Neurocircuitry of Addiction (2010)

COMMENTS: Another addiction review describing the mechanisms of addiction by two of the worlds leading experts. Note that Nora Volkow is the head of NIDA, and has stated that behavioral addictions involve the same fundamental mechanisms and brain changes as drug addictions.


FULL STUDY – Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010 January; 35(1): 217–238.

Lomia i luga o le initaneti 2009 Aukuso 26. Tui: 10.1038 / npp.2009.110

George F Koob and Nora D Volkow

lē faʻatino

Drug addiction is a chronically relapsing disorder that has been characterized by (1) compulsion to seek and take the drug, (2) loss of control in limiting intake, and (3) emergence of a negative emotional state (eg, dysphoria, anxiety, irritability) reflecting a motivational withdrawal syndrome when access to the drug is prevented. Drug addiction has been conceptualized as a disorder that involves elements of both impulsivity and compulsivity that yield a composite addiction cycle composed of three stages: ‘binge/intoxication’, ‘withdrawal/negative affect’, and ‘preoccupation/anticipation’ (craving). Animal and human imaging studies have revealed discrete circuits that mediate the three stages of the addiction cycle with key elements of the ventral tegmental area and ventral striatum as a focal point for the binge/intoxication stage, a key role for the extended amygdala in the withdrawal/negative affect stage, and a key role in the preoccupation/anticipation stage for a widely distributed network involving the orbitofrontal cortex–dorsal striatum, prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala, hippocampus, and insula involved in craving and the cingulate gyrus, dorsolateral prefrontal, and inferior frontal cortices in disrupted inhibitory control. The transition to addiction involves neuroplasticity in all of these structures that may begin with changes in the mesolimbic dopamine system and a cascade of neuroadaptations from the ventral striatum to dorsal striatum and orbitofrontal cortex and eventually dysregulation of the prefrontal cortex, cingulate gyrus, and extended amygdala. The delineation of the neurocircuitry of the evolving stages of the addiction syndrome forms a heuristic basis for the search for the molecular, genetic, and neuropharmacological neuroadaptations that are key to vulnerability for developing and maintaining addiction.

CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK

Addiction Definitions: Drug Use, Abuse, and Dependence Addiction Cycle

Drug addiction is a chronically relapsing disorder that has been characterized by (1) compulsion to seek and take the drug, (2) loss of control in limiting intake, and (3) emergence of a negative emotional state (eg, dysphoria, anxiety, irritability) reflecting a motivational withdrawal syndrome when access to the drug is prevented (defined as Substance Dependence by the Fuainumera Faʻailoga ma Fuainumera Faʻamaumauga o Manatu o Mafaufauga [DSM] of the American Psychiatric Association; Koob ma Le Moal, 1997; Laulau 1). The occasional but limited use of an abusable drug is clinically distinct from escalated drug use, loss of control over limiting drug intake, and the emergence of chronic compulsive drug-seeking that characterizes addiction. The critical nature of the distinction between drug use, abuse, and dependence has been illuminated by data showing that approximately 15.6% (29 million) of the US adult population will go on to engage in nonmedical or illicit drug use at some time in their lives, with approximately 2.9% (5.4 million) going on to substance dependence on illicit drugs (Grant ma Dawson, 1998; Grant 'ua al, 2004). For alcohol, 51% (120 million) of people over the age of 12 were current users, and of these current users, 7.7% (18 million) met the criteria for Substance Abuse or Dependence on Alcohol. For nicotine, in 2007, approximately 28.6% (70.9 million) Americans aged 12 or older were current (past month) users of a tobacco product, and of these current users, 24.2% (60.1 million) were current cigarette smokers; 5.4% (13.3 million) smoked cigars; 3.2% (8.1 million) used smokeless tobacco; and 0.8% (2.0 million) smoked tobacco in pipes (Faʻasologa o le faʻaaogaina ma le faʻaleleia o le mafaufau, 2008).

Laulau 1

Faʻamatalaga

Although much of the initial study of the neurobiology of drug addiction focused on the acute impact of drugs of abuse (analogous to comparing no drug use to drug use), the focus now is shifting to chronic administration and the acute and long-term neuroadaptive changes in the brain that result in relapse. The purpose of current neurobiological drug abuse research is to understand the genetic/epigenetic, cellular, and molecular mechanisms that mediate the transition from occasional, controlled drug use to the loss of behavioral control over drug-seeking and drug-taking and to chronic relapse even after protracted abstinence which is a hallmark of addiction.

A psychiatric-motivational framework that provides sources of both positive and negative reinforcement for drug-taking is the conceptualization that drug addiction has aspects of both impulse control disorders and compulsive disorders (Laulau 1). Impulse control disorders are characterized by an increasing sense of tension or arousal before committing an impulsive act and pleasure, gratification, or relief at the time of committing the act. Impulse control disorders are largely associated with positive reinforcement mechanisms (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). In contrast, compulsive disorders are characterized by anxiety and stress before committing a compulsive repetitive behavior and relief from the stress by performing the compulsive behavior. Compulsive disorders are largely associated with negative reinforcement mechanisms and automaticity.

Collapsing the cycles of impulsivity and compulsivity yields a composite addiction cycle composed of three stages—binge/intoxication, withdrawal/negative affect, preoccupation/anticipation—in which impulsivity often dominates at the early stages and impulsivity combined with compulsivity dominates at the later stages. As an individual moves from impulsivity to compulsivity, a shift occurs from positive reinforcement driving the motivated behavior to negative reinforcement and automaticity driving the motivated behavior (Koob, 2004; Laulau 1). These three stages are conceptualized as interacting with each other, becoming more intense, and ultimately leading to the pathological state known as addiction (Koob ma Le Moal, 1997; Laulau 2). The transition from occasional drug use to addiction involves neuroplasticity in all of these elements and may begin with initial drug use in vulnerable individuals or individuals at particularly vulnerable developmental periods (eg, adolescence; Koob 'ua al, 2008b). The present review focuses on the brain neurocircuitry that is engaged at each stage of the addiction cycle, how it changes with increasing engagement with drugs of abuse, and how it interacts to produce the pathological state known as addiction.

Laulau 2

Animal and Human Laboratory Models of The Different Stages of The Addiction Cycle

Sources of Reinforcement: Motivation, Opponent Process, Incentive Salience

Changes in the motivation for drugs and natural rewards are a key component of addiction (Laulau 1). Early work by Wikler (1952) stressed the function of changes in drive states associated with dependence (herein referred to as addiction. Subjects described withdrawal changes as a ‘hunger’ or primary need and the effects of morphine on such a state as ‘satiation’ or gratification of the primary need (Wikler, 1952). Although Wikler argued that positive reinforcement was retained even in heavily dependent subjects (eg, thrill of the intravenous opioid injection), addiction produced a new source of gratification, that of negative reinforcement (Laulau 1).

The concept of motivation was linked inextricably with hedonic, affective, or emotional states in the transition to addiction by Solomon’s opponent process theory of motivation. Solomon and Corbit (1974) postulated that hedonic, affective, or emotional states, once initiated, are automatically modulated by the central nervous system with mechanisms that reduce the intensity of hedonic feelings. Positive hedonic responses in drug use occur shortly after presentation of a stimulus, correlate closely with the intensity, quality, and duration of the reinforcer, and show tolerance and affective or hedonic withdrawal (abstinence). In contrast, negative hedonic responses follow the positive hedonic responses, are sluggish in onset, slow to build up to an asymptote, slow to decay, and get larger with repeated exposure. The role of opponent processes begins early in drug-taking, reflects changes in the brain reward and stress systems, and later forms one of the major motivations for compulsivity in drug-taking in the form of a motivational withdrawal syndrome.

In this formulation, manifestation of a withdrawal syndrome after removal of chronic drug administration, either acute or protracted, is defined in terms of motivational aspects of dependence such as the emergence of a negative emotional state (eg, dysphoria, anxiety, irritability) when access to the drug is prevented (Koob ma Le Moal, 2001), rather than on the physical signs of dependence, which tend to be of short duration. Indeed, some have argued that the development of such a negative affective state can define dependence relative to addiction (Russell, 1976; faifalaoa 'ua al, 1987) and that such a negative affective state contributes to compulsivity through negative reinforcement mechanisms (Koob ma Le Moal, 2005).

Another conceptualization of the motivational changes associated with addiction is derived from early work on conditioned reinforcement, incentive motivation, behavioral sensitization, and maladaptive stimulus–response learning, all of which are subsumed under the motivational conceptualization of incentive salience. Drugs are hypothesized to usurp systems in the brain that are put in place to direct animals to stimuli with salience for preservation of the species. The incentive salience hypothesis has significant heuristic value as a common element of drug addiction because it narrows the focus to drug-seeking at the expense of natural rewards. The clinical observation that individuals with substance use disorders have an unusual focus on drug-seeking to the exclusion of natural rewards fits the incentive salience view.

The increase in incentive salience produced by psychostimulant drugs has early roots in the facilitation of conditioned reinforcement and drug-seeking (Robbins, 1976; Hill, 1970). Here, drug-seeking is controlled by a succession of drug-associated discriminative stimuli that can also function as conditioned reinforcers when presented as a consequence of instrumental responses (Faʻaauau 'ua al, 2008). Many have argued that by means of associative learning, the enhanced incentive salience state becomes oriented specifically toward drug-related stimuli, leading to escalating compulsion for seeking and taking drugs (Hyman 'ua al, 2006; Kalivas ma Volkow, 2005). The underlying activation of neural structures involved in maintaining the incentive salience state persists, making addicts vulnerable to long-term relapse.

Another view of incentive salience involved behavioral sensitization, usually measured as increased locomotor responses to repeated administration of a drug. The behavioral sensitization paradigm has provided a major impetus to exploring not only the neurocircuitry of addiction but also a model of the neuroplasticity that may occur during the transition from drug use to addiction. Here, a shift in an incentive salience state, described as ‘wanting’ linked to compulsive use, as opposed to ‘liking’ linked to hedonic responses, was hypothesized to be progressively increased by repeated exposure to drugs of abuse (Robinson ma Berridge, 1993).

Transition to Addiction: Patterns of Drug-Taking, Animal Models

Different drugs produce different patterns of neuroadaptations with chronic drug exposures. For example, opioid-addicted subjects meet most of the DSM criteria for addiction, including dramatic tolerance and withdrawal (classic symptoms associated with physical dependence) and most of the symptoms associated with motivational withdrawal. A pattern of intravenous or smoked drug-taking evolves, including intoxication, tolerance, escalation in intake, and profound dysphoria, physical discomfort, and somatic withdrawal signs during abstinence. Intense preoccupation with obtaining opioids (craving) develops that often precedes the somatic signs of withdrawal and is linked not only to stimuli associated with obtaining the drug but also to stimuli associated with withdrawal and the aversive motivational state. A pattern develops in which the drug must be obtained to avoid the severe dysphoria and discomfort of abstinence. Other drugs of abuse follow a similar pattern but may involve more the binge/intoxication stage (psychostimulants) or less binge/intoxication and more withdrawal/negative affect and preoccupation/anticipation stages (nicotine and cannabinoids).

Much of the recent progress in understanding the neurobiology of addiction has derived from the study of animal models of addiction to specific drugs such as stimulants, opioids, alcohol, nicotine, and Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (Δ9-THC). Although no animal model of addiction fully emulates the human condition, animal models do permit investigation of specific elements of the process of drug addiction. Such elements can be defined by models of different stages of the addiction cycle (see above; Laulau 2).

A progressive increase in the frequency and intensity of drug use is one of the major behavioral phenomena characterizing the development of addiction and has face validity with the DSM criteria: ‘The substance is often taken in larger amounts and over a longer period than was intended’ (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Two animal models, one involving experimenter-administered drug, and the other involving self-administered drug, have been used to explore the effects of repeated drug administration on neuroplasticity in the neurocircuits identified above. Behavioral sensitization typically involved repeated administration by the experimenter of a drug, usually a psychostimulant, in a specific environmental context and the dependent measure was usually locomotor activity. Here, animals that received drug showed a much more dramatic increase in locomotor activity to a challenge dose of drug (sensitization) than controls that had received only repeated measures of vehicle injections.

A framework, perhaps with more face validity with which to model the transition from drug use to drug addiction, can be found in animal models of prolonged access to self- administration of drugs. Here, using intravenous drug self-administration, extended access to drugs is associated with an escalation in intake over days (Koob, 2009a). Such increased self-administration also has been observed with alcohol in which rats drink excessively during acute and protracted withdrawal from dependence induction using either chronic liquid diet or chronic vapor exposure (Gilpin and Koob, 2008). Animals made dependent on alcohol reliably obtain blood alcohol levels in the 100–150mg% range, which are equivalent to the levels abused by moderate to heavy alcohol abusers. Changes in the reinforcing and incentive effects of the drug have been observed following extended access and induction of dependence and include increased progressive-ratio responding (Koob, 2009a), increased drug-induced reinstatement after extinction, decreased latency to goal time in a runway model for drug reward (Deroche-Gamonet 'ua al, 2004), and increased resistance to punishment in which the animal will sustain higher aversive punishment to obtain drug (Vanderschuren ma Everitt, 2004). Whether the enhanced drug-taking with extended access reflects a sensitization of reward (or of incentive motivation) or a reward-deficit state, or both, remains under discussion (Vezina, 2004).

NEUROCIRCUITRY OF ADDICTION: NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGICAL EVIDENCE FROM ANIMAL STUDIES

Binge/Intoxication Stage

Our understanding of the neurobiological substrates for the reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse can be traced to early work on the identification of a reward system in the brain with the discovery of electrical brain stimulation reward or intracranial self-stimulation by Olds ma Milner (1954). Brain stimulation reward involves widespread neurocircuitry in the brain, but the most sensitive sites defined by the lowest thresholds involve the trajectory of the medial forebrain bundle that connects the ventral tegmental area (VTA) to the basal forebrain (Olds ma Milner, 1954). All drugs of abuse, when administered acutely, decrease brain stimulation reward thresholds (ie, increased reward; Kornetsky and Esposito, 1979) and when administered chronically increase reward thresholds during withdrawal (ie, decreased reward; see below). Although much emphasis was focused initially on the role of the ascending monoamine systems in the medial forebrain bundle in reward, first norepinephrine (Stein, 1962) and then dopamine (Fou, 1973; Poto, 1978), other nondopaminergic systems in the medial forebrain bundle clearly have a key role in mediating brain stimulation reward (Hernandez 'ua al, 2006). Indeed, much work suggests that activation of the midbrain dopamine system has multiple roles to give incentive salience to stimuli in the environment (Robinson ma Berridge, 1993) to promote performance of goal-directed behavior (Salamone 'ua al, 2007) or activation in general (Le Moal ma Simon, 1991). More recently, the hypothesis has been raised that the time course of dopamine signaling is a key factor, with the fastest time course predominantly having a preferential role in reward and valuation of predicted outcomes of behavior and steady activation of dopamine release having a preferential role in providing an enabling effect on specific behavior-related systems (Schultz, 2007). Work in the domain of the acute reinforcing effects of drugs of abuse supports this hypothesis in which the mesolimbic dopamine system is critical for the acute rewarding effects of psychostimulant drugs but has a more enabling function for all drugs of abuse.

