).

 
Subjective mood and Psychophysiology

 

Outcome measures were analyzed using repeated-measures analysis of variance. Subgroup (placebo vs d-amphetamine) was the between-subjects factor. Within-subjects factors for the behavioral data were experimental condition (MIST control, MIST 1, MIST 2) and time (baseline, post-task). For HR and cortisol, area under the curves (computed as in ref. 36) for each experimental condition was the within-subjects factor.

 
Top of page 

Results

Eighteen healthy males took part in the study (d-amphetamine n=9; placebo n=9). One participant (d-amphetamine condition) displayed mean absolute changes in BPND (across regions) during exposure to MIST 1 (MIST 1 vs MIST control) three standard deviations above the sample mean (and five times higher than changes reported in response to ‘stress’ in a previous study using the MIST).27 These abnormal BPND values in this participant were almost certainly due to a technical error. This participant was therefore removed from the analysis. The participants in the placebo vs d-amphetamine subgroups (Table 1) did not significantly differ with respect to demographics or personality measurements, or in injected dose of [11C]raclopride in any of the three PET sessions (see Table 1). Although injected amount appeared lower after stress 2 relative to stress 1, this effect was independent of the type of drug (d-amphetamine or placebo; P=0.94).

 

PET study

Voxel-wise analyses

 

Effect of stress exposure before repeated damphetamine or placebo (MIST 1 vs MIST control). Stress exposure before the d-amphetamine regimen (MIST 1 vs MIST control) elicited variable but significant decreases in striatal BPND values, primarily in the putamen. The magnitude of change (% decrease and cluster size) was quite similar for both subgroups (Table 2).

 

 

Effect of stress exposure following repeated d-amphetamine or placebo (MIST 2 vs MIST 1)

 

Although stress exposure before the d-amphetamine sensitization regimen elicited only small clusters of significantly decreased [11C]raclopride BPND values (see above), stress-induced decreases in BPND following the sensitizing regimen were much more widespread (Figure 2; Table 2 and Table 3). These larger clusters of decreased BPND following re-exposure to stress were not observed after the placebo regimen.

 
Figure 2.

Figure 2 - Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative text for this. If you require assistance to access this image, please contact help@nature.com or the author

Voxel-wise t-maps of [11C]raclopride BPND changes during MIST who received the repeated d-amphetamine regimens (left, n=8) and placebo regimens (right, n=9), relative to the control condition. MIST 1–MIST 2=change in [11C]raclopride BPND during second exposure to stress relative to first stress exposure. A greater t-value reflects greater decreases in [11C]raclopride BPND (that is, greater dopamine release). BPND, non-displaceable binding potential; MIST, Montreal Imaging Stress Task.

Full figure and legend (181K)

 

 

VOI analyses

 

VOI analyses showed that prior exposure to the d-amphetamine regimen led to highly variable, but nonsignificant, changes in [11C]raclopride BPND, nor were there significant differences between MIST 2 and MIST 1 in the placebo group (Supplementary Figure S1). However, in the MIST 2 vs MIST control condition, secondary VOI analyses showed that, in the placebo group, significant decreases in BPND were observed in the right associative striatum (F(2,16)=4.44, P=0.03), left ventral striatum (F(2,16)=4.11, P=0.04) and right (F(2,16)=3.76, P=0.05) and left (F(2,16)=4.94, P=0.02) sensorimotor striatum.

 

Behavior and psychophysiology

Mood states

 

Relative to MIST control, the MIST 1 stress exposure resulted in an increased ‘anxiety’ response, as measured by the Profile of Mood States (experimental condition × time: F(2,30)=4.31, P=0.02; MIST 1 vs control: (1,15)=8.81; P=0.01) and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (experimental condition × time: F(2,30)=4.12, P=0.02; F(1,15)=8.41; P=0.01). These effects were not observed on MIST re-exposure at 21 days; nor did these effects differ between subgroups (d-amphetamine or placebo). There was no significant correlation between changes in [11C]raclopride BPND and the behavioral stress response. See also Supplementary Table S1.

