សកម្មភាពប្រែប្រួលនៃខួរក្បាលទាក់ទងនឹងបំណងប្រាថ្នានិងការឆ្លុះបញ្ចាំងក្នុងមនុស្សដែលមានបញ្ហាលេងល្បែងតាមប្រព័ន្ធអ៊ីនធឺណិត: ភស្តុតាងពីការប្រៀបធៀបជាមួយអ្នកលេងហ្គេមអនឡាញកំសាន្ត (2017)

។ 2017; 8: 1150 ។

បោះពុម្ភ online 2017 Jul 11 ។ doi:  10.3389 / fpsyg.2017.01150

PMCID: PMC5504237

អរូបី

Although the neural substrates of cue reactivity in Internet gaming disorder (IGD) have been examined in previous studies, most of these studies focused on the comparison between IGD subjects and healthy controls, which cannot exclude a potential effect of cue-familiarity. To overcome this limitation, the current study focuses on the comparison between IGD subjects and recreational Internet game users (RGU) who play online games recreationally but do not develop dependence. Data from 40 RGU and 30 IGD subjects were collected while they were performing an event-related cue reactivity task in the fMRI scanner. The results showed that the IGD subjects were associated with enhanced activation in the left orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and decreased activation in the right anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), right precuneus, left precentral gyrus and right postcentral gyrus in comparison with the RGU subjects. OFC is involved in reward evaluation and ACC is implicated in executive control function based on previous researches. Moreover, the activation of OFC were correlated with the desire for game-playing. Thus, the higher activation in OFC might suggests high desire for game playing, and the lower activation in ACC might indicates impaired ability in inhibiting the urge to gaming-related stimuli in IGD subjects. Additionally, decreased activation in the precuneus, the precentral and postcentral gyrus may suggest the deficit in disentangling from game-playing stimuli. These findings explain why IGD subjects develop dependence on game-playing while RGU subjects can play online games recreationally and prevent the transition from voluntary game-playing to eventually IGD.

ពាក្យគន្លឹះ: recreational Internet game users, Internet gaming disorder, cue-reactivity, impulse inhibition, intense desire

សេចក្តីផ្តើម

Internet gaming disorder (IGD), the most prevalent (57.5 percent) subtype of Internet addiction disorder (IAD) (; ; ), is defined as the incapacity to control the desire for obsessive online game playing, which leads to various functional impairments, such as social, financial, occupational, and behavioral difficulties (; ; ,, , ; ; ; ). It has been regarded as a type of non-financial pathologic gambling (; ), a form of behavior addiction (), or a type of impulse control disorder (). Based on the similarities among IGD, substance disorder, and pathologic gambling, the DSM-5 proposed the diagnostic criteria for IGD in conditions for further study ().

Craving is defined as the intense desire for the experience of a psychoactive substance or behavior (). It has been considered as the central feature of pathologic gambling and substance disorder (). The degree of craving can be increased by addiction-related cues (), which is thought to play a critical role in developing and maintaining addictive behaviors (; ; ), as well as relapse to addictive behaviors (; ; ). Previous neuroimaging researches on substance dependence and pathological gambling have revealed abnormal brain activity in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), amygdala, hippocampus, and precuneus in response to addiction-relevant cues (; ; ; ; ; ). Similarly, studies on the IGD have reported that compared to healthy controls (HC), subjects with IGD showed aberrant activation in OFC, DLPFC, ACC, precuneus, caudate nucleus in response to gaming pictures (; ; ; ; ).

However, all these studies on the cue-reactivity of IGD focused on the contrast between IGD subjects and HC (; ; ; ). This method has some limitations. First, it failed to control the game familiarities between IGD and HC, as IGD subjects are more familiar with the gaming cues than the HC; Second, IGD subjects played online games a lot, however, the HC subjects are low-frequent/none game players, they have limited experience with online gaming. To overcome these limitations, it is important to include a specific group of game players—the recreational Internet game users (RGU) as the control group. RGU are individuals who play online games recreationally but do not develop transition to addiction (; ). They do not show the core symptoms of addiction, such as loss of control, withdrawal, and conflict (). More importantly, they do not meet the diagnostic criteria for IGD by DSM-5 and do not need treatment (). Thus, the present study focused on the differences in neural activity of craving and cue-reactivity between IGD and RGU to expand the understanding of specific features of IGD, and explore risk factors and effective interventions for IGD.