The acute rewarding properties of psychostimulant drugs have long been known to depend on activation of the mesolimbic dopamine system, but activation of this system is not necessarily critical for the acute reinforcing effects of other drugs of abuse (Koob, 1992; Nestler, 2005; Hnasko 'ua al, 2005). Neurotoxin-selective lesions of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system block the reinforcing effects of cocaine and -amphetamine (McGregor and Roberts, 1993). In contrast, neurochemically specific lesions of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens with 6-hydroxydopamine failed to block heroin or ethanol self-administration, supporting this hypothesis (Koob ma Le Moal, 2006).

Using the technique of intracranial self-administration (Laulau 1) and intracranial place conditioning (Laulau 1), opioids and alcohol have been shown to be directly self-administered into the VTA. Opioids also produce conditioned place preference when injected into the VTA. Opioids, phencyclidine, and psychostimulants are directly self-administered into the nucleus accumbens, and psychostimulants produce a conditioned place preference when injected into the nucleus accumbens. Cocaine and phencyclidine are directly self-administered into the frontal cortex (McBride 'ua al, 1999). The mesolimbic dopamine system is activated by acute administration of opioids, ethanol, nicotine, and Δ9-THC (Di Chiara ma Imperato, 1988).

Intravenous nicotine self-administration is blocked by neurotoxin-specific lesions of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system, and the neuropharmacological action has been hypothesized to be through nicotinic receptor activation of release of dopamine primarily in the VTA and also presynaptically in the nucleus accumbens (Watkins 'ua al, 2000). However, nicotine reward measured by conditioned place preference appears to be independent of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system (Laviolette 'ua al, 2002). Other substrates implicated in nicotine reward include cholinergic inputs to the pedunculopontine nucleus (Yeomans and Baptista, 1997). In the VTA, activation of the β2 subunit of nicotinic receptors appears to be critical for nicotine activation of dopamine neurons (Mameli-Engvall 'ua al, 2006). Neuropharmacological studies on cannabinoids have implicated both cannabinoid and opioid mechanisms. Opioid and cannabinoid CB1 antagonists block intravenous self-administration of Δ9-THC in squirrel monkeys (Justinova 'ua al, 2003). Similar to other drugs of abuse, Δ9-THC administration activates dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens shell (Tanda 'ua al, 1997).

Thus, all drugs of abuse activate the mesolimbic dopamine system, but much evidence suggests that dopamine-independent reinforcement occurs at the level of the nucleus accumbens, suggesting multiple inputs to the activation of critical reinforcement circuitry in these brain regions (Koob, 1992; Nestler, 2005).

The central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) also has a key function in the acute reinforcing actions of drugs of abuse. Microinjections of dopamine D1 receptor antagonists into the CeA block cocaine self-administration (Caine 'ua al, 1995; McGregor and Roberts, 1993). The most sensitive site for γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and opioid antagonism of oral alcohol self-administration in nondependent rats was the CeA (Hyytia and Koob, 1995; Heyser 'ua al, 1999). Lesions of the CeA block oral self-administration of alcohol (Moller 'ua al, 1997). Serotonin-3 antagonists injected into the CeA block oral ethanol self-administration in nondependent rats, an effect hypothesized to possibly involve the ability of serotonin-3 receptor antagonists to block drug-induced dopamine release (Dyr and Kostowski, 1995).

A major output from the nucleus accumbens is to the ventral pallidum/substantia innominata. Consistent with the nucleus accumbens as a key substrate for drug reward, lesions of the ventral pallidum are particularly effective in blocking the motivation to work for intravenous cocaine and intravenous heroin (Hubner and Koob, 1990; Robledo and Koob, 1993). In addition, blockade of dopamine and GABAA receptors in the ventral pallidum blocks the reinforcing effects of alcohol (Melendez 'ua al, 2004; Iuni 'ua al, 2003). Thus, elements of the ventral pallidum may not only be critical for further processing of the drug reward signal but may also be directly modulated by drugs of abuse.

The dorsal striatum does not appear to have a major role in the acute reinforcing effects of drugs abuse but appears to be recruited during the development of compulsive drug-seeking (Faʻaauau 'ua al, 2008). 6-Hydroxydopamine lesions of the dorsal striatum do not block cocaine-induced locomotor activity or cocaine self-administration (Roberts, 1992) but do block amphetamine-induced stereotyped behavior (Kelly and Iversen, 1976; Creese and Iversen, 1974). Using a second-order schedule (Laulau 1), lesions of the nucleus accumbens and basolateral amygdala blocked the acquisition of cocaine-seeking (Whitelaw 'ua al, 1996). Similarly, when the nucleus accumbens core was selectively lesioned on one side of the brain and combined with dopamine receptor blockade in the contralateral dorsal striatum, no effect was observed in animals immediately after acquisition, but greatly decreased cocaine-seeking was observed in rats with stable responding on a second-order schedule (Belin ma Everitt, 2008). These results suggest that the dorsal striatum may have a minor role in the acute reinforcing effects of psychostimulant drugs but a key role in the transition to compulsive use (Faʻaauau 'ua al, 2008).

Data with knockout mice also provide key insights into the role of dopamine in the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse. Genetically altered mice homozygous with a lack of the dopamine D1 receptor do not self-administer cocaine (Caine 'ua al, 2007). Although the initial report that dopamine transporter (DAT) knockout mice continued to self-administer cocaine (Rocha 'ua al, 1998) questioned the function of the DAT in cocaine’s reinforcing effects, a recent study showed that transgenic animals that expressed DAT that did not bind cocaine but that was functional as a dopamine reuptake carrier did not show cocaine reward measured by conditioned place preference (Chen 'ua al, 2006a). These results support the hypothesis of a crucial role of the DAT in cocaine’s reinforcing effects.

On the basis of this synthesis, an early neurobiological circuit for drug reward was proposed (Koob, 1992) that has been elaborated and expanded (Koob ma Nestler, 1997; Ata 1). The starting point for the reward circuit was the medial forebrain bundle, composed of myelinated fibers connecting bidirectionally the olfactory tubercle and nucleus accumbens with the hypothalamus and VTA (Nauta and Haymaker, 1969) and including the ascending monoamine pathways such as the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system.

Ata 1

Sagittal section through a representative rodent brain illustrating the pathways and receptor systems implicated in the acute reinforcing actions of drugs of abuse. Cocaine and amphetamines activate the release of dopamine in the nucleus accumbens and ...

The initial action of drug reward was hypothesized to depend on dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens for cocaine, amphetamine, and nicotine; opioid peptide receptor activation in the VTA (dopamine activation) and nucleus accumbens (independent of dopamine activation) for opiates; and GABAA systems in the nucleus accumbens and amygdala for alcohol. The nucleus accumbens is situated strategically to receive important limbic information from the amygdala, frontal cortex, and hippocampus that could be converted to motivational action through its connections with the extrapyramidal motor system. Thus, an early critical role for the nucleus accumbens was established for the acute reinforcing effects of drugs, with a supporting role for the CeA and ventral pallidum (Faʻafanua 1 ma and2a2a).

Ata 2

Neural circuitry associated with the three stages of the addiction cycle. (a) Binge/intoxication stage. Reinforcing effects of drugs may engage reward neurotransmitters and associative mechanisms in the nucleus accumbens shell and core and then engage ...

Withdrawal/Negative Affect Stage

O le vaega neuroanatomical ua taʻua o le amygdala lautele (Heimer ma Alheid, 1991) may represent a common anatomical substrate integrating brain arousal–stress systems with hedonic processing systems to produce the negative emotional states that promote negative reinforcement mechanisms associated with the development of addiction. The extended amygdala is composed of the CeA, bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST), and a transition zone in the medial (shell) subregion of the nucleus accumbens (Ata 2b). Each of these regions has cytoarchitectural and circuitry similarities (Heimer ma Alheid, 1991). The extended amygdala receives numerous afferents from limbic structures such as the basolateral amygdala and hippocampus and sends efferents to the medial part of the ventral pallidum and a large projection to the lateral hypothalamus, thus further defining the specific brain areas that interface classical limbic (emotional) structures with the output of extrapyramidal motor system (Alheid 'ua al, 1995). O le amygdala lautele ua leva ona manatu e iai se matafaioi taua e le gata i le faʻafefe o le fefe (Le Doux, 2000) ae faʻapea foʻi i le vaega faʻalagona o le faʻaogaina o tiga (Neugebauer 'ua al, 2004).

Within-system neuroadaptations to chronic drug exposure include decreases in function of the neurotransmitter systems in the neurocircuits implicated in the acute reinforcing effects of drug of abuse. One prominent hypothesis is that dopamine systems are compromised in crucial phases of the addiction cycle, such as withdrawal, and lead to decreased motivation for nondrug-related stimuli and increased sensitivity to the abused drug (Melis 'ua al, 2005; see brain imaging studies below). Psychostimulant withdrawal in humans is associated with fatigue, decreased mood and psychomotor retardation, and in animals is associated with decreased motivation to work for natural rewards (Barr ma Phillips, 1999) and decreased locomotor activity (Pulvirenti and Koob, 1993), behavioral effects that may involve decreased dopaminergic function. Animals during amphetamine withdrawal show decreased responding on a progressive-ratio schedule for a sweet solution, and this decreased responding was reversed by the dopamine partial agonist terguride (Orsini 'ua al, 2001), suggesting that low dopamine tone contributes to the motivational deficits associated with psychostimulant withdrawal. Decreases in activity of the mesolimbic dopamine system and decreases in serotonergic neurotransmission in the nucleus accumbens occur during acute drug withdrawal from all major drugs of abuse in animal studies (Rossetti 'ua al, 1992; Weiss 'ua al, 1992, 1996).

A second component of the withdrawal/negative affect stage is a between-system neuroadaptation in which different neurochemical systems involved in stress modulation also may be engaged within the neurocircuitry of the brain stress and aversive systems in an attempt to overcome the chronic presence of the perturbing drug to restore normal function despite the presence of drug. Both the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and the brain stress/aversive system mediated by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) are activated during withdrawal from chronic administration of all major drugs with abuse potential, with a common response of elevated adrenocorticotropic hormone, corticosterone, and amygdala CRF during acute withdrawal (Koob, 2008; Koob ma Kreek, 2007). Acute withdrawal from all drugs of abuse also produces an aversive or anxiety-like state in which CRF and other stress-related systems (including noradrenergic pathways) have key roles.

The aversive stimulus effects of drug withdrawal can be measured using place aversion (lima 'ua al, 1988), and the opioid partial agonist buprenorphine dose dependently decreased the place aversion produced by precipitated opioid withdrawal. Systemic administration of a CRF1 fa'afeagai tali ma le fa'atonuina o le intracerebral o le peptide CRF1/ CRF2 na fa'aitiitia fo'i e le tagata fa'afeagai le fa'ate'a ese o le opioid i nofoaga (Stinus 'ua al, 2005; Heinrichs 'ua al, 1995). Functional noradrenergic antagonists administered directly into the BNST blocked opioid withdrawal-induced place aversion, implicating the importance of noradrenergic stimulation in the stress responses that follow acute drug withdrawal (Delfs 'ua al, 2000). Indeed, classical medications used to treat physical withdrawal in heroin abusers and alcoholics include α-adrenergic drugs (eg, clonidine) that inhibit noradrenergic release and decrease some symptoms of alcohol and heroin withdrawal.

Another candidate for the aversive effects of drug withdrawal is dynorphin. Much evidence shows that dynorphin is increased in the nucleus accumbens in response to dopaminergic activation and, in turn, that overactivity of the dynorphin systems can decrease dopaminergic function. κ-Opioid agonists are aversive, and cocaine, opioid, and ethanol withdrawal is associated with increased dynorphin in the nucleus accumbens and/or amygdala (Koob, 2008). An exception is salvidorin A, which is a κ-agonist abused by humans, but this may reflect its hallucinogenic effects rather than any pleasurable properties (Gonzalez 'ua al, 2006).

Another common between-system response to acute withdrawal and protracted abstinence from all major drugs of abuse is the manifestation of anxiety-like responses. For example, withdrawal from repeated administration of cocaine produces an anxiogenic-like response in the elevated plus maze and defensive burying test, both of which are reversed by CRF antagonists. Similarly, ethanol withdrawal produces anxiety-like behavior that is reversed by intracerebroventricular administration of CRF1/ CRF2 peptidergic antagonists, systemic administration of a small molecule CRF1 antagonist, and microinjection of a peptidergic CRF1/ CRF2 tagata tetee i le amygdala (Mea malie 'ua al, 2006; Koob, 2008). CRF antagonists injected intracerebroventricularly or systemically also block the potentiated anxiety-like responses to stressors observed during protracted abstinence from chronic ethanol, and the effects of CRF antagonists have been localized to the CeA (Koob, 2008). O le alu ese mai le nicotine e maua mai ai tali e pei o le popole lea e fesuiaʻi foi e le CRF antagonists (Tucci 'ua al, 2003; George 'ua al, 2007).

Thus, acute withdrawal is associated with within-system changes reflected in a decrease in dopaminergic activity in the mesolimbic dopamine system and with between-system recruitment of neurotransmitter systems that convey stress and anxiety-like effects such as CRF and dynorphin. Other neurotransmitter systems known to be involved in emotional dysregulation of the motivational effects of drug withdrawal include norepinephrine, substance P, vasopressin, neuropeptide Y (NPY), endocannabinoids, and nociceptin (Koob, 2008).

Preoccupation/Anticipation (Craving) Stage

The preoccupation/anticipation or craving stage of the addiction cycle has long been hypothesized to be a key element of relapse in humans and defines addiction as a chronic relapsing disorder. Although often linked to the construct of craving, craving taʻitasi has been difficult to measure clinically (Tiffany 'ua al, 2000) and often does not correlate well with relapse. Nevertheless, the stage of the addiction cycle in which the individual reinstates drug-seeking behavior after abstinence remains a challenging focus for neurobiological mechanisms and medications development for treatment. Animal models of craving can be divided into two domains: drug-seeking induced by drug or stimuli paired with drug-taking, and drug-seeking induced by an acute stressor or a residual negative emotional state, often a state of stress, termed protracted abstinence (see Transition to addiction: patterns of drug-taking, animal models section).

Much evidence from animal studies suggests that drug-induced reinstatement is localized to the medial prefrontal cortex/nucleus accumbens/ventral pallidum circuit mediated by the neurotransmitter glutamate (McFarland ma Kalivas, 2001). In contrast, cue-induced reinstatement appears to involve the basolateral amygdala as a critical substrate with a possible feed-forward mechanism through the prefrontal cortex system involved in drug-induced reinstatement (Everitt ma Wolf, 2002; Weiss 'ua al, 2001). The association of previously neutral stimuli paired with precipitated opioid withdrawal (conditioned withdrawal) also depends critically on the basolateral amygdala (Schulteis 'ua al, 2000), and such stimuli may have motivational significance (Kenny 'ua al, 2006). Neurocircuitry changes associated with drug- and cue-induced reinstatement after extinction have been linked to a glutamatergic pathway from the prefrontal cortex to the nucleus accumbens core, the dopamine projection from the VTA to the medial prefrontal cortex, and the GABA projection from the nucleus accumbens to the ventral pallidum (Kalivas ma O'Brien, 2008).

In contrast, stress-induced reinstatement of drug-related responding in animal models appears to depend on the activation of both CRF and norepinephrine in elements of the extended amygdala (both the CeA and BNST; for reviews, see Shaham 'ua al, 2003; Shalev 'ua al, 2002). Protracted abstinence, largely described in alcohol dependence models, appears to involve overactive glutamatergic and CRF systems, presumably in the extended amygdala, although to a large extent this remains to be explored (de Witte 'ua al, 2005; Valdez 'ua al, 2002).