 
Physiological measures

 

The MIST significantly increased heart rate during the first MIST PET scan and re-exposure at 21 days (main effect of experimental condition: F(2,30)=18.58, P<0.001; MIST 1 vs control: F(1,15)=19.66, P<0.001; MIST 2 vs control: F(1,15)=19.81; P<0.001), but there were no interactions with subgroup (amphetamine, placebo) or differences between MIST 1 vs MIST 2. Cortisol marginally increased during the MIST 1 exposure (F(1,15)=2.93; P=0.107) and more robustly at MIST re-exposure (at day 21; F(1,15)=18.88; P=0.001). The condition × subgroup interaction showed a trend towards significance (F(2,30)=3.15, P=0.057), with the cortisol response during re-exposure to the MIST greater following the d-amphetamine regimen (F(1,15)=5.20; P=0.038), relative to placebo. There were no significant correlations between changes in [11C]raclopride BPND and the psychophysiological or cortisol stress responses. Please see Supplementary Table S2.

 
Amphetamine levels

 

Consistent with our previous study,18 plasma amphetamine concentrations confirmed the presence of the stimulant drug in all the three sessions equally (see Supplementary Information for more details).

 
Top of page 

Discussion

The present study investigated whether a regimen of d-amphetamine exposure previously demonstrated to induce sensitization in human volunteers would lead to greater responses to psychosocial stress. The results of the present study provide preliminary evidence that it might. Consistent with the hypothesis, the stress-induced DA and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis responses were significantly greater 14 days after a repeated d-amphetamine regimen. These heightened responses appear to be in line with reports of cross-sensitization in laboratory animals.8, 11, 37

DA release in the nucleus accumbens has been well documented in experimental animals following exposure to stressful events, such as electric shock, tail pinch and bodily restraint.38, 39, 40 In humans, few studies have investigated dopaminergic responses to stress. The DA responses to psychosocial stress appear highly variable and are often limited to susceptible individuals (for example, individuals with low self-esteem, a history of low maternal care or those at risk for psychosis)26, 27, 41 The present study raises the possibility that these variable responses might reflect, in part, differential lifetime histories of stressful experiences.

Consistent with the main hypothesis here, it was found that, following repeated d-amphetamine, re-exposure to stress further decreased BPND values in the healthy participants. These findings are reminiscent of observations of altered [11C]-(+)-PHNO (a D2/D3 agonist ligand) binding responses in individuals with psychosis, using this laboratory stress paradigm.42 The present findings strengthen the view that repeated exposure to drugs, over and above other vulnerability factors (for example, genetic), might summate their specific effects to alter stress responses in striatal areas, and, possibly, the risk of DA-related disorders.

Changes in BPND were also observed following the placebo regimen. The reported changes in the d-amphetamine subgroup were regionally specific, occurring in the left ventral striatum and bilaterally in the posterior putamen. Repeated stress alone has been demonstrated to alter meso-corticolimbic DA release in animal models.43 In humans, previous stress exposure, particularly early-life stress, has been identified as one important factor for the development of psychiatric disorders later in life.44, 45 Although it is currently impossible to establish a direct causal relationship, it has been demonstrated that early-life stress is associated with increased ventral striatum DA release to subsequent stress26 as well as to psychostimulant exposure later in life.46 Our findings of decreased BPND (in the placebo subgroup) supports previous literature that repeated, uncontrolled stress exposure alone, can lead to sensitization.3 Re-exposure to stress following d-amphetamine (relative to placebo) may produce different effects in different striatal subregions.18

Although the results are in line with studies showing increased stress or amphetamine-induced DA responses in laboratory animals that had been previously exposed to repeated amphetamine,3 the results contrast with evidence of attenuated DA responses in patients with substance use disorders (relative to controls) following an acute challenge with methylphenidate or amphetamine47, 48, 49 or exposure to a laboratory stressor.41 The reasons for this discrepancy remain unclear, but could reflect pre-existing traits, protracted withdrawal effects in those with extensive histories of substance abuse or a shift from DA to other neurobiological substrates mediating heightened behavioral responses to various challenges.50, 51, 52 Together, these findings highlight the need to study systematically DA (cross) sensitization mechanisms in clinical samples with various levels of prior drug exposure to further understand the relevance of DA (cross)sensitization in the onset and relapse of drug dependence/abuse.