As reviewed above, previous studies have demonstrated that IGD subjects reported stronger craving for game-playing and showed aberrant brain activities in regions responsible for reward evaluation, such as DLPFC, OFC (; ; ; ) as compared to HC subjects. Accordingly, we expected similar brain activities to game relevant cues in IGD subjects as compared to RGU subjects. In addition, it has been noted that IGD subjects are associated with failures in controlling the desire for playing online games (). Numerous imaging studies have found impaired executive control ability in IGD subjects (; , , , , , ; ; ; ,; ; ), yet, the direct evidence for the impaired executive control ability in inhibiting craving for game-playing in the context of online gaming cues are still lacking (, ; ; ). Thus, the present study filled in the gap. We expected that IGD subjects would show dysfunctional brain activities in the executive-control-related regions.

សំភារៈ​និង​វិធី​សា​ស្រ្ត

អ្នកចូលរួម

The present study was approved by the Human Investigations Committee of Zhejiang Normal University. Forty RGU and 30 individuals with IGD were recruited in this study. All participants were right-handed and provided written informed content in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were screened according to their scores on Young’s online Internet addiction test (IAT) (), the nine-item diagnostic criteria of IGD proposed by the DSM-5 committee (), and their weekly Internet gaming time. Young’s IAT consists of 20 items. Previous studies have testified the reliability and validity of IAT in classifying IAD (; ). Each item of Young’s IAT assesses the degree of Internet use-related problems (i.e., psychological dependence, withdrawal, and related problems in sleep, school, or work) on a 5-point-scale. Individuals who scored between 31 and 49 points are regarded as average online users who maintain control of Internet use, though sometimes they may spent a bit too long in surfing the Internet. Scores between 50 and 80 points reveal occasional or frequent Internet use-related problems as a result of uncontrolled Internet usage1.

Inclusion criteria for the IGD group were the following: (1) scored larger than 50 on Young’s IAT (,; ,); (2) met at least 5 DSM-5 criteria; (3) play online games is their major Internet activity; (4) play online games more than 14 h per week, for a minimum of 2 years; (5) endorsement of League of Legends (a popular online game in China) as the only source of Internet online games. The inclusion of RGU is the key step of the current study. The inclusion criteria for the RGU group were used previously () and described briefly as follows: (1) scored less than 50 on Young’s IAT; (2) met fewer than 5 DSM-5 criteria; (3) play online games more than 14 h per week, for a minimum of 2 years; (4) endorsement of League of Legends as the only source of Internet online games; (5) reported no feeling of remorse or guilt about playing online games and stated that their regular use did not interfere with school, family, work, or social obligations. Exclusion criteria for all participants included (1) historical records of or current psychiatric/neurological disorders (e.g., depression, anxiety, schizophrenia and substance dependence) assessed by a structured psychiatric interviews (MINI) (); (2) previous or current use of gambling and illegal drugs (i.e., heroin, marijuana) or any other types of addictions (e.g., alcohol). Participants were required to not take any medicine or substances including tea and coffee on the day of scanning.

តារាង តារាង 11 shows the demographic information of the two groups. There was no significant difference in age, BDI score, education level and the Internet gaming time between the IGD and RGU group, while the IAT scores and DSM-5 scores of the IGD group were significantly higher than those of the RGU group.

តារាង 1 

Demographic information and group differences.

កិច្ចការ និងនីតិវិធី

An event-related cue reactivity task was applied in this study. It contains two types of cue pictures: 30 gaming-related pictures and 30 typing-related pictures (neutral baseline). And in each type, half of the 30 pictures contained a face and the half contained a hand. As shown in តួលេខ រូបភាព 1A1A, gaming-related pictures describe a person who is playing the online game (LOL) on a computer, with half pictures showing faces and the other half showing hands. In typing-related pictures, the same person is typing an article on keyboard in front of a computer. The task of participants was to answer whether there was a face in the picture. They had to press the button ‘1’ (refer to ‘yes’) on the keyboard when a face was present and press ‘2’ (refer to ‘no’) when there was no face presented.

រូបភាព 1 

(ក) Examples of gaming-related stimuli (left) and typing-related stimuli (right). (ខ) The timeline of one trial in the event-related cue-reactivity task. The test version of the task is in Chinese. “An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc. Object name is fpsyg-08-01150-i001.jpg” means “The degree of your ...