Human subjects with cocaine addiction show impaired performance in tasks involving attention, cognitive flexibility, and delayed reward discounting that are mediated by the medial and orbital prefrontal cortices, as well as spatial, verbal, and recognition memory impairments that are mediated by the hippocampus, and these deficits can predict poor treatment outcomes (Aharonovich 'ua al, 2006; Bolla 'ua al, 2003). Parallel animal studies of the orbitofrontal, prefrontal cortex, and hippocampus in addiction using animal models have begun to show some of the deficits reflected in human studies. Experimenter-administered cocaine produced impairments in reversal learning (an orbital frontal task) in rats and monkeys (Jentsch 'ua al, 2002; Schoenbaum 'ua al, 2004; Calu 'ua al, 2007). Perhaps even more compelling, animals allowed extended access, but not limited access, to cocaine showed deficits in working memory (a prefrontal-cortex-dependent task), sustained attention task (a prefrontal-cortex-dependent task), and an object recognition task (a hippocampus-dependent task; Briand 'ua al, 2008a, 2008b; George 'ua al, 2008). I se tasi suʻesuʻega (Briand 'ua al, 2008a), these deficits were associated with a significant decrease in dopamine D2 receptor mRNA in the medial and orbital prefrontal cortices, an observation also consistent with human imaging studies. Thus, animal studies using models of compulsive stimulant administration are beginning to show deficits associated with human cocaine addiction (see Human studies: imaging and neuropsychopharmacology).

HUMAN STUDIES: IMAGING AND NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY

As noted above, evidence from preclinical and clinical studies suggests that addiction represents sequential neuroadaptations. As a result, an initial impulsive action turns compulsive and becomes (eventually) chronic and relapsing. Work from imaging studies has provided evidence that this transition involves reprogramming of neuronal circuits that process (1) reward and motivation; (2) memory, conditioning, and habituation; (3) executive function and inhibitory control; (4) interoception and self-awareness; and (5) stress reactivity. This transition is heavily influenced by genetic, developmental, and environmental factors and their dynamic interactions, which will determine the course and severity of the addiction.

Similar to preclinical investigations, distinguishing the three stages in the recurring course of addiction in humans (intoxication, withdrawal, and craving/relapse) has been useful. The following sections describe these stages and some of the relevant neuronal circuits that underlie them.

Binge/Intoxication Stage

Most cases of addiction are initiated by the abuse of substances that are sought because of their hedonic properties. However, drug experimentation also results from the reinforcing effects of conforming to social groups (peer pressure) with the eventual subsequent transfer of motivation to taking the drug for its reinforcing effects. Infrequently, the first use of a drug may be related to its therapeutic properties (such as opiate analgesics for pain or stimulants for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder). As shown by preclinical studies, a key element of the reinforcing effects of drugs is broadly accepted to involve their ability to trigger large increases in extracellular dopamine in limbic regions (including the nucleus accumbens). Although acute drug self-administration is a good animal model for drug intoxication, using animal models to assess the subjective correlates of drug-induced dopamine increases is difficult. Brain imaging studies in humans have been instrumental in showing that drug-induced increases in dopamine in the striatum (including the ventral striatum where the nucleus accumbens is located) are associated with subjective descriptors of reward (eg, pleasure, high, euphoria; Volkow 'ua al, 1996b). Moreover, these studies have shown that fast dopamine changes are associated with the subjective perception of reward, whereas slow and stable dopamine increases do not induce these subjective responses (Grace, 2000; Volkow ma Swanson, 2003).

The pharmacokinetic properties of drugs, which influence the speed of delivery into the brain as well as the duration of their actions, are key elements of their addiction potential. Pharmacokinetic properties determine the doses, routes of administration, and frequency of drug use within a given binge episode. For example, comparison of the brain pharmacokinetics of cocaine and of methamphetamine reveals that both reach the brain very rapidly (although cocaine is somewhat faster than methamphetamine) but that cocaine clears out of the brain much faster than methamphetamine (Ata 3). This difference helps explain why cocaine is taken every 30–60min during a binge, whereas methamphetamine is taken every couple of hours (Fowler 'ua al, 2008). The importance of pharmacokinetics also helps explain why most abused drugs (with the exception of alcohol) are injected, smoked, or snorted. These routes allow for a much faster delivery of the drug to the brain than when taken orally (Volkow 'ua al, 2000). Pharmacokinetics also help explain why stimulant drugs such as methylphenidate or amphetamine, which also increase dopamine, are not typically perceived as reinforcing when taken orally as prescribed therapeutically (Chait, 1994; Volkow 'ua al, 2001b).

Ata 3

Brain images obtained at different times after administration for [11C]-methamphetamine and for [11C]cocaine (n=19 for each drug) showing axial planes at a level that transects the basal ganglia. Note the fast uptake of both drugs in the brain and the ...

Clinical studies have also shown that the expectation of the drug’s effects significantly influences the rewarding responses to drugs, such that the behavioral as well as regional brain activation response of the brain to the drug tends to be more intense when a rewarding drug is expected compared with when the same drug is received unexpectedly (Volkow 'ua al, 2003). The dependency of the drug’s rewarding effects on context and expectation suggests the importance of other neurotransmitters such as glutamate, which modulates the reactivity of dopamine cells and dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens, in the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse (Kalivas ma Volkow, 2005).

Withdrawal/Negative Affect Stage

The response that follows the stage of drug intoxication differs markedly across drugs and is influenced by the chronicity and frequency of its abuse. For some drugs such as opiates, alcohol, and sedative hypnotics, drug discontinuation in chronic drug users can trigger an intense, acute physical withdrawal syndrome that, if not properly managed and when severe, can sometimes be fatal. All drugs of abuse are associated with a motivational withdrawal syndrome characterized by dysphoria, irritability, emotional distress, and sleep disturbances that persist even after protracted withdrawal. The neurobiology of acute withdrawal is distinct from protracted or motivational withdrawal, and both contribute to relapse. Few imaging studies have been carried out during acute withdrawal. One such study that measured changes in dopamine during heroin withdrawal failed to document the dopamine decreases in the nucleus accumbens that had previously been reported with microdialysis in the rodent brain (Wang 'ua al, 1997). From this study, it is unclear whether the results reflect the lack of involvement of striatal dopamine during acute withdrawal in heroin abusers or the limited sensitivity of the positron emission tomography (PET) technology.

The mechanisms underlying acute withdrawal are likely to be drug specific and reflect adaptations in the molecular targets of these drugs. For example, during the first few days of cocaine withdrawal, enhanced sensitivity of the brain to the effects of GABA-enhancing drugs occurs that may reflect the downregulation of this neurotransmitter with chronic cocaine use (Volkow 'ua al, 1998). Similarly, brain imaging studies have also revealed decreases in endogenous opioids during cocaine withdrawal, which may contribute to the irritability, malaise, and dysphoria that occur during this phase of motivational withdrawal (Zubieta 'ua al, 1996).

During protracted withdrawal, once the signs and symptoms of acute withdrawal have subsided, imaging studies have documented hypofunction in dopamine pathways, demonstrated by decreases in D2 receptor expression and decreases in dopamine release, which may contribute to the anhedonia (ie, decreased sensitivity to rewarding stimuli) and amotivation reported by drug-addicted subjects during protracted withdrawal (Volkow 'ua al, 1997b, 2007; Martinez 'ua al, 2004, 2005). The decreased reactivity of dopamine to reinforcing stimuli is also present after protracted withdrawal from alcohol when acute physical withdrawal has subsided. In contrast to the decreased sensitivity to rewards (including drug rewards), imaging studies have reported that during detoxification, enhanced sensitivity to conditioned cues also occurs. Abstinence from smoking, for example, can dramatically potentiate neural responses to smoking-related cues (McClernon 'ua al, 2009). These conditioned responses sustain the cycle of abstinence and relapse that characterizes substance use disorders (Tamaititi 'ua al, 1988).

In addition, imaging studies evaluating markers of brain function have shown that drug abusers tested during protracted detoxification show evidence of disrupted activity of frontal regions, including dorsolateral prefrontal regions, cingulate gyrus, and orbitofrontal cortex, which is hypothesized to underlie their impaired inhibitory control and impulsivity and contribute to relapse (see the following section for discussion).

Preoccupation/Anticipation (Craving) Stage

The enhanced sensitivity to conditioned cues, which include emotional states, triggers the latent preoccupation/anticipation (craving) stage, which is characterized by an increase in drug craving. Indeed, stress is a powerful trigger of relapse to drug-taking behaviors through the activation of brain circuits involved in reward processing and in the attentional and mnemonic bias for drug use reminders (Duncan 'ua al, 2007). This chronic relapse phenomenon is broadly recognized as one of the most challenging problems in fighting drug addiction. Addicted subjects are liable to return to compulsive drug-taking long after experiencing acute withdrawal symptoms (Langleben 'ua al, 2008). The gradual reorganization of reward and memory circuits, brought about by chronic drug abuse, is hypothesized to be crucial to the mounting of these responses. Both dopamine and glutamate have been identified in preclinical studies as contributing to the neuroplastic changes associated with conditioned responses. Moreover, plastic changes in CRF and glucocorticoid receptors likely participate in the enhanced sensitivity to stressors. In humans, the lack of suitable radiotracers to assess glutamate neurotransmission and the lack of ligands for CRF or glucocorticoid receptors have limited the studies of craving mostly to the dopamine system.

NEUROCIRCUITRY DYNAMICS IN THE TRANSITION TO ADDICTION

The neurocircuitry outlined above forms the basis for the neuroplasticity associated with the development of addiction. Summarized below are neuroadaptive changes engaged within the circuits that represent the stages of the addiction cycle outlined above. Five circuits are hypothesized to be engaged in succession, including (1) mesolimbic dopamine system, (2) ventral striatum, (3) ventral striatum/dorsal striatum/thalamus circuits, (4) dorsolateral frontal cortex/inferior frontal cortex/hippocampus circuits, and (5) extended amygdala (Ata 4). The relative weighting and direction of these neuroadaptive changes is illustrated in the circuit diagram of the addicted state (Ata 5).

Ata 4

Schematic drawing describing the sequential and cumulative effects of neuroadaptive changes hypothesized to contribute to the neuroplasticity that promotes compulsive drug-seeking. An early neuroadaptation, common to all drugs of abuse and observed after ...
Ata 5

Neurocircuitry schematic illustrating the combination of neuroadaptations in the brain circuitry for the three stages of the addiction cycle that promote drug-seeking behavior in the addicted state. Note the activation of the ventral striatum/dorsal striatum/extended ...

Mesolimbic Dopamine System: Incentive Salience Pathways, Salience Attribution

One major hypothesis guiding the neuroplasticity associated with addiction is focused on the mesolimbic dopamine system. The hypothesis is that drugs of abuse, particularly cocaine and amphetamine, increase dopamine release in a more prolonged and unregulated manner than natural stimuli, resulting in changes in synaptic plasticity both within the dopamine system and in dopamine-receptive neurons (Wolf, 2002). These changes ultimately usurp normal learning mechanisms to shift neurocircuitry to associations or a form of habit-learning that persists in the face of significant adverse consequences (a component of compulsivity; Everitt ma Wolf, 2002; Hyman 'ua al, 2006).

Animal models of behavioral sensitization have focused largely on the increased locomotor-activating effects of psychomotor stimulant drugs in animals with a history of stimulant exposure. Such studies have revealed a rich neuroplasticity associated with mesolimbic dopamine systems and its terminal projection to the ventral striatum (where the nucleus accumbens is located). Drugs of abuse induce short- and long-term modifications of firing of dopamine neurons in the VTA (Bonci 'ua al, 2003). Studies have shown that burst firing of dopamine neurons in the VTA appears to be correlated with an orienting response to a sensory stimulus (Freeman 'ua al, 1985). A single i vivo exposure to cocaine or amphetamine induces long-term potentiation (LTP) of AMPA-mediated excitatory neurotransmission in dopamine neurons (Ungless 'ua al, 2001). The potentiation of synaptic AMPA responses has been hypothesized to increase the incidence of burst firing (Jones ma Bonci, 2005). Persistent LTP lasting for 3 months of abstinence was induced in the VTA in rats that actively self-administered cocaine but not in passively injected rats (Chen 'ua al, 2008). Similar effects of induction of LTP of glutamate transmission on dopamine neurons have been observed with morphine and nicotine (Saal 'ua al, 2003).

However, more chronic repeated administration of psychostimulants failed to produce sensitization of mesolimbic dopamine activity as measured by i vivo microdialysis (Maisonneuve 'ua al, 1995). In addition, extended access to cocaine fails to produce locomotor sensitization (Peni-Saara 'ua al, 2004) but does produce a sensitized stereotyped behavior response (Ferrario 'ua al, 2005). Moreover, human cocaine abusers showed attenuated dopamine responses when challenged with a stimulant drug, which is opposite to that predicted by the enhanced sensitization of mesolimbic dopamine activity (Volkow 'ua al, 1997b; Martinez 'ua al, 2007).

Ventral Striatum: Incentive Salience Pathways, Salience Attribution

Another plasticity associated with behavioral sensitization is a persistent potentiation of nucleus accumbens excitatory synapses that is observed after repeated drug exposure followed by an extended drug-free period (Kourrich 'ua al, 2007). Repeated cocaine administration increases glutamate neurotransmission only in rats that showed behavioral sensitization (suʻi 'ua al, 1996). In addition, cocaine-sensitized mice showed an enhancement of LTP in nucleus accumbens slices during withdrawal, presumably reflecting increased activity of glutamatergic activity (Yao 'ua al, 2004). An increased surface-to-intracellular ratio of glutamate-1 receptors (GluR1) has been observed 21 days after the last injection of cocaine, suggesting a slowly developing redistribution of AMPA receptors to the surface of nucleus accumbens neurons, particularly in those lacking GluR2 (Boudreau ma Wolf, 2005; Conrad 'ua al, 2008). The increases in cell-surface AMPA receptors depends on activation of dopamine D1 receptors and subsequent protein kinase A signaling (Chao 'ua al, 2002). Functionally, overexpression of GluR1 in the nucleus accumbens facilitated extinction of cocaine-seeking responses (Sutton 'ua al, 2003) and increased brain stimulation reward thresholds, reflecting decreased reward and possibly decreased motivated behavior (Todtenkopf 'ua al, 2006). However, a single reexposure to cocaine during extended withdrawal produced synaptic depression, which may reflect the enhanced glutamate release during cocaine reexposure (Kourrich 'ua al, 2007). Curiously, the increase in AMPA receptor expression observed with cocaine does not occur in amphetamine-sensitized rats, leading to the hypothesis of different functional effects of glutamate projections to the nucleus accumbens during cocaine vs amphetamine withdrawal (Nelson 'ua al, 2009).

Consistent with the results of altered glutamate neurotransmission in cocaine-sensitized rats, microdialysis and microinjection studies have shown that following chronic cocaine, decreased basal release of glutamate occurs but sensitized synaptic glutamate release during reinstatement of extinguished drug-seeking in rats (Kalivas ma O'Brien, 2008; McFarland 'ua al, 2003). This glutamate dysregulation has been hypothesized to be caused by decreased function of the cystine–glutamate exchanger (faifalaoa 'ua al, 2003) and desensitization of the metabotropic glutamate mGlu2/3 receptor. Lower basal levels of glutamate, combined with increased release of synaptic glutamate from activation of prefrontal cortex afferents to the nucleus accumbens, are hypothesized to result in a drive to engage in drug-seeking (Kalivas, 2004).