Strengths and limitations

This study benefitted from selecting a carefully screened, homogenous sample of males who were all monitored carefully for drug use and stressful experiences throughout the 30-day period of testing, thus minimizing potential confounds. It would be of interest, however, to determine whether the results could be generalized to other samples, including females, patients or following the exposure to chronic repeated stress. Longitudinal follow-up here would help.

Though this sample size was no different from the one examined in previous sensitization studies (including our own), it did not allow reliable investigation of higher-order interactions between personality, psychophysiology and DA response. A larger sample would have also allowed to study the potential moderating role of specific alleles/genotypes (for example, Met allele of the COMT Val(158)Met polymorphism, such as in Hernaus et al.53). Similarly, our sample may not have provided sufficient statistical power to detect significant correlations between BPND and behavioral and physiological measures. Moreover, the VOI analyses indicated that the DA response to stress re-exposure after the d-amphetamine regimen was highly variable. This high variability is hypothesized to account for the fact that the observed effects using t-maps could not be confirmed by VOI analyses. Alternatively, the activation loci in the t-maps differed from the VOI boundaries and thus may not have been revealed by VOI analyses. Indeed, striatal subregions based on cortical functional connectivity in humans appear to be larger in number than suggested by the tripartite model.54

Though the initial stress exposures elicited the expected effects on HR and negative mood states,55 re-exposure to stress after subchronic d-amphetamine administration did not elicit negative mood. The absence of mood lowering on stress re-exposure may be explained by the fact that behavioral sensitization to psychostimulants in healthy individuals may mainly be expressed as mood-elevation, arousal or psychomotor effects18 alterations that may even counteract negative responses to psychosocial stress.

The increased DA response to stress in the subchronic d-amphetamine subgroup might have been influenced by testing participants in the environment that had been paired with the drug. For example, in laboratory animals, drug-paired stimuli can facilitate the expression of DA sensitization and elicit long-lasting conditioned DA release.56, 57 Our previous studies have identified evidence of these same effects in humans. When the participants were tested in the drug-paired PET environment, we obtained evidence of drug-induced DA sensitization18 and conditioned DA release.58 In comparison, re-exposure to the drug-paired environment in the absence of the discrete drug cue (placebo capsule) did not lead to a conditioned DA response. As, in the present study, our final stress challenge was given in the drug-paired environment without a placebo capsule, it is possible that the expression of cross-sensitization was augmented by the drug-paired stimuli, while reflecting something other than the adding together of conditioned and stress-induced DA responses.59, 60

Although a strength of the study was the use of a well-validated method, [11C]raclopride is only sensitive to changes in DA release in the striatum. It would be of interest to study whether cross-sensitization or sensitization to stress also occurs in extrastriatal areas (for example, using [18F]fallypride). Indeed, we previously demonstrated DA release in the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex following acute laboratory psychosocial stress.55

Another potential limitation is the controllable nature of the stress exposure. Pre-clinical research has distinguished between two specific classes of stressors in their ability to precipitate sensitization: ‘controllable’ versus ‘uncontrollable’ events.61 Uncontrollable, intermittent stress appears to be a key feature of stressful events that trigger the neurobiological changes leading to cross-sensitization.62, 63 For ethical reasons, participants were allowed to terminate the experiment at any time they would choose, which may have reduced the perceived uncontrollability of the situation, allowing for a degree of ‘control’ which could have influenced stress ‘responses’, thus ‘sensitization’ to stress.