តួលេខ រូបភាព 1B1B shows the timeline of a sample trial in the task. Firstly, a fixed 500 ms of cross was presented, followed by a cue picture as stated above. All pictures were presented in a randomized order. Each picture was presented for up to 3000 ms, during which participants had to make a response. The screen turned to black after button pressing and lasted for (3000 – response time)ms. Then, in the craving evaluation stage, participants were asked to evaluate the level of their craving for the corresponding stimuli on a 5-point scale, 1 (no craving) to 5 (extremely high craving). This stage lasted for up to 3000 ms and was terminated by a button-press. Finally, a 1500–3500 ms blank screen was presented between each trial. The whole task contained 60 trials and took almost 9 min. The task was presented and behavioral data was collected by the E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.). All participants were asked to fill out a 10-item gaming urge questionnaire, range from 1 to 10 to assess the gaming craving prior to the fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) scan ().

ការវិភាគទិន្នន័យអាកប្បកិរិយា

The performance parameters for the cue reactivity task were mean reaction time (RT) and mean scores of craving (gaming-related minus typing-related), named as induced craving scores. In addition, the scores of craving prior to the fMRI scan, named as initial craving scores, were also analyzed. In order to examine the difference between the IGD and RGU group, we performed an independent sample t-test on these three parameters.

Image Acquisition and Pre-processing

Functional MRI data were collected by a 3T MR system (Siemens Trio) with a gradient-echo EPI T2* sensitive pulse sequence in 33 slices, interleaved sequence, 3 mm thickness, 30 ms echo time (TE), 2000 ms repetition time (TR), 220 mm × 220 mm of field of view, 90° flip angle and 64 × 64 for matrix. All trials were presented using Invivo synchronous system (Invivo Company2) via a monitor in the head coil, which allowed participants to view the trials presented on the screen.

The fMRI data were analyzed using SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping3). Images were sliced-timed, reoriented and realigned to the first volume. And then T1-co-registered volumes were normalized to an SPM T1 template and smoothed using a 6 mm FWHM Gaussian Kernel spatially. No participant was excluded due to large head motion coefficients based on the criteria (head motion < 2.5 mm and 2.5 degree).

First-Level FMRI Analysis

In the present study, we applied a general linear model (GLM) to examine blood oxygen level dependence (BLOD) signal related to the two event types (gaming-related trials, typing-related trials) and others (missed or error response). GLM built a design matric to represent a combination of the experimental onsets convolved with a canonical haemodynamic response function (HRF), which included all trial conditions (gaming-related trials, typing-related trials, and missed trials) and six head motion parameters. Then, to improve the signal-to-noise ratio, a high pass filter (cut-off period was 128 s) was used to filter out low frequency noise.

Second-Level Group FMRI Analysis

Second-level analysis was conducted at the group level. At first, we identified voxels that showed a main effect in the gaming-related trials versus the typing-related trials among each group (IGD, RGU). Secondly, we determined voxels that were significantly different in BOLD signal between the two groups [(IGD ការលេងហ្គេម – IGD ការវាយ) – (RGU ការលេងហ្គេម – RGU ការវាយ)]. We then identified clusters of contiguous significantly different voxels at an uncorrected threshold p < 0.005. Finally, these clusters were tested for cluster-level FWE (family-wise-error) correction p < 0.05. Specially, the AlphaSim estimation indicated that clusters extent of 15 adjoining voxels would achieve the FEW threshold p < 0.05 effectively. The smoothing kernel applied in simulating false-positive (noise) maps using AlphaSim software was 6.0 mm and was estimated from residual fields of the contrast maps being pooled into the one-sample t-សាកល្បង។

វិភាគតំរែតំរង់

To identify the correlation between brain activities and behavioral performances, we first extracted the BOLD signal from the mean value of the remaining clusters that showed between-group differences. Then the BOLD data for all subjects were submitted to robust regression analyses with the RT, the induced craving scores, the initial craving scores, and the IAT and DSM scores. Note, robust regression analysis was used here to eliminate the effect of outliers, which represents the correlations between brain activations and behavioral performances.

លទ្ធផល

Behavioral Performance

The behavioral results showed significantly higher induced craving scores (IGD: 1.98 ± 1.10, RGU: 1.21 ± 0.78, t(1,69) = 3.25, p = 0.002) and initial craving scores (IGD: 53.10 ± 15.36, RGU: 39.13 ± 15.71, t(1,69) = 3.72, p = 0.000) in the IGD group as compared to the RGU group. No significant group-difference was found in the RT to cue pictures. Additionally, we found a significantly positive correlation between the IAT, DSM scores and the initial craving scores for all subjects (តួលេខ 2A, B) and for the IGD group (តួលេខ 2C, D). And the induced craving scores showed positively correlation with the IAT, DSM scores for all participants, respectively (តួលេខ 2E, F).