These long-lasting synaptic effects produce both a decrease in glutamate neurotransmission during chronic administration of the drug and a persistent increase in the efficacy of glutamatergic synaptic neurotransmission during reinstatement following withdrawal. These dynamic changes may promote cellular excitation, which has been hypothesized to be an important substrate for sensitization and drug-related learning in the addictive state (Kauer ma Malenka, 2007; luko 'ua al, 2004).

As previously suggested by animal models, the magnitude of striatal dopamine release (particularly in its ventral aspect) in humans correlates positively with the hedonic response to most drugs of abuse, including amphetamine (Drevets 'ua al, 2001), cocaine (Volkow 'ua al, 1997a), methylphenidate (Volkow 'ua al, 2002), ma le nicotine (Sharma and Brody, 2009). The drug-dependent, fast, and supraphysiological increases in dopamine are likely to mimic the dopamine changes induced by the phasic dopamine cell firing that occurs in response to salient stimuli, thus categorizing the drug experience as one that is highly salient, an experiential outcome that commands attention and promotes arousal, conditioned learning, and motivation (Volkow 'ua al, 2004b). On the basis of findings in laboratory animals, the frequent exposure to these drug responses in drug abusers is postulated to result in the recalibration of dopamine-activating (reward) thresholds for natural reinforcers.

Thus, one can envision the development of a change in firing in mesolimbic dopamine neurons that begins with one administration of the drug, develops into LTP first in the VTA then nucleus accumbens, and via feedback loops subsequently engages the dorsal striatum. Moreover, long-term changes in the CeA and medial prefrontal cortex may follow, and combined with dysregulation of the brain stress systems (see below) may provide a powerful drive for drug-seeking behavior even months after drug withdrawal (Ata 4 ma ma55).

Ventral Striatum/Dorsal Striatum/Thalamus: Voluntary to Habitual Drug-Seeking

The hypothesis that dorsal striatal circuitry has a key role in the development of habitual compulsive cocaine use is supported by data showing the importance for the dorsal striatum in stimulus–response habit learning (Yin 'ua al, 2005) and microdialysis studies showing that prolonged cocaine-seeking increased dopamine release in the dorsal striatum but not ventral striatum (Ito 'ua al, 2002). Disconnection of the ventral striatum from the dorsal striatum in rats self-administering cocaine on a second-order schedule only showed a deficit in animals with well-established ‘compulsive’ intake but not in animals that recently acquired the second-order schedule (Belin ma Everitt, 2008). Thus, the hypothesis is that drug addiction represents changes in associative structures to become automatic or habitual and involves a gradual engagement of dorsal striatal mechanisms.

Animal studies have strongly suggested that with repeated drug exposure neutral stimuli that are associated with the drug can eventually acquire the ability to increase dopamine by themselves. Brain imaging studies confirmed this in addicted humans (Volkow 'ua al, 2008a; Heinz 'ua al, 2004). These studies showed that drug-associated cues induced dopamine increases in the dorsal striatum (caudate and putamen), an effect that correlated with self-reports of craving. The fact that the magnitude of the dopamine increases triggered by the cues was associated with the degree of addiction severity highlights the importance of these conditioned dopamine responses in the process of drug addiction in humans.

Clinical studies have also shown that the striatal slow dopamine increases induced by acute administration of oral methylphenidate do not elicit craving in cocaine abusers unless they are coupled to drug-associated cues (Volkow 'ua al, 2008a). This most likely reflects the fact that the craving results from fast dopamine increases achieved with phasic dopamine firing, as opposed to slow dopamine increases achieved with tonic dopamine firing and in the experiment with oral methylphenidate. In fact, intravenous administration of methylphenidate, which results in fast dopamine increases, induces intense craving.

Brain imaging studies have also shown that, in drug-addicted subjects, these processes involve the orbitofrontal cortex, a brain region implicated in salience attribution and motivation, disruption of which results in compulsivity, and is a brain region with heavy projections to the dorsal striatum. The cingulate gyrus is also involved and is a brain region implicated in inhibitory control and conflict resolution, disruption of which results in impulsivity (Volkow 'ua al, 2004b). Moreover, in cocaine-addicted, but not nonaddicted, subjects, the intravenous administration of methylphenidate, which cocaine abusers report has effects similar to those of cocaine, activated the orbital and medial prefrontal cortices, and this activation was associated with cocaine craving (Volkow 'ua al, 2005). Similarly, in marijuana-addicted subjects, but not in nonaddicted individuals, acute administration of Δ9-THC activated the obitofrontal cortex (Volkow 'ua al, 1996a). Activation of the obitofrontal cortex and cingulate gyrus is also triggered by conditioned cues that predict reward and trigger craving (McClernon 'ua al, 2009). Interestingly, these are regions that regulate dopamine cell firing and release, which have been postulated to be necessary for the enhanced incentive motivational values of drugs in addicted individuals (mirroring a hypothesis based on animal studies; Volkow 'ua al, 1999). When combined, these observations strongly suggest that the dopamine increases associated with conditioned cues are not primary responses, but rather the result of feedback stimulation of dopamine cells, most likely glutamatergic afferents from the prefrontal cortex and/or amygdala. On the basis of these findings, the activation of the obitofrontal cortex, with concomitant increases in dopamine produced by the drug, has been hypothesized to contribute to the compulsive drug consumption that characterizes drug bingeing in addicted individuals (Volkow 'ua al, 2007).

Indeed, human neuroimaging studies show that the prefrontal cortex (orbitofrontal, medial prefrontal, prelimbic/cingulate) and the basolateral amygdala are critical in drug- and cue-induced craving in humans (Franklin 'ua al, 2007). In prefrontal regions (eg, cingulate gyrus and obitofrontal cortex), these changes have been associated with a reduction in striatal dopamine D2 receptor availability observed in addicted subjects (Heinz 'ua al, 2004; Volkow 'ua al, 1993, 2001a, 2007). These associations could either reflect a disruption of frontal brain regions secondary to changes in striatal dopamine activity, or alternatively they could reflect a primary disruption of frontal regions that regulate dopamine cell activity. Indeed, a recent PET study provided evidence that prefrontal brain regions regulate the value of rewards by modulating dopamine increases in the ventral striatum, a regulatory mechanism that becomes dysfunctional in addicted individuals (Volkow 'ua al, 2007).

Thus, concomitant dopamine and glutamate neurotransmission in the dorsal striatum, a region implicated in habit learning and action initiation, is involved in cue/context-dependent craving. As such, the dorsal striatum may be a fundamental component of addiction (Volkow 'ua al, 2006). Research on novel strategies to inhibit cue-conditioned dopamine and glutamate responses is a major focus of current medications development efforts.

The thalamus has not been studied as extensively in the context of addiction. However, because of its integrative function in the regulation of arousal and attentional modulation, this region has been increasingly implicated in the addiction process. For example, intravenous administration of a stimulant drug in cocaine abusers, but not in controls, increased dopamine neurotransmission in the thalamus, an effect associated with craving (Volkow 'ua al, 1997a). In contrast, compared with controls, cocaine abusers show hypoactivation of the thalamus, possibly reflecting noradrenergic and/or dopaminergic deficits, when performing a cognitive task (Tomasi 'ua al, 2007b). Similarly, the thalamus was reported to show attenuated activation while performing a visual cognitive task in smokers exposed to nicotine (Sharma and Brody, 2009). These results suggest that thalamic abnormalities in cocaine abusers may contribute not only to impairments in sensory processing and attention but also to craving. Interestingly, changes in dopamine transmission in the thalamus and striatum appear to be involved in the deterioration of cognitive performance (eg, visual attention and working memory) that inexorably follows a period of sleep deprivation (Volkow 'ua al, 2008b). Thus, more research that builds upon the available preliminary data is warranted.

Dorsolateral Frontal Cortex, Inferior Frontal Cortex, Hippocampus: Cognitive Control, Delayed Gratification, and Memory

Addiction also entails perturbations in cortically regulated cognitive and emotional processes, which cause the overvaluing of drug reinforcers at the expense of the undervaluing of natural reinforcers, and deficits in inhibitory control of drug responses (Goldstein ma Volkow, 2002). As a result, an underperforming prefrontal system is widely believed to be crucial to the addiction process.

One of the components in such a system is impulse control, which is among the most robust cognitive risk factors for substance use disorders. Cocaine appears to have a direct effect on the neurobiology underlying impulse control. After an intravenous injection of cocaine, cocaine users actually showed an improvement in a motor response inhibition task and concomitant increased activation in their right dorsolateral and inferior frontal cortices (Garavan 'ua al, 2008). Because these areas are considered to be important in impulse control, this observation suggests that some of the acute effects of cocaine could in fact mediate a transient reversal of the chronic hypofunction in impulse control circuitry.

Another important function that resides in frontocortical areas is the ability to choose between small and immediate rewards compared to large but deferred rewards, which can be measured using a delayed discounting task. A recent study found that both the dorsolateral and inferolateral frontal cortex gray matter volumes inversely correlated with preference for immediate gratification during decision-making (Bjork 'ua al, 2009). This finding suggests that abnormalities in frontocortical regions may underlie the inability to delay gratification, a trait that is characteristic of addiction and other psychiatric disorders.

The neural substrates of memory and conditioned learning are among the major circuits undergoing aberrant neuroadaptations in response to chronic drug exposure (Volkow 'ua al, 2004a). Different memory systems have been proposed to be involved in drug addiction, including conditioned-incentive learning (via the nucleus accumbens and amygdala), habit learning (via the caudate and putamen), and declarative memory (via the hippocampus; White, 1996), which is the focus of this section.

Over the past decade, many provocative animal studies have suggested that addictive drugs can disrupt neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus (Canales, 2007). Damage to the ventral subiculum of the hippocampus was shown to affect cocaine self-administration in rats (Caine 'ua al, 2001). Such observations have provided insights into the possible involvement of a dysregulated hippocampus in human addiction. This hypothesis is an extension of current knowledge because the hippocampus is broadly viewed as important in contextual conditioning, namely in the processing of contextual cues by which memories can be accessed and retrieved. In fact, declarative memory has been long recognized to be involved in learning and the linking of affective conditions or circumstances with drug-taking experiences. Studies with PET and functional magnetic resonance imaging have shown that cue-elicited craving, as well as acute intoxication, activates the hippocampus and amygdala (Volkow 'ua al, 2004a). For example, the craving that cocaine users experience while exposed to drug-related stimuli is accompanied by blood flow increases in a distributed region implicated in several forms of memory, including the amygdala (Tamaititi 'ua al, 1999; Grant 'ua al, 1996; Kilts 'ua al, 2001) and hippocampus (Kilts 'ua al, 2001).

Therefore, new approaches to disrupt memory reconsolidation may help erode the strong associations between context and drug (Lee, 2008; Lee 'ua al, 2005). E malie, β-blockers have already shown a promising capacity to inhibit conditioned responses to both natural reinforcers and aversive stimuli (Miranda 'ua al, 2003). Moreover, results from a more recent study suggest that drug-induced conditioned responses may also be sensitive to β-blockade treatment (Milton 'ua al, 2008). Similarly, further research on GABA-enhancing drugs also seems warranted. GABAergic stimulation, which can attenuate Pavlovian conditioning, appears to disrupt the response to drugs of abuse in animals (Volkow 'ua al, 2004a) and may be a useful strategy to treat addiction in humans (Dewey 'ua al, 1998).

Extended Amygdala: Negative Reinforcement Pathways

Compulsive drug use defined by increased intake of drug with extended access is accompanied by a chronic perturbation in brain reward homeostasis using measures of brain stimulation reward thresholds. The differential exposure to drug self-administration has dramatic effects on reward thresholds that progressively increase (ie, decreased reward) in extended-access, but not in limited-access, rats across successive self-administration sessions (Ahmed 'ua al, 2002; Kenny 'ua al, 2006; Wee 'ua al, unpublished results). Animals with extended access to cocaine are more sensitive to the blockade of self-administration by dopamine antagonists and partial agonists (Ahmed ma Koob, 2004; Matou 'ua al, 2007), and the opioid partial agonist buprenorphine dose dependently decreased heroin self-administration in extended-access, opioid-dependent rats (Chen 'ua al, 2006b), suggesting that reversal of reward deficits can blunt the motivational drives of drug addiction. This mechanism could underlie the benefit of methadone and buprenorphine treatment in heroin addiction.

As noted above, CRF antagonists blocked the anxiogenic- and aversive-like effects of drug withdrawal, and withdrawal from all drugs of abuse activated CRF in the CeA. These observations led to the hypothesis that activation of CRF, specifically extrahypothalamic CRF in the CeA, contributed to the motivational state driving compulsivity from the negative reinforcement perspective (Koob ma Le Moal, 2008). Thus, one would predict that blockade of the brain stress systems in animal models of extended access to drugs may block the motivation for excessive drug intake. CRF antagonists selectively blocked the increased self-administration of drugs associated with extended access to intravenous self-administration of cocaine, nicotine (Koob, 2008), heroin (Greenwell 'ua al, 2009), ma le ava malosi (Koob, 2008). A particularly dramatic example of the motivational effects of CRF in the extended amygdala in dependence can be observed in animal models of ethanol self-administration in dependent animals in which a CRF1/2 peptide antagonist injected into the amygdala blocked the increase in ethanol self-administration during withdrawal (Funk 'ua al, 2006; Koob, 2008).

Although less well developed, evidence suggests involvement of norepinephrine systems in the extended amygdala in the negative motivational state and increased self-administration associated with dependence (Koob, 2009b). Consistent with the role of the dynorphin-κ opioid system in the aversive effects of drug withdrawal, a κ-opioid antagonist blocked the excessive drinking associated with ethanol withdrawal in dependent rats and selectively blocked the increased progressive-ratio performance in rats with extended access to cocaine (Koob, 2009b; Matou 'ua al, 2009).

Neuropeptide Y has dramatic anxiolytic-like properties localized to the amygdala and has been hypothesized to have effects opposite to CRF in the negative motivational state of withdrawal from drugs of abuse (Heilig 'ua al, 1994; Heilig ma Koob, 2007). NPY administered intracerebroventricularly blocked the increased drug intake associated with ethanol dependence (Thorsell 'ua al, 2005a, 2005b). Injection of NPY into the CeA (Gilpin 'ua al, 2008) and viral vector-enhanced expression of NPY in the CeA also blocked the increased drug intake associated with ethanol dependence (Thorsell 'ua al, 2007).

Thus, the CRF increases in the CeA that occur with acute withdrawal from drugs have motivational significance not only for the anxiety/aversive-like effects of acute withdrawal but also for the increased drug intake associated with dependence. Acute withdrawal also may increase the release of norepinephrine in the BNST and dynorphin in the nucleus accumbens, both of which may contribute to the negative emotional state associated with dependence. Decreased activity of NPY in the CeA also may contribute to the anxiety-like state associated with ethanol dependence. Activation of brain stress systems (CRF, norepinephrine, dynorphin), combined with inactivation of brain antistress systems (NPY) in the extended amygdala may elicit powerful emotional dysregulation with motivational significance to addiction. A number of other neurotransmitter systems have been hypothesized to modulate the extended amygdala both from the stress-induction domain (vasopressin, substance P, orexin) and the antistress domain (nociceptin, endocannabinoids; for review, see Koob, 2008). Such dysregulation may be a significant contribution to the between-system opponent processes that help maintain dependence and also sets the stage for more prolonged state changes in emotionality such as protracted abstinence.