Finally, although the participants were carefully screened for use of drugs in the past and a urine drug test was done at the beginning of each session, some participants had used tobacco or cannabis in the past, and the timing of last nicotine exposure was not confirmed by blood testing. On the basis of the findings, in animals, that repeated nicotine or cannabis exposure can induce sensitization,64 it could be argued that participants who had previously smoked may already be sensitized, hence the theoretical possibility of another confound. However, the amount of prior nicotine or cannabis exposure was very low. Moreover, the d-amphetamine and placebo group did not significantly differ in their prior use, and an effect of the stress-amphetamine regimen could still be observed despite the potential influence of past use.

Top of page 

Summary and conclusion

The present study provides preliminary experimental evidence in vivo that DA sensitization to psychostimulants may generalize to stress in humans. Sensitization-like phenomena have been frequently proposed to account for stress-induced relapses in addiction or psychosis, that is, in disorders in which DA is believed to have a major role.2, 65 The present study tentatively identified amphetamine-related effects that could potentially be linked to how repeated drug exposure would progressively lead to onset or relapse, particularly for putative DA-related disorders, when someone is exposed to further life stressors.

Interestingly, repeated stress exposure alone also elicited some DA release within the striatum. Nevertheless, though speculative, it offers some support for the theory that repeated stressors with or without stimulants, may trigger a cascade of neurobiological events66 that may also influence the onset of or relapse to a number of DA-related disorders. In particular, the specific role of ‘sensitization’ to repeated ‘stress’ was envisaged, highlighted and discussed.

Previous studies have raised the possibility that sensitization and cross-sensitization could be relevant for the development and expression of psychiatric phenomenology in vulnerable individuals.4, 5, 6, 7, 67 For example, in a study of cocaine users who had experienced drug-induced psychotic reactions, 65% reported becoming progressively more susceptible to these effects (that is, paranoid ideation became more severe or was triggered by lower doses, indicative of behavioral sensitization), and these individuals were more likely to relapse to drug use at follow-up, as indexed by a greater number of re-hospitalizations.5 Determining more definitively whether DA sensitization underlies increased susceptibility to these and other problems will require longitudinal behavioral, neuroimaging and psychopharmacological challenge studies. Moreover, it will be important to determine the relevance of sensitization and cross-sensitization to a nonspecific lowering of the threshold for diverse symptoms vs specific symptoms; for example, autonomic system responsiveness, acute anxiety reactions, psychotic ‘breaks’, manic symptoms including increased goal-directed behaviors and renewed bouts of drug seeking and use. Although the proposition that the present observations can be generalized to psychiatric samples is compelling, for now, this remains to be confirmed.