រូបភាព 2 

Correlations between the severity and the craving scores. (A, B) The initial craving scores show significantly positive correlation with the IAT scores, DSM scores for all subjects, respectively. (C, D) The initial craving scores show significantly positive ...

លទ្ធផលរូបភាព

We examined the brain activities in the cue-reactivity task between the IGD and RGU group (តួលេខ រូបភាព 33 និង តារាង តារាង 22). The IGD group showed increased BOLD signal activation in the left OFC compared to the RGU group, and decreased brain activities in the right ACC, right precuneus, left precentral gyrus and right postcentral gyrus in the IGD group when comparing to the RGU group.

រូបភាព 3 

Brain regions showing significant difference in IGD subjects compared with RGU subjects. The IGD subjects showed enhanced activation (shown in red) in the left orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and decreased activation (shown in blue) in the right anterior ...
តារាង 2 

Brain regions showing significant group-difference in BOLD signal.

Regression Analysis Results

As the robust regression lines in the តួលេខ រូបភាព 44, there were significant regression correlations between brain activations in the OFC, ACC, precuneus, left precentral gyrus and right postcentral gyrus and the IAT, DSM scores, which means brain activations in the these regions were correlated positively or negatively with the IAT, DSM scores for all participants. At the same time, the regression correlations between brain activation in these regions (except ACC) and the initial craving scores were significant or marginally significant. Also, we put the results of linear regression in the figure to show the differences between linear regression and robust regression.

រូបភាព 4 

Regression relationship between brain activities and behavioral performances. p(linear) in each figure refers to the p-value of linear regression coefficient. p(robust) in each figure refers to the p-value of robust regression coefficient. (ក) បង្ហាញឯកសារ ...

ការពិភាក្សា

As far as we know, this is the first study comparing the neural activities associated with gaming-cues evoked craving between subjects with IGD and RGU. The IGD subjects reported higher scores of craving for game-playing and showed dysfunctional brain activation in the left OFC, the right ACC, right precuneus, left precentral gyrus and right postcentral gyrus as compared to the RGU group.

Higher Desire for Game-Playing in IGD

The present imaging results demonstrated that the IGD subjects showed higher brain activity in the left OFC than the RGU group when exposed to the gaming-related cues. The OFC is widely thought to be involved in goal-directed behavior through assessing the significant stimuli and selecting appropriate behavior to achieve desired outcomes (). The similar feature has been reported in subjects with substance disorders, pathologic gambling, and online games addiction (; ; ; ; ). The OFC was found to be activated by expectations and delivery of reward (; ; ). It generates and maintains expectations for potential reward associated with reinforcement by integrating experiential history with current events (). These findings may reveal the important role of OFC in the craving for game-playing in IGD.

In current study, the IGD group reported significantly higher craving for online games than the RGU group both during and before fMRI scan. There was a positive association between the BOLD signal of the OFC, the initial craving scores, and the severity values of IGD (the IAT scores, DSM scores) among all participants. Thus, the larger the IGD values are, the stronger craving for gaming-playing and higher activation in the OFC would be observed. Taking all, we suggest that subjects with IGD generate expectations for game-playing through assessing the reward value of gaming behavior (which was induced by gaming-related cues) in the OFC. Therefore, they show stronger desire for game-playing than the RGU group, in line with the intense desire for drug-taking in drug addictions (; ). Alternatively, the OFC might be involved in other functions, such as the inhibition and so on. Further studies are warranted to investigate this possibility.

Impaired Control Ability in IGD

In the current study, decreased brain activity were detected in the right ACC in the IGD group comparing to the RGU group in response to gaming-related cues. Also, negative trends between the activities of ACC, the DSM and IAT scores were found among all participants, which suggest that the lower activation in the ACC is accompanied with higher severity of IGD. These results indicate that the ACC plays an essential role in the cue-reactivity of IGD, in accordance with prior researches on the cue-reactivity of IGD and other addictions (; ; ; ; ).