Research on negative reinforcement mechanisms in human addiction has been very limited. With cocaine, for example, the amygdala and lateral orbitofrontal cortex were shown to be activated by unexpected but not expected cocaine infusions in active cocaine abusers (Kufahl 'ua al, 2008), but cocaine abstinence was associated with large reductions in the activity of dopamine projection regions, including the amygdala (Tomasi 'ua al, 2007a). In apparent contrast, smoking abstinence was associated with increased cerebral blood flow in the extended amygdala, among other regions (Wang 'ua al, 2007), whereas a nasal nicotine spray reduced regional cerebral blood flow in the right amygdala and left anterior temporal cortex of habitual smokers subjected to 12h of smoking deprivation (Zubieta 'ua al, 2001).

The amygdala may be equally important for processing positive reward (Murray, 2007) and reward expectancy (Holani ma Gallagher, 2004), similar to processing negative reward. Particularly interesting in the context of brain imaging research will be to understand the function of the amygdala in generating the anxiety and negative emotion frequently seen during abstinence.

A recent report highlighted the importance in addiction of the interoceptive circuit that most likely interfaces with the extended amygdala and ventral striatum. The study showed that smokers with damage to their insula (but not smokers with extrainsular lesions) were able to stop smoking easily and without experiencing either cravings or relapse (Naqvi 'ua al, 2007). The insula, particularly its more anterior regions, is reciprocally connected to several limbic regions (eg, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and ventral striatum) and appears to have an interoceptive function, integrating the autonomic and visceral information with emotion and motivation and providing conscious awareness of these urges (Naqvi ma Bechara, 2009). Indeed, brain lesion studies suggest that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and insula are necessary components of the distributed circuits that support emotional decision-making (Clark 'ua al, 2008). Consistent with this hypothesis, many imaging studies show differential activation in the insula during craving (Naqvi ma Bechara, 2009). The reactivity of this brain region has been suggested to serve as a biomarker to help predict relapse.

MOLECULAR TARGETS FOR NEUROPLASTICITY: BINGE/INTOXICATION, WITHDRAWAL/NEGATIVE AFFECT, AND PREOCCUPATION/ANTICIPATION (CRAVING)

The focus of the present review is on the neurocircuitry of addiction. However, parallel to the neuroplasticity of the neurocircuitry are the molecular changes that occur in these same structures. Chronic exposure to opiates and cocaine leads to activation of cyclic adenosine monophosphate response-element binding protein (CREB) in the nucleus accumbens and CeA (Shaw-Lutchman 'ua al, 2002; Edwards 'ua al, 2007). CREB can be phosphorylated by protein kinase A and by protein kinase regulated by growth factors, putting it at a point of convergence for several intracellular messenger pathways that can regulate gene expression. Activation of CREB in the nucleus accumbens with psychostimulant drugs is linked to the motivational symptoms of psychostimulant withdrawal, such as dysphoria, possibly through induction of the opioid peptide dynorphin, which binds to κ-opioid receptors and has been hypothesized to represent a mechanism of motivational tolerance and dependence (Nestler, 2005). Repeated CREB activation promotes dynorphin expression in the nucleus accumbens, which in turn decreases dopaminergic activity, both of which can contribute to negative emotional states. Extracellular signal-regulated kinase is another key element of intracellular signaling considered a key component in the plasticity associated with repeated administration of cocaine, specifically behavioral sensitization, cocaine reward, and time-dependent increases in cocaine-seeking after withdrawal (ie, incubation effect; Lu 'ua al, 2006; Li 'ua al, 2008).

Another molecular target for regulating the plasticity that leads to addiction is dysregulation of cystine–glutamate exchange, which is hypothesized to promote pathological glutamate signaling related to several components of the addiction cycle. Here, repeated administration of cocaine blunts cystine–glutamate exchange, leading to reduced basal and increased cocaine-induced glutamate in the nucleus accumbens that persists for at least 3 weeks after the last cocaine treatment (faifalaoa 'ua al, 2003). Most compelling is the observation that treatment with N-acetylcysteine, by activating cystine–glutamate exchange, prevented cocaine-induced escalation and behavioral sensitization, restored the ability to induce LTP and long-term depression in the nucleus accumbens, and blunted reinstatement in animals and conditioned reactivity to drug cues in humans (Moussawi 'ua al, 2009; LaRowe 'ua al, 2007; Madayag 'ua al, 2007).

CREB and other intracellular messengers can activate transcription factors, which can change gene expression and produce long-term changes in protein expression, and, as a result, neuronal function. Although acute administration of drugs of abuse can cause a rapid (within hours) activation of members of the Fos protein family, such as c-fos, FosB, Fra-1, and Fra-2 in the nucleus accumbens, other transcription factors, isoforms of ΔFosB, a highly stable form of FosB, have been shown to accumulate over longer periods of time (days) with repeated drug administration (Nestler, 2005). Animals with activated ΔFosB have exaggerated sensitivity to the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse, and ΔFosB may be a sustained molecular ‘switch’ that helps to initiate and maintain a state of addiction (McClung 'ua al, 2004). Whether (and how) such transcription factors influence the function of the brain stress systems, such as CRF and those described above, remains to be determined.

TULAGA MA FAAIUGA

In summary, multiple brain regions and circuits are disrupted in drug addiction and are likely to contribute differentially to the complex phenotype observed in addicted individuals (Ata 5). Although some of these functional abnormalities may be present to a greater or lesser extent across all classes of drug addictions, some of the changes may be specific to certain types of drugs. For example, long-lasting decrements in the DAT in the striatum are observed in methamphetamine but not in alcohol or cocaine addictions. Conversely, decrements in dopamine D2 receptors in the striatum are observed in subjects addicted to all of the drugs of abuse that have been investigated, and increased activation of brain stress systems such as CRF has been observed in animal models during acute withdrawal for all types of drugs. Importantly, the neuronal abnormalities that become manifest in an addicted individual and that can be uncovered by imaging and/or neuropsychopharmacological studies are a reflection of not only a given chronic drug exposure trajectory, but also an individual’s specific constellations of genetic, developmental, and environmental characteristics.

FESOASOANI MO SUʻESUʻEGA LUA

The advances outlined above point the way to future directions for research in the neurocircuitry of addiction in the same conceptual framework of binge/intoxication, withdrawal/negative affect, and preoccupation/anticipation. The rich resources of modern neurosciences applied to the neurobiology of addiction offer an opportunity to not only understand the neurocircuitry of the addiction process but also to provide the keys to understanding vulnerability and providing treatment for this devastating disease.

In the binge/intoxication stage of the addiction cycle, how neuroplasticity that begins with a change in firing in mesolimbic dopamine neurons during initial drug exposure is translated to engagement of the dorsal striatum, disruption of frontal system function, and recruitment of brain stress systems and results in a residual powerful drive for drug-seeking behavior even months after withdrawal remains to be determined. For example, what is the relationship between vulnerability to impulsivity and subsequent compulsivity in the neuroplasticity of the circuits described above? Such future studies may involve molecular genetic approaches that range from selective breeding to upregulation or knockdown of molecular mechanisms within specific brain circuits using short-hairpin RNA technology.

In the withdrawal/negative affect stage, engagement of the brain stress systems, such as CRF, in animal models needs to be extended to other interactive brain stress systems and explored in human studies. Numerous other neurotransmitter systems that interact with the brain stress system are only now being explored, such as dynorphin, NPY, substance P, nociceptin, and orexin. Virtually unexplored at this stage are human imaging studies of this component of the addiction cycle and human imaging of brain neurotransmitter systems implicated in motivational aspects of drug withdrawal. The development of novel radioactive ligands for human imaging studies that bind to the receptors of the above neurotransmitter systems would be a great boost to the field.

In the preoccupation/anticipation stage, human neuroimaging studies show that the prefrontal cortex (orbitofrontal, medial prefrontal, prelimbic/cingulate) and the basolateral amygdala are critical in drug- and cue-induced craving. Whether such associations reflect a disruption of frontal brain regions secondary to changes in striatal dopamine activity, or alternatively reflect a primary disruption of frontal regions that regulate dopamine cell activity, remains to be determined. New approaches to the study of memory reconsolidation may help elucidate the strong associations between context and drug. The importance in addiction of the interoceptive circuit involving the insula and other regions that most likely interface with the extended amygdala and ventral striatum remains to be determined. The reactivity of these brain circuits may serve as a biomarker to help predict relapse and help predict treatment efficacy. Human post-mortem studies, human laboratory studies, and neurocircuitry studies in parallel animal models will likely yield promising results in this domain.

Finally, molecular and genetic changes that convey the changes in activity of the neurocircuits in all three stages of the addiction cycle described above are only now being elucidated. Changes in transmitter regulatory systems, transcription factors, and even gene regulation at the epigenetic level may explain how circuits are dysregulated, stay dysregulated, and provide vulnerability to dysregulation initially or long into abstinence. Ultimately, neurobiological targets elucidated through the framework of the neurocircuitry of addiction will provide targets for identifying genetic vulnerability in the human population, and genetic vulnerability in the human studies may identify novel targets to be explored at the mechanistic level in animal studies.

tautinoga

This is publication number 20084 from The Scripps Research Institute. Preparation of this work was supported by the Pearson Center for Alcoholism and Addiction Research and National Institutes of Health grants AA12602, AA08459, and AA06420 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; DA04043, DA04398, and DA10072 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse; DK26741 from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases; and 17RT-0095 from the Tobacco-Related Disease Research Program from the State of California. We thank Michael Arends and Ruben Baler for their assistance with paper preparation.

Faamatalaga Faʻamatalaga

TALANOAGA

O tusitala e leai se feteenaiga e fiafia i ai.