Top of page 

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Top of page 

References

  1. Lieberman JA, Sheitman BB, Kinon BJ. Neurochemical sensitization in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia: deficits and dysfunction in neuronal regulation and plasticity. Neuropsychopharmacology 1997; 17: 205–229. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  2. Leyton M, Vezina P. Dopamine ups and downs in vulnerability to addictions: a neurodevelopmental model. Trends Pharmacol Sci 2014; 35: 268–276. | Article | PubMed |
  3. Kalivas PW, Stewart J. Dopamine transmission in the initiation and expression of drug- and stress-induced sensitization of motor activity. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1991; 16: 223–244. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  4. Angrist BM, Gershon S. The phenomenology of experimentally induced amphetamine psychosis—preliminary observations. Biol Psychiatry 1970; 2: 95–107. | PubMed | CAS |
  5. Bartlett E, Hallin A, Chapman B, Angrist B. Selective sensitization to the psychosis-inducing effects of cocaine: a possible marker for addiction relapse vulnerability? Neuropsychopharmacology 1997; 16: 77–82. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  6. Pierre PJ, Vezina P. Predisposition to self-administer amphetamine: the contribution of response to novelty and prior exposure to the drug. Psychopharmacology 1997; 129: 277–284. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  7. Hooks MS, Jones GH, Liem BJ, Justice JB Jr. Sensitization and individual differences to IP amphetamine, cocaine, or caffeine following repeated intra-cranial amphetamine infusions. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1992; 654: 444–447. | Article | PubMed |
  8. Antelman SM, Eichler AJ, Black CA, Kocan D. Interchangeability of stress and amphetamine in sensitization. Science 1980; 207: 329–331. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  9. Pierce RC, Kalivas PW. A circuitry model of the expression of behavioral sensitization to amphetamine-like psychostimulants. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 1997; 25: 192–216. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  10. Pani L, Porcella A, Gessa GL. The role of stress in the pathophysiology of the dopaminergic system. Mol Psychiatry 2000; 5: 14–21. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  11. Barr AM, Hofmann CE, Weinberg J, Phillips AG. Exposure to repeated, intermittent d-amphetamine induces sensitization of HPA axis to a subsequent stressor. Neuropsychopharmacology 2002; 26: 286–294. | Article | PubMed |
  12. Nikulina EM, Covington HE 3rd, Ganschow L, Hammer RP Jr, Miczek KA. Long-term behavioral and neuronal cross-sensitization to amphetamine induced by repeated brief social defeat stress: Fos in the ventral tegmental area and amygdala. Neuroscience 2004; 123: 857–865. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  13. Leyton M, Stewart J. Preexposure to foot-shock sensitizes the locomotor response to subsequent systemic morphine and intra-nucleus accumbens amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1990; 37: 303–310. | Article | PubMed |
  14. Matuszewich L, Carter S, Anderson EM, Friedman RD, McFadden LM. Persistent behavioral and neurochemical sensitization to an acute injection of methamphetamine following unpredictable stress. Behav Brain Res 2014; 272: 308–313. | Article | PubMed |
  15. Prasad BM, Sorg BA, Ulibarri C, Kalivas PW. Sensitization to stress and psychostimulants. Involvement of dopamine transmission versus the HPA axis. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1995; 771: 617–625. | Article | PubMed |
  16. Piazza PV, Le Moal ML. Pathophysiological basis of vulnerability to drug abuse: role of an interaction between stress, glucocorticoids, and dopaminergic neurons. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 1996; 36: 359–378. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  17. Reid MS, Ho LB, Tolliver BK, Wolkowitz OM, Berger SP. Partial reversal of stress-induced behavioral sensitization to amphetamine following metyrapone treatment. Brain Res 1998; 783: 133–142. | Article | PubMed |
  18. Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Gunn RN, Baker GB, Diksic M et al. Modeling sensitization to stimulants in humans: an [11C]raclopride/positron emission tomography study in healthy men. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63: 1386–1395. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  19. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. . Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition. SCID-I/NP: New York, NY, USA, 2002.
  20. Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Burnout, perceived stress, and cortisol responses to awakening. Psychosom Med 1999; 61: 197–204. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  21. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Wesleyan University Press: Middleton, CT, USA, 1989.
  22. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1970.
  23. Kirschbaum C, Prussner JC, Stone AA, Federenko I, Gaab J, Lintz D et al. Persistent high cortisol responses to repeated psychological stress in a subpopulation of healthy men. Psychosom Med 1995; 57: 468–474. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  24. Schommer NC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Dissociation between reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosom Med 2003; 65: 450–460. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  25. Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Low self-esteem, induced failure and the adrenocortical stress response. Pers Indiv Differ 1999; 27: 477–489. | Article |
  26. Pruessner JC, Champagne F, Meaney MJ, Dagher A. Dopamine release in response to a psychological stress in humans and its relationship to early life maternal care: a positron emission tomography study using [11C]raclopride. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 2825–2831. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  27. Soliman A, O’Driscoll GA, Pruessner J, Holahan AL, Boileau I, Gagnon D et al. Stress-induced dopamine release in humans at risk of psychosis: a [11C]raclopride PET study. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33: 2033–2041. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  28. Lorr M, McNair DM, Fisher SU. Evidence for bipolar mood states. J Pers Assess 1982; 46: 432–436. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  29. Costes N, Dagher A, Larcher K, Evans AC, Collins DL, Reilhac A. Motion correction of multi-frame PET data in neuroreceptor mapping: simulation based validation. Neuroimage 2009; 47: 1496–1505. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  30. Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC. Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1994; 18: 192–205. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  31. Lammertsma AA, Hume SP. Simplified reference tissue model for PET receptor studies. Neuroimage 1996; 4: 153–158. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  32. Gunn RN, Lammertsma AA, Hume SP, Cunningham VJ. Parametric imaging of ligand-receptor binding in PET using a simplified reference region model. Neuroimage 1997; 6: 279–287. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  33. Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp 1996; 4: 58–73. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  34. Collins L, Evans AC, Holmes C, Peters TM. Automatic 3D segmentation of neuro-anatomical structures from MRI. Information processing in medical imaging. Vol. 3. Kluwer: Dordrecht, 1995.
  35. Evans AC, Marrett S, Neelin P, Collins L, Worsley K, Dai W et al. Anatomical mapping of functional activation in stereotactic coordinate space. Neuroimage 1992; 1: 43–53. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  36. Pruessner JC, Kirschbaum C, Meinlschmid G, Hellhammer DH. Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003; 28: 916–931. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  37. Cruz FC, Marin MT, Leao RM, Planeta CS. Stress-induced cross-sensitization to amphetamine is related to changes in the dopaminergic system. J Neural Transm 2012; 119: 415–424. | Article | PubMed |
  38. Abercrombie ED, Keefe KA, DiFrischia DS, Zigmond MJ. Differential effect of stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial frontal cortex. J Neurochem 1989; 52: 1655–1658. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  39. Finlay JM, Zigmond MJ. The effects of stress on central dopaminergic neurons: possible clinical implications. Neurochem Res 1997; 22: 1387–1394. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  40. Rouge-Pont F, Piazza PV, Kharouby M, Le Moal M, Simon H. Higher and longer stress-induced increase in dopamine concentrations in the nucleus accumbens of animals predisposed to amphetamine self-administration. A microdialysis study. Brain Res 1993; 602: 169–174. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  41. Mizrahi R, Kenk M, Suridjan I, Boileau I, George TP, McKenzie K et al. Stress-induced dopamine response in subjects at clinical high risk for schizophrenia with and without concurrent cannabis use. Neuropsychopharmacology 2014; 39: 1479–1489. | Article | PubMed |
  42. Mizrahi R, Addington J, Rusjan PM, Suridjan I, Ng A, Boileau I et al. Increased stress-induced dopamine release in psychosis. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 71: 561–567. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  43. Tidey JW, Miczek KA. Social defeat stress selectively alters mesocorticolimbic dopamine release: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res 1996; 721: 140–149. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  44. McLaughlin KA, Kubzansky LD, Dunn EC, Waldinger R, Vaillant G, Koenen KC. Childhood social environment, emotional reactivity to stress, and mood and anxiety disorders across the life course. Depress Anxiety 2010; 27: 1087–1094. | Article | PubMed |