mau faasino

  • Aharonovich E, Hasin DS, Brooks AC, Liu X, Bisaga A, Nunes EV. O le faaletonu o le mafaufau e valoia le maualalo o togafitiga i tagata gasegase e faalagolago i cocaine. Fualaau oona. 2006;81: 313-322. [PubMed]
  • Ahmed SH, Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Neurobiological faʻamaoniga mo hedonic allostasis e fesoʻotaʻi ma le faʻateleina o le faʻaaogaina o cocaine. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5: 625-626. [PubMed]
  • Ahmed SH, Koob GF. 1998. Suiga mai le feololo i le tele o fualaau faasaina: suiga i le tulaga fa'atulagaina o le hedonic saienisi 282298–300.300This study showed that rats given extended access to cocaine escalate intake and show behavior consistent with an increase in hedonic set point (lower reward) for the drug. [PubMed]
  • Ahmed SH, Koob GF. Changes in response to a dopamine antagonist in rats with escalating cocaine intake. Psychopharmacology. 2004;172: 450-454. [PubMed]
  • Ahmed SH, Walker JR, Koob GF. Persistent increase in the motivation to take heroin in rats with a history of drug escalation. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22: 413-421. [PubMed]
  • Alheid GF, De Olmos JS, Beltramino CA. 1995. Amygdala and extended amygdalaIn: Paxinos G (ed).The Rat Nervous System Academic Press: San Diego; 495–578.578.
  • Allen TJ, Moeller FG, Rhoades HM, Cherek DR. Impulsivity and history of drug dependence. Fualaau oona. 1998;50: 137-145. [PubMed]
  • American Psychiatric Association 1994. Fuainumera Faʻailoga ma Fuainumera Faʻamaumauga o Manatu o Mafaufauga4th edn.American Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC.
  • American Psychiatric Association 2000. Fuainumera Faʻailoga ma Fuainumera Faʻamaumauga o Manatu o Mafaufauga4th edn, text revisionAmerican Psychiatric Press: Washington, DC.
  • Arroyo M, Markou A, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Acquisition, maintenance and reinstatement of intravenous cocaine self-administration under a second-order schedule of reinforcement in rats: effects of conditioned cues and continuous access to cocaine. Psychopharmacology. 1998;140: 331-344. [PubMed]
  • Baker DA, McFarland K, Lake RW, Shen H, Tang XC, Toda S, et al. Neuroadaptations i le cystine-glutamate exchange e mafua ai le toe faʻafoʻi o cocaine. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6: 743-749. [PubMed]
  • Baker TB, Morse E, Sherman JE. 1987. The motivation to use drugs: a psychobiological analysis of urgesIn: River PC (ed).Alcohol and Addictive Behavior(series title: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol 34).University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, NE; 257–323.323.
  • Baldwin HA, Rassnick S, Rivier J, Koob GF, Britton KT. CRF antagonist reverses the ‘anxiogenic’ response to ethanol withdrawal in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1991;103: 227-232. [PubMed]
  • Barr AM, Phillips AG. Aveesea ina ua uma ona faʻaalia i d-amphetamine ua faaitiitia le tali atu mo se vaifofo o le suauu e fuaina i se faasologa o faasologa o faasologa o le faamalosia. Psychopharmacology. 1999;141: 99-106. [PubMed]
  • Belin D, Everitt BJ. 2008. Cocaine saili masani e faʻalagolago i luga o le fesoʻotaʻiga o le fesoʻotaiga o le dopamine-faʻalagolago i le faʻalauteleina o le 'auʻaunaga ma le taʻavale Neuron 57432–441.441This study showed that the interactions between the ventral and dorsal striatum are critical for the development of compulsive-like cocaine-seeking behavior. [PubMed]
  • Ben-Shahar O, Ahmed SH, Koob GF, Ettenberg A. The transition from controlled to compulsive drug use is associated with a loss of sensitization. Brain Res. 2004;995: 46-54. [PubMed]
  • Bjork JM, Momenan R, Hommer DW. Delay discounting correlates with proportional lateral frontal cortex volumes. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65: 710-713. [PubMed]
  • Bolla KI, Eldreth DA, London ED, Kiehl KA, Mouratidis M, Contoreggi C, et al. Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a decision-making task. Neuroimage. 2003;19: 1085-1094. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Bonci A, Bernardi G, Grillner P, Mercuri NB. The dopamine-containing neuron: maestro or simple musician in the orchestra of addiction. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2003;24: 172-177. [PubMed]
  • Boudreau AC, Wolf ME. O le faʻalauteleina o le tino i le cocaine e fesoʻotaʻi ma le faʻateleina o le faʻaaliga o le tali a le AMPA i totonu o le nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2005;25: 9144-9151. [PubMed]
  • Briand LA, Flagel SB, Garcia-Fuster MJ, Watson SJ, Akil H, Sarter M, et al. Persistent alterations in cognitive function and prefrontal dopamine D2 receptors following extended, but not limited, access to self-administered cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008a;33: 2969-2980. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Briand LA, Gross JP, Robinson TE. Impaired object recognition following prolonged withdrawal from extended-access cocaine self-administration. Neuroscience. 2008b;155: 1-6. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Caine SB, Heinrichs SC, Coffin VL, Koob GF. Effects of the dopamine D-1 antagonist SCH 23390 microinjected into the accumbens, amygdala or striatum on cocaine self-administration in the rat. Brain Res. 1995;692: 47-56. [PubMed]
  • Caine SB, Humby T, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Behavioral effects of psychomotor stimulants in rats with dorsal or ventral subiculum lesions: locomotion, cocaine self-administration, and prepulse inhibition of startle. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115: 880-894. [PubMed]
  • Caine SB, Thomsen M, Gabriel KI, Berkowitz JS, Gold LH, Koob GF, et al. Lack of self-administration of cocaine in dopamine D1 receptor knock-out mice. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 13140-13150. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Calu DJ, Stalnaker TA, Franz TM, Singh T, Shaham Y, Schoenbaum G. Withdrawal from cocaine self-administration produces long-lasting deficits in orbitofrontal-dependent reversal learning in rats. Aoao le Mem. 2007;14: 325-328. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Canales JJ. Adult neurogenesis and the memories of drug addiction. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007;257: 261-270. [PubMed]
  • Chait LD. Reinforcing and subjective effects of methylphenidate in humans. Behav Pharmacol. 1994;5: 281-288. [PubMed]
  • Chao SZ, Ariano MA, Peterson DA, Wolf ME. O le faʻaosofia o le nusipepa D1 dopamine e faʻateleina ai le faʻaaliga o foliga o le GluR1 i laina faʻapipiʻi. J Neurochem. 2002;83: 704-712. [PubMed]
  • Chen BT, Bowers MS, Martin M, Hopf FW, Guillory AM, Carelli RM, et al. O cocaine ae le o se taui faʻanoanoa ile puleaina poʻo le faʻaaogaina o cocaine e maua ai le LTP tumau ile VTA. Neuron. 2008;59: 288-297. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Chen R, Tilley MR, Wei H, Zhou F, Zhou FM, Ching S, et al. Abolished cocaine reward in mice with a cocaine-insensitive dopamine transporter. Faʻaalia Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006a;103: 9333-9338.
  • Chen SA, O’Dell L, Hoefer M, Greenwell TN, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF. 2006b. Unlimited access to heroin self-administration: independent motivational markers of opiate dependence Neuropsychopharmacology 312692–2707.2707(corrigendum: 31: 2802). [PubMed]
  • Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrman R, O’Brien CP. 1988. Classically conditioned responses in opioid and cocaine dependence: a role in relapseIn: Ray BA (ed).Learning Factors in Substance Abuse(series title: NIDA Research Monograph, vol 84).National Institute on Drug Abuse: Rockville, MD; 25–43.43.
  • Childress AR, Mozley PD, McElgin W, Fitzgerald J, Reivich M, O'Brien CP. Le faʻamalosia faʻagata i le taimi o le gaoia o le koko. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156: 11-18. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Clark L, Maea A, Damasio H, Aitken MR, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Eseesega o aʻafiaga o le tino ma le faʻataunuʻu muamua o faʻamaʻi pipisi i luga o le faia o faʻaiʻuga lamatia. Brain. 2008;131: 1311-1322. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Collins RJ, Weeks JR, Cooper MM, Good PI, Russell RR. Prediction of abuse liability of drugs using IV self-administration by rats. Psychopharmacology. 1984;82: 6-13. [PubMed]
  • Conrad KL, Tseng KY, Uejima JL, Reimers JM, Heng LJ, Shaham Y, et al. Formation of accumbens GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors mediates incubation of cocaine craving. Natura. 2008;454: 118-121. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Creese I, Iversen SD. The role of forebrain dopamine systems in amphetamine-induced stereotyped behavior in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 1974;39: 345-357.
  • Crow TJ. Catecholamine-containing neurones and electrical self-stimulation: 2. A theoretical interpretation and some psychiatric implications. Mafaufauga Psychol. 1973;3: 66-73. [PubMed]
  • de Witte P, Littleton J, Parot P, Koob G. Neuroprotective and abstinence-promoting effects of acamprosate: elucidating the mechanism of action. CNS Drugs. 2005;19: 517-537. [PubMed]
  • Delfs JM, Zhu Y, Druhan JP, Aston-Jones G. Noradrenaline i totonu o le ventral forebrain e taua tele mo le faʻaaogaina o le opiate-induced aversion. Natura. 2000;403: 430-434. [PubMed]
  • Deroche-Gamonet V, Belin D, Piazza PV. Molimau mo amioga e fai ma vaisu-faapei o le rat. Saienisi. 2004;305: 1014-1017. [PubMed]
  • Dewey SL, Morgan AE, Ashby CR, Jr, Horan B, Kushner SA, Logan J, et al. A novel strategy for the treatment of cocaine addiction. Synapse. 1998;30: 119-129. [PubMed]
  • Di Chiara G, Imperato A. O fualaau faasaina e tagata e sili ona maualuga le faateleina o le synaptic concentamine concentrations i le mesolimbic system of rats saoloto. Faʻaalia Natl Acad Sci USA. 1988;85: 5274-5278.
  • Drevets WC, Gautier C, Price JC, Kupfer DJ, Kinahan PE, Grace AA, et al. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release in human ventral striatum correlates with euphoria. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49: 81-96. [PubMed]
  • Duncan E, Boshoven W, Harenski K, Fiallos A, Tracy H, Jovanovic T, et al. An fMRI study of the interaction of stress and cocaine cues on cocaine craving in cocaine-dependent men. Am J Pule. 2007;16: 174-182. [PubMed]
  • Dyr W, Kostowski W. Evidence that the amygdala is involved in the inhibitory effects of 5-HT3 receptor antagonists on alcohol drinking in rats. Alu. 1995;12: 387-391. [PubMed]
  • Edwards S, Graham DL, Bachtell RK, Self DW. Region-specific tolerance to cocaine-regulated cAMP-dependent protein phosphorylation following chronic self-administration. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25: 2201-2213. [PubMed]
  • Everitt BJ, Belin D, Economidou D, Pelloux Y, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. Iloiloga. Faʻaaogaina o le aʻafiaga o loʻo aʻafia ai le faʻaletonu e atiae ai le faʻamalosia o togafitiga o fualaau faasaina ma le tagofia o mea ua fai ma vaisu. Phil Trans Royal Soc London B Biol Sci. 2008;363: 3125-3135.
  • Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. 2005. Auala faʻavaomalo o le faʻamalosia mo le tagofiaina o vailaau faʻasaina: mai gaioiga i masaniga faʻamalosi Nat Neurosci 81481–1489.1489(erratum: 9(7): 979). [PubMed]
  • Everitt BJ, Wolf ME. 2002. Psychomotor stimulant addiction: a neural systems perspective J Neurosci 223312–3320.3320(erratum: 22(16): 1a). [PubMed]
  • Ferrario CR, Gorny G, Crombag HS, Li Y, Kolb B, Robinson TE. Neural and behavioral plasticity associated with the transition from controlled to escalated cocaine use. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58: 751-759. [PubMed]
  • Fowler JS, Volkow ND, Logan J, Alexoff D, Telang F, Wang GJ, et al. Fast uptake and long-lasting binding of methamphetamine in the human brain: comparison with cocaine. Neuroimage. 2008;43: 756-763. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Franklin TR, Wang Z, Wang J, Sciortino N, Harper D, Li Y, et al. Le faʻamalosia o le limbic i sikaleti ulaula faʻailoga e tutoatasi mai le faʻamavaeina o le nicotine: o se suʻesuʻega fMRI. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32: 2301-2309. [PubMed]
  • Freeman AS, Meltzer LT, Bunney BS. Firing properties of substantia nigra dopaminergic neurons in freely moving rats. Soifua Sci. 1985;36: 1983-1994. [PubMed]
  • Funk CK, O’Dell LE, Crawford EF, Koob GF. 2006. Corticotropin-releasing factor within the central nucleus of the amygdala mediates enhanced ethanol self-administration in withdrawn, ethanol-dependent rats J Neurosci 2611324–11332.11332This study showed that blockade of CRF receptors in the area of the central nucleus of the amygdala blocks the increased alcohol intake associated with dependence but not alcohol intake in nondependent animals. [PubMed]
  • Garavan H, Kaufman JN, Hester R. Acute effects of cocaine on the neurobiology of cognitive control. Phil Trans Royal Soc London B Biol Sci. 2008;363: 3267-3276.
  • George O, Ghozland S, Azar MR, Cottone P, Zorrilla EP, Parsons LH, et al. CRF-CRF1 fa'agaoioia faiga fa'afeso'ota'i fa'atupu fa'atupu fa'atupu fa'atupula'ia o le pulea e le tagata lava ia o le nicotine i isumu fa'alagolago i le nicotine. Faʻaalia Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104: 17198-17203.
  • George O, Mandyam CD, Wee S, Koob GF. O le fa'alauteleina o le avanoa i le fa'atonuga a le tagata lava ia cocaine e maua ai le fa'aletonu o le manatuaga o mea e fa'alagolago i luma o le pito i luma. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33: 2474-2482. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gilpin NW, Koob GF. Overview: neurobiology of alcohol dependence with a focus on motivational mechanisms. Alcohol Res Health. 2008;31: 185-195. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gilpin NW, Misra K, Koob GF. Neuropeptide Y in the central nucleus of the amygdala suppresses dependence-induced increases in alcohol drinking. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;90: 475-480. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. O vailaau faʻasaina ma lona aʻaʻo mai le neurobiology faavae: faʻamaonia faʻamaoniga mo le aʻafiaga o le paʻu o luma. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159: 1642-1652. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gonzalez D, Riba J, Bouso JC, Gomez-Jarabo G, Barbanoj MJ. Pattern of use and subjective effects of Salvia divinorum among recreational users. Fualaau oona. 2006;85: 157-162. [PubMed]
  • Grace AA. The tonic/phasic model of dopamine system regulation and its implications for understanding alcohol and psychostimulant craving. Togafitiga. 2000;95 (Supl 2:S119–S128. [PubMed]
  • Grant BF, Dawson DA. Age of onset of drug use and its association with DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence: results from the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Fa'atautala Fa'atatau. 1998;10: 163-173. [PubMed]
  • Grant BF, Dawson DA, Stinson FS, Chou SP, Dufour MC, Pickering RP. The 12-month prevalence and trends in DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United States, 1991–1992 and 2001–2002. Fualaau oona. 2004;74: 223-234. [PubMed]
  • Grant S, London ED, Newlin DB, Villemagne VL, Liu X, Contoreggi C, et al. Activation of memory circuits during cue-elicited cocaine craving. Faʻaalia Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93: 12040-12045.
  • Greenwell TN, Funk CK, Cottone P, Richardson HN, Chen SA, Rice K, et al. Corticotropin-releasing factor-1 receptor antagonists decrease heroin self-administration in long-, but not short-access rats. Addict Biol. 2009;14: 130-143. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hand TH, Koob GF, Stinus L, Le Moal M. Aversive properties of opiate receptor blockade: evidence for exclusively central mediation in naive and morphine-dependent rats. Brain Res. 1988;474: 364-368. [PubMed]
  • Hebb DO. 1972. Textbook of Psychology3rd edn.WB Saunders: Philadelphia.
  • Heilig M, Koob GF. O se matafaioi autu mo le corticotropin-faʻamavaeina o vaega i le ava malosi. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30: 399-406. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Heilig M, Koob GF, Ekman R, Britton KT. Corticotropin-releasing factor and neuropeptide Y: role in emotional integration. Trends Neurosci. 1994;17: 80-85. [PubMed]
  • Heimer L, Alheid G. 1991. Piecing together the puzzle of basal forebrain anatomyIn: Napier TC, Kalivas PW, Hanin I (eds).The Basal Forebrain: Anatomy to Function(series title: Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, vol 295).Plenum Press: New York; 1–42.42.
  • Heinrichs SC, Menzaghi F, Schulteis G, Koob GF, Stinus L. O le taofiofia o le corticotropin-releasing factor i le amygdala e faʻaitiitia ai aʻafiaga leaga o le morphine withdrawal. Behav Pharmacol. 1995;6: 74-80. [PubMed]
  • Heinz A, Siessmeier T, Wrase J, Hermann D, Klein S, Grusser SM, et al. 2004. Faamaopoopo le va dopamine MF (2) receptors i le striatum ventral ma gaosi tutotonu o faailoilo o le ava malosi ma le naunau Am J Psychiatry 1611783–1789.1789(erratum: 161: 2344). [PubMed]