  45. Booij L, Tremblay RE, Szyf M, Benkelfat C. Genetic and early environmental influences on the serotonin system: consequences for brain development and risk for psychopathology. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2015; 40: 5–18. | Article | PubMed |
  46. Oswald LM, Wand GS, Kuwabara H, Wong DF, Zhu S, Brasic JR. History of childhood adversity is positively associated with ventral striatal dopamine responses to amphetamine. Psychopharmacology 2014; 231: 2417–2433. | Article | PubMed |
  47. Martinez D, Gil R, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Huang Y, Perez A et al. Alcohol dependence is associated with blunted dopamine transmission in the ventral striatum. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58: 779–786. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  48. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gatley SJ, Hitzemann R et al. Decreased striatal dopaminergic responsiveness in detoxified cocaine-dependent subjects. Nature 1997; 386: 830–833. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  49. Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M et al. Profound decreases in dopamine release in striatum in detoxified alcoholics: possible orbitofrontal involvement. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 12700–12706. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  50. Gipson CD, Kalivas PW. More cocaine-more glutamate-more addiction. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 76: 765–766. | Article | PubMed |
  51. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science 1997; 278: 52–58. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  52. Leyton M. What’s deficient in reward deficiency? J Psychiatry Neurosci 2014; 39: 291–293. | Article | PubMed |
  53. Hernaus D, Collip D, Lataster J, Ceccarini J, Kenis G, Booij L et al. COMT Val158Met genotype selectively alters prefrontal [18F]fallypride displacement and subjective feelings of stress in response to a psychosocial stress challenge. PLoS One 2013; 8: e65662. | Article | PubMed |
  54. Choi EY, Yeo BT, Buckner RL. The organization of the human striatum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 2012; 108: 2242–2263. | Article | PubMed |
  55. Nagano-Saito A, Dagher A, Booij L, Gravel P, Welfeld K, Casey KF et al. Stress-induced dopamine release in human medial prefrontal cortex—18F-fallypride/PET study in healthy volunteers. Synapse 2013; 67: 821–830. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  56. Duvauchelle CL, Ikegami A, Asami S, Robens J, Kressin K, Castaneda E. Effects of cocaine context on NAcc dopamine and behavioral activity after repeated intravenous cocaine administration. Brain Res 2000; 862: 49–58. | Article | PubMed |
  57. Weiss F, Maldonado-Vlaar CS, Parsons LH, Kerr TM, Smith DL, Ben-Shahar O. Control of cocaine-seeking behavior by drug-associated stimuli in rats: effects on recovery of extinguished operant-responding and extracellular dopamine levels in amygdala and nucleus accumbens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000; 97: 4321–4326. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  58. Boileau I, Dagher A, Leyton M, Welfeld K, Booij L, Diksic M et al. Conditioned dopamine release in humans: a positron emission tomography [11C]raclopride study with amphetamine. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 3998–4003. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  59. Vezina P, Leyton M. Conditioned cues and the expression of stimulant sensitization in animals and humans. Neuropharmacology 2009; 56(Suppl 1): 160–168. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  60. Robinson TE, Browman KE, Crombag HS, Badiani A. Modulation of the induction or expression of psychostimulant sensitization by the circumstances surrounding drug administration. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1998; 22: 347–354. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  61. Cabib S, Puglisi-Allegra S. Stress, depression and the mesolimbic dopamine system. Psychopharmacology 1996; 128: 331–342. | Article | PubMed | ISI | CAS |
  62. MacLennan AJ, Maier SF. Coping and the stress-induced potentiation of stimulant stereotypy in the rat. Science 1983; 219: 1091–1093. | Article | PubMed |
  63. Anisman H, Hahn B, Hoffman D, Zacharko RM. Stressor invoked exacerbation of amphetamine-elicited perseveration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1985; 23: 173–183. | Article | PubMed |
  64. Vezina P, McGehee DS, Green WN. Exposure to nicotine and sensitization of nicotine-induced behaviors. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2007; 31: 1625–1638. | Article | PubMed | CAS |
  65. Howes OD, Murray RM. Schizophrenia: an integrated sociodevelopmental-cognitive model. Lancet 2014; 383: 1677–1687. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  66. Seo D, Tsou KA, Ansell EB, Potenza MN, Sinha R. Cumulative adversity sensitizes neural response to acute stress: association with health symptoms. Neuropsychopharmacology 2014; 39: 670–680. | Article | PubMed | ISI |
  67. Post RM. Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149: 999–1010. | Article | PubMed | ISI |