  • Hernandez G, Hamdani S, Rajabi H, Conover K, Stewart J, Arvanitogiannis A, et al. Prolonged rewarding stimulation of the rat medial forebrain bundle: neurochemical and behavioral consequences. Behav Neurosci. 2006;120: 888-904. [PubMed]
  • Heyser CJ, Roberts AJ, Schulteis G, Koob GF. O le fa'atonuga tutotonu o se fa'alavelave fa'afefe opiate e fa'aitiitia ai le fa'aogaina e le tagata lava ia o le ethanol i isumu. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1999;23: 1468-1476. [PubMed]
  • Hill RT. 1970. Facilitation of conditioned reinforcement as a mechanism of psychomotor stimulationIn: Cost E, Garattini S (eds).Amphetamines and Related Compounds Raven Press: New York; 781–795.795.
  • Hnasko TS, Sotak BN, Palmiter RD. Morphine reward in dopamine-deficient mice. Natura. 2005;438: 854-857. [PubMed]
  • Holland PC, Gallagher M. Amygdala–frontal interactions and reward expectancy. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14: 148-155. [PubMed]
  • Hubner CB, Koob GF. The ventral pallidum plays a role in mediating cocaine and heroin self-administration in the rat. Brain Res. 1990;508: 20-29. [PubMed]
  • Hyman SE, Malenka RC, Nestler EJ. Faʻaaogaina o mea e fai ma vaisu: o le matafaioi o aʻoaʻoga ma le manatua. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2006;29: 565-598. [PubMed]
  • Hyytia P, Koob GF. GABA-A receptor antagonism in the extended amygdala decreases ethanol self-administration in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1995;283: 151-159. [PubMed]
  • Ito R, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Tuuina atu o le Dopamine i le pito i tua i le taimi o le amio o le cocaine i lalo o le vaavaaiga o se vailaʻau e fesootai i fualaau faasaina. J Neurosci. 2002;22: 6247-6253. [PubMed]
  • Jentsch JD, Olausson P, de la Garza R, II, Taylor JR. Impairments of reversal learning and response perseveration after repeated, intermittent cocaine administrations to monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26: 183-190. [PubMed]
  • Jones S, Bonci A. Synaptic plasticity and drug addiction. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2005;5: 20-25. [PubMed]
  • June HL, Foster KL, McKay PF, Seyoum R, Woods JE, Harvey SC, et al. The reinforcing properties of alcohol are mediated by GABA(A1) receptors in the ventral pallidum. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28: 2124-2137. [PubMed]
  • Justinova Z, Tanda G, Redhi GH, Goldberg SR. Self-administration of delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) by drug naive squirrel monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 2003;169: 135-140. [PubMed]
  • Kalivas PW. Glutamate systems in cocaine addiction. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2004;4: 23-29. [PubMed]
  • Kalivas PW, O'Brien C. Drug o mea ua fai ma vaisu o se togafitiga o le siakiina o le neuroplasticity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33: 166-180. [PubMed]
  • Kalivas PW, Volkow ND. O le faavae o le tagofia o mea ua fai ma vaisu: o se togafitiga o le faaosofia ma le filifiliga. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162: 1403-1413. [PubMed]
  • Kauer JA, Malenka RC. Synaptic plasticity ma vaisu. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8: 844-858. [PubMed]
  • Kelly PH, Iversen SD. Selective 6-OHDA-induced destruction of mesolimbic dopamine neurons: abolition of psychostimulant-induced locomotor activity in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1976;40: 45-56. [PubMed]
  • Kenny PJ, Chen SA, Kitamura O, Markou A, Koob GF. Fa'amuta fa'amuta e fa'aoso ai le taumafaina o le heroin ma fa'aitiitia ai le lagona o taui. J Neurosci. 2006;26: 5894-5900. [PubMed]
  • Kilts CD, Schweitzer JB, Quinn CK, Gross RE, Faber TL, Muhammad F, et al. Neural activity related to drug craving in cocaine addiction. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58: 334-341. [PubMed]
  • Kitamura O, Wee S, Specio SE, Koob GF, Pulvirenti L. Escalation of methamphetamine self-administration in rats: a dose–effect function. Psychopharmacology. 2006;186: 48-53. [PubMed]
  • Koob GF. Fualaau o le faʻaleagaina: anatomy, pharmacology, ma galuega o auala taui. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 1992;13: 177-184. [PubMed]
  • Koob GF. 2004. Allostatic view of motivation: implications for psychopathologyIn: Bevins RA, Bardo MT (eds).Motivational Factors in the Etiology of Drug Abuse(series title: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol 50).University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, NE; 1–18.18.
  • Koob GF. The neurocircuitry of addiction: implications for treatment. Clin Neurosci Res. 2005;5: 89-101.
  • Koob GF. O se matafaioi mo fai'ai fa'amamafa faiga i vaisu. Neuron. 2008;59: 11-34. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Koob GF. Neurobiological substrates mo le itu pogisa o le faʻamalosi i vaisu. Neuropharmacology. 2009a;56 ( Supl 1:18–31 . [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Koob GF. 2009b. Faiga faʻalavelave faʻafaufau i le amygdala ma vaisu Brain Res(i le lomitusi).
  • Koob GF, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. 2008a. Reward, motivation, and addictionIn: Squire LG, Berg D, Bloom FE, Du Lac S, Ghosh A, Spitzer N (eds).Fundamental Neuroscience3rd edn.Academic Press: Amsterdam; 987–1016.1016.
  • Koob GF, Kandel D, Volkow ND. 2008b. Pathophysiology of addictionIn: Tasman A, Kay J, Lieberman JA, First MB, Maj M (eds).Fomaʻi3rd edn, vol 1Wiley: Chichester; 354–378.378.
  • Koob GF, Kreek MJ. Stress, dysregulation of drug reward pathways, and the transition to drug dependence. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164: 1149-1159. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. 1997. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation saienisi 27852–58.58This theoretical review argued that drug addiction involves decreased hedonic homeostatic dysregulation (dysregulation of reward function) driven by both decreased activity in reward pathways and recruitment of brain stress systems. [PubMed]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addictive, dysregulation o taui, ma le allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24: 97-129. [PubMed]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Plasticity o taui neurocircuitry ma le 'itu pogisa' o vaisu fualaau faasaina. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8: 1442-1444. [PubMed]
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Neurobiology o vaisu. Academic Press: London; 2006.
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. Vaʻaia ma le faʻaaogaina ole faiʻai. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59: 29-53. [PubMed]
  • Koob GF, Lloyd GK, Mason BJ. Development of pharmacotherapies for drug addiction: a Rosetta Stone approach. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2009;8: 500-515. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Koob GF, Nestler EJ. Le neurobiology o vaisu fualaau. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997;9: 482-497. [PubMed]
  • Kornetsky C, Bain G. 1990. Brain-stimulation reward: a model for drug induced euphoriaIn: Adler MW, Cowan A (eds).Testing and Evaluation of Drugs of Abuse(series title: Modern Methods in Pharmacology, vol 6).Wiley-Liss: New York; 211–231.231.
  • Kornetsky C, Esposito RU. Euphorigenic drugs: effects on the reward pathways of the brain. Faiga Fafaga. 1979;38: 2473-2476. [PubMed]
  • Kourrich S, Rothwell PE, Klug JR, Thomas MJ. O le siakiina o cocaine e pulea ai le synaptic plasticity in the nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 7921-7928. [PubMed]
  • Kufahl P, Li Z, Risinger R, Rainey C, Piacentine L, Wu G, et al. Expectation modulates human brain responses to acute cocaine: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63: 222-230. [PubMed]
  • Langleben DD, Ruparel K, Elman I, Busch-Winokur S, Pratiwadi R, Loughead J, et al. Acute effect of methadone maintenance dose on brain FMRI response to heroin-related cues. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165: 390-394. [PubMed]
  • LaRowe SD, Myrick H, Hedden S, Mardikian P, Saladin M, McRae A, et al. Is cocaine desire reduced by N-acetylcysteine. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164: 1115-1117. [PubMed]
  • Laviolette SR, Alexson TO, van der Kooy D. Lesions of the tegmental pedunculopontine nucleus block the rewarding effects and reveal the aversive effects of nicotine in the ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci. 2002;22: 8653-8660. [PubMed]
  • Le Doux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23: 155-184. [PubMed]
  • Le Moal M, Simon H. Mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic networking: galuega faatino ma tulafono faatonutonu. Faʻafino Rev. 1991;71: 155-234. [PubMed]
  • Lee JL. Memory reconsolidation mediates the strengthening of memories by additional learning. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11: 1264-1266. [PubMed]
  • Lee JL, Di Ciano P, Thomas KL, Everitt BJ. Disrupting reconsolidation of drug memories reduces cocaine-seeking behavior. Neuron. 2005;47: 795-801. [PubMed]
  • Li YQ, Li FQ, Wang XY, Wu P, Zhao M, Xu CM, et al. Central amygdala extracellular signal-regulated kinase signaling pathway is critical to incubation of opiate craving. J Neurosci. 2008;28: 13248-13257. [PubMed]
  • Logan GD, Schachar RJ, Tannock R. Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psycho Sci. 1997;8: 60-64.
  • Lu L, Koya E, Zhai H, Faamoemoe BT, Shaham Y. Matafaioi a ERK i mea ua fai ma vaisu o kokeni. Trends Neurosci. 2006;29: 695-703. [PubMed]
  • Madayag A, Lobner D, Kau KS, Mantsch JR, Abdulhameed O, Hearing M, et al. Repeated N-acetylcysteine ​​pulega e suia aʻafiaga faʻalagolago i palasitika o cocaine. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 13968-13976. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Maisonneuve IM, Ho A, Kreek MJ. Chronic administration of a cocaine ‘binge’ alters basal extracellular levels in male rats: an i vivo suʻesuʻega microdialysis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1995;272: 652-657. [PubMed]
  • Mameli-Engvall M, Evrard A, Pons S, Maskos U, Svensson TH, Changeux JP, et al. Hierarchical control of dopamine neuron-firing patterns by nicotinic receptors. Neuron. 2006;50: 911-921. [PubMed]
  • Markou A, Kosten TR, Koob GF. Neurobiological similarities in depression and drug dependence: a self-medication hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;18: 135-174. [PubMed]
  • Martinez D, Broft A, Foltin RW, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Huang Y, et al. 2004. Cocaine dependence and d2 receptor availability in the functional subdivisions of the striatum: relationship with cocaine-seeking behavior Neuropsychopharmacology 291190–1202.1202(erratum: 29: 1763). [PubMed]
  • Martinez D, Gil R, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Huang Y, Perez A, et al. Alcohol dependence is associated with blunted dopamine transmission in the ventral striatum. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58: 779-786. [PubMed]
  • Martinez D, Narendran R, Foltin RW, Slifstein M, Hwang DR, Broft A, et al. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release: markedly blunted in cocaine dependence and predictive of the choice to self-administer cocaine. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164: 622-629. [PubMed]
  • McBride WJ, Murphy JM, Ikemoto S. Localization o faiga faʻaleleia o le faiai: faʻaogaina o le tagata lava ia ma le faʻaogaina o suʻesuʻega. Behav Brain Res. 1999;101: 129-152. [PubMed]
  • McClernon FJ, Kozink RV, Lutz AM, Rose JE. 24-h smoking abstinence potentiates fMRI-BOLD activation to smoking cues in cerebral cortex and dorsal striatum. Psychopharmacology. 2009;204: 25-35. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • McClung CA, Ulery PG, Perrotti LI, Zachariou V, Berton O, Nestler EJ. DeltaFosB: se fomaʻi mole mole mo le taimi umi i le faiʻai. Mol Brain Res. 2004;132: 146-154.
  • McFarland K, Kalivas PW. 2001. The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior J Neurosci 218655–8663.8663This study established a key role of the dorsal frontal cortex-nucleus accumbens-ventral pallidal circuit in cocaine-induced reinstatement. [PubMed]
  • McFarland K, Lapish CC, Kalivas PW. O le muaʻi faʻatagaina o le faʻamaʻaloga i totonu o le tumutumu o le nucleus accumbens e faʻatautaia ai le faʻaleleia o le cocaine i le toe faʻaleleia o amioga faʻatau fualaau. J Neurosci. 2003;23: 3531-3537. [PubMed]
  • McGregor A, Roberts DCS. Dopaminergic antagonism within the nucleus accumbens or the amygdala produces differential effects on intravenous cocaine self-administration under fixed and progressive ratio schedules of reinforcement. Brain Res. 1993;624: 245-252. [PubMed]
  • Melendez RI, Rodd ZA, McBride WJ, Murphy JM. Involvement of the mesopallidal dopamine system in ethanol reinforcement. Alu. 2004;32: 137-144. [PubMed]
  • Melis M, Spiga S, Diana M. The dopamine hypothesis of drug addiction: hypodopaminergic state. Faʻaaliga Neurobiol. 2005;63: 101-154. [PubMed]
  • Milton AL, Lee JL, Everitt BJ. Reconsolidation of appetitive memories for both natural and drug reinforcement is dependent on β-adrenergic receptors. Aoao le Mem. 2008;15: 88-92. [PubMed]
  • Miranda MI, LaLumiere RT, Buen TV, Bermudez-Rattoni F, McGaugh JL. O le polokaina o le noradrenergic receptors i le basolateral amygdala e faʻaleagaina ai le tofo. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18: 2605-2610. [PubMed]
  • Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Vaega o mafaufauga o le le mautonu. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158: 1783-1793. [PubMed]
  • Moller C, Wiklund L, Sommer W, Thorsell A, Heilig M. Decreased experimental anxiety and voluntary ethanol consumption in rats following central but not basolateral amygdala lesions. Brain Res. 1997;760: 94-101. [PubMed]
  • Moussawi K, Pacchioni A, Moran M, Olive MF, Gass JT, Lavin A, et al. N-acetylcysteine reverses cocaine-induced metaplasticity. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12: 182-189. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Murray EA. O le amygdala, taui ma lagona. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11: 489-497. [PubMed]
  • Naqvi NH, Bechara A. O le motu natia o vaisu: o le insula. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32: 56-67. [PubMed]
  • Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A. 2007. O le faʻaleagaina o le iniseti e faʻalavelave ai mea ua fai ma vaisu i sikaleti sikaleti saienisi 315531–534.534This study showed that damage to the insula in human smokers was associated with cessation of smoking, establishing a link between the insula and nicotine addiction. [PubMed]
  • Nauta JH, Haymaker W. 1969. Hypothalamic nuclei and fiber connectionsIn: Haymaker W, Anderson E, Nauta WJH (eds).The Hypothalamus Charles C Thomas: Springfield, IL; 136–209.209.
  • Nelson CL, Milovanovic M, Wetter JB, Ford KA, Wolf ME. E le o gatasi ma le amphetamine ona o le suiga i le faʻaaogaina o le talipamate i totonu o le kul ratus accumbens. J Neurochem. 2009;109: 35-51. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Nestler EJ. 2005. E i ai se auala masani mole molemole mo le tagofia o mea ua fai ma vaisu Nat Neurosci 81445–1449.1449This review summarizes a body of work characterizing the role of molecular changes mediating the transition from drug taking to addiction with a special emphasis on the accumulation of the transcription factor ΔFosB in the nucleus accumbens following chronic drug exposure. [PubMed]
  • Neugebauer V, Li W, Bird GC, Han JS. The amygdala and persistent pain. Neuroscientist. 2004;10: 221-234. [PubMed]
  • O’Dell LE, Koob GF. Nicotine deprivation effect in rats with intermittent 23-h access to intravenous nicotine self-administration. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2007;86: 346-353. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Olds J, Milner P. Faʻamalosia lelei e maua mai i le faʻamalosia o le eletise o le septal area ma isi vaega o le faiʻai iole. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1954;47: 419-427. [PubMed]
  • Orsini C, Koob GF, Pulvirenti L. Dopamine partial agonist reverses amphetamine withdrawal in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;25: 789-792. [PubMed]
  • Pierce RC, Bell K, Duffy P, Kalivas PW. Repeated cocaine augments excitatory amino acid transmission in the nucleus accumbens only in rats having developed behavioral sensitization. J Neurosci. 1996;16: 1550-1560. [PubMed]
  • Pulvirenti L, Koob GF. Lisuride reduces psychomotor retardation during withdrawal from chronic intravenous amphetamine self-administration in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1993;8: 213-218. [PubMed]
  • Rachlin H, Green L. Commitment, choice and self-control. J Exp Anal Behav. 1972;17: 15-22. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Robbins TW. Relationship between reward-enhancing and stereotypical effects of psychomotor stimulant drugs. Natura. 1976;264: 57-59. [PubMed]
  • Roberts AJ, Heyser CJ, Cole M, Griffin P, Koob GF. O le inu tele o le ethanol pe a mae'a se tala'aga o le fa'alagolago: fa'ata'ita'iga manu o allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22: 581-594. [PubMed]
  • Roberts DCS. 1992. Neural substrates mediating cocaine reinforcement: the role of monoamine systemsIn: Lakoski JM, Galloway MP, White FJ (eds).Cocaine: Pharmacology, Physiology and Clinical Strategies CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL; 73–90.90.
  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC. O le faavae faʻavae o fualaau oona: o se faʻaosofiaga-faʻalauiloa manatu o vaisu. Brain Res Faʻaaliga 1993;18: 247-291.
  • Robledo P, Koob GF. Two discrete nucleus accumbens projection areas differentially mediate cocaine self-administration in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 1993;55: 159-166. [PubMed]
  • Rocha BA, Fumagalli F, Gainetdinov RR, Jones SR, Ator R, Giros B, et al. Cocaine self-administration in dopamine-transporter knockout mice. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1: 132-137. [PubMed]
  • Rossetti ZL, Hmaidan Y, Gessa GL. Marked inhibition of mesolimbic dopamine release: a common feature of ethanol, morphine, cocaine and amphetamine abstinence in rats. Eur J Pharmacol. 1992;221: 227-234. [PubMed]
  • Russell MAH. 1976. What is dependenceIn: Edwards G (ed).Drugs and Drug Dependence Lexington Books: Lexington, MA; 182–187.187.
  • Saal D, Dong Y, Bonci A, Malenka RC. 2003. O fualaau o le sauaina ma le atuatuvale e mafua ai se fetuutuunaiga masani i synaptic i le dopamine neurons Neuron 37577–582.582(erratum: 38: 359). [PubMed]
  • Salamone JD, Correa M, Farrar A, Mingote SM. Gaioiga e fesoʻotaʻi faʻatasi ai ma le aʻafia o le dopamine ma isi faʻasologa faʻasolosolo. Psychopharmacology. 2007;191: 461-482. [PubMed]
  • Sanchis-Segura C, Spanagel R. Behavioural assessment of drug reinforcement and addictive features in rodents: an overview. Addict Biol. 2006;11: 2-38. [PubMed]
  • Sarnyai Z, Biro E, Gardi J, Vecsernyes M, Julesz J, Telegdy G. Brain corticotropin-releasing factor mediates ‘anxiety-like’ behavior induced by cocaine withdrawal in rats. Brain Res. 1995;675: 89-97. [PubMed]
  • Schoenbaum G, Saddoris MP, Ramus SJ, Shaham Y, Setlow B. Cocaine-experienced rats exhibit learning deficits in a task sensitive to orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Eur J Neurosci. 2004;19: 1997-2002. [PubMed]
  • Schulteis G, Ahmed SH, Morse AC, Koob GF, Everitt BJ. Conditioning and opiate withdrawal: the amygdala links neutral stimuli with the agony of overcoming drug addiction. Natura. 2000;405: 1013-1014. [PubMed]
  • Schulteis G, Stinus L, Risbrough VB, Koob GF. Clonidine poloka le mauaina ae le o le faʻaalia o le faʻamavaeina o le opiate faʻamaʻi i isumu. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;19: 406-416. [PubMed]
  • Schultz W. E tele galuega dopamine i vasega taimi eseese. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2007;30: 259-288. [PubMed]
  • Shaham Y, Shalev U, Lu L, de Wit H, Stewart J. Le faʻataʻitaʻiga toe faʻaleleia o le toe faʻafoʻiina o vailaʻau: talafaasolopito, metotia ma suʻesuʻega tetele. Psychopharmacology. 2003;168: 3-20. [PubMed]
  • Shalev U, Grimm JW, Shaham Y. 2002. Neurobiology of relapse to heroin and cocaine seeking: a review Pharmacol Rev 541–42.42This review summarizes the neurocircuitry associated with drug-, cue-, and stress-induced relapse determined from extensive work with animal models. [PubMed]
  • Sharma A, Brody AL. I vivo brain imaging of human exposure to nicotine and tobacco. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2009;192: 145-171. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Shaw-Lutchman TZ, Barrot M, Wallace T, Gilden L, Zachariou V, Impey S, et al. Regional and cellular mapping of cAMP response element-mediated transcription during naltrexone-precipitated morphine withdrawal. J Neurosci. 2002;22: 3663-3672. [PubMed]
  • Solomon RL, Corbit JD. An opponent-process theory of motivation: 1. Temporal dynamics of affect. Faʻamanatuga o le Tino. 1974;81: 119-145. [PubMed]
  • Stein L. Effects and interactions of imipramine, chlorpromazine, reserpine, and amphetamine on self-stimulation: possible neurophysiological basis of depression. Recent Adv Biol Psychiatry. 1962;4: 288-309. [PubMed]
  • Stinus L, Cador M, Zorrilla EP, Koob GF. Buprenorphine and a CRF1 antagonist block the acquisition of opiate withdrawal-induced conditioned place aversion in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30: 90-98. [PubMed]
  • Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration 2008. Results from the 2007 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: National Findings(Office of Applied Statistics, NSDUH Series H-34, DHHS Publication No. SMA 08-4343). Rockville, MD.
  • Sutton MA, Schmidt EF, Choi KH, Schad CA, Whisler K, Simmons D, et al. Extinction-induced upregulation in AMPA receptors reduces cocaine-seeking behaviour. Natura. 2003;421: 70-75. [PubMed]
  • Tanda G, Pontieri FE, Di Chiara G. Cannabinoid and heroin activation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by a common μ1 opioid le masini faʻaleleia. Saienisi. 1997;276: 2048-2050. [PubMed]
  • Thorsell A, Rapunte-Canonigo V, O’Dell L, Chen SA, King A, Lekic D, et al. Viral vector-induced amygdala NPY overexpression reverses increased alcohol intake caused by repeated deprivations in Wistar rats. Brain. 2007;130: 1330-1337. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, Ehlers CL. Effects of neuropeptide Y and corticotropin-releasing factor on ethanol intake in Wistar rats: interaction with chronic ethanol exposure. Behav Brain Res. 2005a;161: 133-140. [PubMed]
  • Thorsell A, Slawecki CJ, Ehlers CL. Effects of neuropeptide Y on appetitive and consummatory behaviors associated with alcohol drinking in Wistar rats with a history of ethanol exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005b;29: 584-590. [PubMed]
  • Tiffany ST, Carter BL, Singleton EG. Challenges in the manipulation, assessment and interpretation of craving relevant variables. Togafitiga. 2000;95 (Suppl 2:s177–s187. [PubMed]
  • Todtenkopf MS, Parsegian A, Naydenov A, Neve RL, Konradi C, Carlezon WA., Jr Brain taui na faʻatonuina e le siama a le AMPA i totonu o le tumutumu o tumutumu. J Neurosci. 2006;26: 11665-11669. [PubMed]
  • Tomasi D, Goldstein RZ, Telang F, Maloney T, Alia-Klein N, Caparelli EC, et al. Widespread disruption in brain activation patterns to a working memory task during cocaine abstinence. Brain Res. 2007a;1171: 83-92. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Tomasi D, Goldstein RZ, Telang F, Maloney T, Alia-Klein N, Caparelli EC, et al. Thalamo-cortical dysfunction in cocaine abusers: implications in attention and perception. Maualuga Resitala. 2007b;155: 189-201. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Tornatzky W, Miczek KA. Cocaine self-administration ‘binges’: transition from behavioral and autonomic regulation toward homeostatic dysregulation in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2000;148: 289-298. [PubMed]
  • Tucci S, Cheeta S, Seth P, File SE. Corticotropin releasing factor antagonist, α-helical CRF9-41, fa'aliliuina fa'aoso fa'aosoina i le nicotine fa'aoso, ae le fa'apea, popole. Psychopharmacology. 2003;167: 251-256. [PubMed]
  • Tzschentke TM. Fuaina o taui faʻatasi ma le tulaga o mea e fiafia iai: se iloiloga auiliili o fualaau oona, alualu i luma lata mai ma mataupu fou. Prog Neurobiol. 1998;56: 613-672. [PubMed]
  • Ungless MA, Whistler JL, Malenka RC, Bonci A. Single cocaine exposure i vivo faʻaosofia le malosi i le uumi i le dopamine neurons. Natura. 2001;411: 583-587. [PubMed]
  • Valdez GR, Roberts AJ, Chan K, Davis H, Brennan M, Zorrilla EP, et al. Increased ethanol self-administration and anxiety-like behavior during acute withdrawal and protracted abstinence: regulation by corticotropin-releasing factor. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2002;26: 1494-1501. [PubMed]
  • Vanderschuren LJ, Everitt BJ. O le faʻatagaina o fualaau faʻamalosi e faʻamalosi pe a maeʻa le puleaina o le cocaine umi. Saienisi. 2004;305: 1017-1019. [PubMed]
  • Vezina P. Sensitization of midbrain dopamine neuron reactivity and the self-administration of psychomotor stimulant drugs. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27: 827-839. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Chang L, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Ding YS, Sedler M, et al. 2001a. Laʻu maualalo o le faiʻai o le dopamine D2 i methamphetamine tagata faʻatau: faʻatasi ma le metabolism i le cortex poʻo le itu Am J Psychiatry 1582015–2021.2021This study shows an association between the decreases in dopamine function in addiction and decreased function of the orbitofrontal cortex, establishing a key link between compromised striatal activity and orbitofrontal dysfunction in addiction. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Ding YS, Gatley SJ. Role of dopamine in the therapeutic and reinforcing effects of methylphenidate in humans: results from imaging studies. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2002;12: 557-566. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Hitzemann R, Logan J, Schlyer DJ, et al. O le faʻaitiitia o le dopamine D2 o le mauaina o le talipupuni e fesoʻotaʻi ma le faʻaitiitia o le metabolism i le faʻasaina o le koko. Synapse. 1993;14: 169-177. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. O le tagata faiʻai ua fai ma vaisu e tilotilo i le malamalama o suʻesuʻega ata: gaioiga o faiʻai ma togafitiga togafitiga. Neuropharmacology. 2004a;47 ( Supl 1:3–13 . [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Swanson JM. Dopamine i le faʻaaoga sese o fualaau faasaina ma le tagofia o mea ua fai ma vaisu: e mafua mai i suʻesuʻega faʻataʻitaʻi ma togafitiga. Mol Psychiatry. 2004b;9: 557-569. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Gillespie H, Mullani N, Tancredi L, Grant C, Valentine A, et al. Brain glucose metabolism in chronic marijuana users at baseline and during marijuana intoxication. Maualuga Resitala. 1996a;67: 29-38. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Swanson JM. Variata e aafia ai le faʻaaogaina o falemaʻi ma le faʻaleagaina o le methylphenidate i le togafitiga o le ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160: 1909-1918. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang G, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gerasimov M, Maynard L, et al. Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly increase extracellular dopamine in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2001b;21: RC121. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fischman MW, Foltin RW, Fowler JS, Abumrad NN, et al. Relationship between subjective effects of cocaine and dopamine transporter occupancy. Natura. 1997a;386: 827-830. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Franceschi D, Thanos PK, Wong C, et al. Cocaine abusers show a blunted response to alcohol intoxication in limbic brain regions. Soifua Sci. 2000;66:PL161–PL167. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Gatley SJ, Ding YS, Logan J, et al. Relationship between psychostimulant-induced ‘high’ and dopamine transporter occupancy. Faʻaalia Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996b;93: 10388-10392.
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Hitzemann R, Angrist B, Gatley SJ, et al. Association of methylphenidate-induced craving with changes in right striato-orbitofrontal metabolism in cocaine abusers: implications in addiction. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156: 19-26. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Hitzemann R, Gatley SJ, Dewey SS, et al. Enhanced sensitivity to benzodiazepines in active cocaine-abusing subjects: a PET study. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155: 200-206. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Logan J, Gatley SJ, Hitzemann R, et al. 1997b. Faʻitiʻitia le tali atu o le dopaminergic i totonu o mataupu faʻapitoa o cocaine-faalagolago natura 386830–833.833This study using PET showed decreased release of dopamine in the striatum and a decreased ‘high’ produced by methylphenidate, suggesting a compromised striatal dopamine system in addiction. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Wong C, Ding YS, et al. Activation of orbital and medial prefrontal cortex by methylphenidate in cocaine-addicted subjects but not in controls: relevance to addiction. J Neurosci. 2005;25: 3932-3939. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Ma Y, Fowler JS, Zhu W, Maynard L, et al. Expectation enhances the regional brain metabolic and the reinforcing effects of stimulants in cocaine abusers. J Neurosci. 2003;23: 11461-11468. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, et al. Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction. J Neurosci. 2006;26: 6583-6588. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Childress AR, et al. Dopamine increases in striatum do not elicit craving in cocaine abusers unless they are coupled with cocaine cues. Neuroimage. 2008a;39: 1266-1273. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Jayne M, et al. Faʻaitiitia le faʻaitiitia o le faʻamalolo o le dopamine i le striatum i 'ava malosi faʻamaʻi: mafai ona aʻafia orbitofrontal. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 12700-12706. [PubMed]
  • Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Telang F, Fowler JS, Logan J, Wong C, et al. Sleep deprivation decreases binding of [11C]raclopride to dopamine D2/D3 receptors in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2008b;28: 8454-8461. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Logan J, Abumrad NN, Hitzemann RJ, et al. Dopamine D2 receptor availability in opiate-dependent subjects before and after naloxone-precipitated withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;16: 174-182. [PubMed]
  • Wang Z, Faith M, Patterson F, Tang K, Kerrin K, Wileyto EP, et al. Neural substrates of abstinence-induced cigarette cravings in chronic smokers. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 14035-14040. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Watkins SS, Stinus L, Koob GF, Markou A. Reward and somatic changes during precipitated nicotine withdrawal in rats: centrally and peripherally mediated effects. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2000;292: 1053-1064. [PubMed]
  • Wee S, Wang Z, Woolverton WL, Pulvirenti L, Koob GF. Effect of aripiprazole, a partial D2 receptor agonist, on increased rate of methamphetamine self-administration in rats with prolonged access. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32: 2238-2247. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wee S, Orio L, Ghirmai S, Cashman J, Koob GF. 2009. Inhibition of kappa opioid receptors attenuates the increased motivation for cocaine in rats with extended access to cocaine Psychopharmacology(i le lomitusi).
  • Weiss F, Ciccocioppo R, Parsons LH, Katner S, Liu X, Zorrilla EP, et al. 2001. Compulsive drug-seeking behavior and relapse: neuroadaptation, stress, and conditioning factorsIn: Quinones-Jenab V (ed).The Biological Basis of Cocaine Addiction(series title: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol 937)New York Academy of Sciences: New York; 1–26.26.
  • Weiss F, Markou A, Lorang MT, Koob GF. O le maualuga o le Basal extracellular dopamine i totonu o le nucleus accumbens ua faaitiitia i le taimi o le tosoina o cocaine pe a uma le puleaina o le puleaina faaletagata lava ia. Brain Res. 1992;593: 314-318. [PubMed]
  • Weiss F, Parsons LH, Schulteis G, Hyytia P, Lorang MT, Bloom FE, et al. Ethanol self-administration restores withdrawal-associated deficiencies in accumbal dopamine and 5-hydroxytryptamine release in dependent rats. J Neurosci. 1996;16: 3474-3485. [PubMed]
  • White NM. Addictive drugs as reinforcers: multiple partial actions on memory systems. Togafitiga. 1996;91: 921-949. [PubMed]
  • Whitelaw RB, Markou A, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala impair the acquisition of cocaine-seeking behaviour under a second-order schedule of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology. 1996;127: 213-224. [PubMed]
  • Wikler A. A psychodynamic study of a patient during experimental self-regulated re-addiction to morphine. Fomaʻi F. Q. 1952;26: 270-293. [PubMed]
  • Atamai poto. Catecholamine manatu o taui: o se iloiloga taua. Brain Res. 1978;152: 215-247. [PubMed]
  • Wolf ME. Addiction: making the connection between behavioral changes and neuronal plasticity in specific pathways. Mol Intervent. 2002;2: 146-157.
  • Wolf ME, Sun X, Mangiavacchi S, Chao SZ. Fomaʻi faʻamalositino ma mea e faʻamalosi ai le tino. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47 ( Supl 1:61–79 . [PubMed]
  • Yao WD, Gainetdinov RR, Arbuckle MI, Sotnikova TD, Cyr M, Beaulieu JM, et al. Identification of PSD-95 as a regulator of dopamine-mediated synaptic and behavioral plasticity. Neuron. 2004;41: 625-638. [PubMed]
  • Yeomans J, Baptista M. Both nicotinic and muscarinic receptors in ventral tegmental area contribute to brain-stimulation reward. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;57: 915-921. [PubMed]
  • Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22: 513-523. [PubMed]
  • Zubieta JK, Gorelick DA, Stauffer R, Ravert HT, Dannals RF, Frost JJ. Increased mu opioid receptor binding detected by PET in cocaine-dependent men is associated with cocaine craving. Nat Med. 1996;2: 1225-1229. [PubMed]
  • Zubieta J, Lombardi U, Minoshima S, Guthrie S, Ni L, Ohl LE, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow effects of nicotine in overnight abstinent smokers. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49: 906-913. [PubMed]