Convergent evidences have demonstrated that the ACC is involved in executive control function (; ; ; ). Executive control refers to one’s ability to direct or stop behaviors and thoughts, particularly when the behaviors (or thoughts) may not be advantageous or are regarded as improper (). Several neuroimaging researches have examined the impaired executive control ability indexed by dysfunction or structural abnormalities in ACC in people with IGD (, ; ; ; ), as well as in drug addiction and pathologic gambling (; ; ; ; ). Better control ability in recreational online game users than online game addicts are associated with enhanced activation in ACC in a decision-making task (). Combined with these findings, the present result may reveal a deficient executive control ability in IGD subjects, accompanied with comparably better control ability in RGU subjects. Additionally, subjects with IGD have been reported to link with deficit in executive control ability in cognitive tasks (; ; ) and are characterized by diminished control ability in Internet game-playing (). Besides, they showed higher scores in impulsivity () and thus are labeled as impulse control disorder (). These behavioral phenomena of IGD are compatible with our results. Alternatively, the ACC is also implicated in attention processes (; ), thus, the lower activation in ACC might also suggest reduced attentional capacity in IGD subjects. However, considering the feature of the current event-related cue-reactivity task, which requires participants to suppress their strong craving for game-playing and focus on the task (pressing the correct button), and taking the findings above together, we speculated that IGD subjects show deficit in controlling their intensive desire for game-playing (provoked by gaming-related cues) as compared to RGU subjects, which is consistent with the impaired control ability to inhibit the craving for drug intake in drug addiction (; ; ). Further studies are needed to investigate this issue. Notably, a cognitive-behavioral model of IGD proposed by have revealed enhanced desires for game-playing and poor control over such desires determined by the impairments of executive control function in individuals with IGD. The present result may contribute to certify the generalizability and availability of the model.

The Roles of Precuneus, Precentral, and Postcentral Gyrus

Decreased brain activation in the right precuneus, left precentral and right postcentral gyrus were detected in subjects with IGD as compared to RGU subjects in response to gaming-related cues. In correlation analysis results, we found negative trends between the BOLD signal of the precuneus, the precentral, postcentral gyrus and the initial craving scores. Altered activities in these regions have been reported in previous studies about the cue-reactivity of IGD (; ; ). These results may indicate that the precuneus, the precentral and postcentral gyrus have a strong relationship with the cue reactivity in IGD.

A review about neural substrates of smoking cue reactivity has argued that the precuneus play an important role in cue reactivity (). The precuneus has been proposed to contribute to attentional tracking of stimuli and the preparation of motor behaviors (). And it is involved in shifting attention between motor targets and motor imagery (). The precentral gyrus located in the Brodmann area 6 (pre-motor and supplementary motor cortex) is implicated in motor planning and execution. And the postcentral gyrus, as the primary somatosensory cortex, are the main sensory receptive region for the sense of touch. Besides, we found that the IGD subjects showed lower accuracy and longer RT than the RGU subjects, which demonstrated bad behavioral performance in subjects with IGD. On the aspect of our experimental task, participants were required to press buttons by observing if there was a face in the cue pictures with the background of game-playing. The lower activation in precuneus, precentral and postcentral gyrus in IGD may suggest the deficit in integrating visual and motor information from the cue-pictures and shifting attention from the stimuli of game-playing to the experimental task (pressing correct buttons). So far, the precuneus, precentral and postcentral gyrus received little attention in previous studies on the cue-reactivity of IGD. Thus, the present intriguing speculation may suggest that these three areas could be important areas of interest for further researches of cue reactivity in IGD.

ដែនកំណត់

There are some limitations of the present study to be noted. First, the causal relationship between IGD and the abnormal activity in the regions stated above could not be confirmed in the present study. It will be interesting to explore this relationship in future studies. Second, only seven female subjects were recruited for this study due to the higher popularity of online gaming in men than in women. Although they were balanced in the two groups (3 female IGD, 4 female RGU), the results may be biased. Further researches are needed to explore the gender effect in IGD. Third, several other variables, e.g., IQ, self-efficiency and socioeconomic status of the subjects, were not measured in the current study. The potential subgroup-difference in these variables may bias the results. Future studies should take these aspects into consideration. Finally, as the diagnostic criteria of IGD were still under consideration, the findings based on this criteria might be affected by it. A better diagnostic criterion of IGD might bring new insight into this issue.

សន្និដ្ឋាន

The present study examined different brain activation pattern between subjects with IGD and RGU using an event-related cue reactivity task. Hyperactive OFC indicates higher desire for game-playing and lower activation in ACC suggests impaired control ability in inhibiting the craving for game-playing in IGD subjects. Additionally, we infer that the decreased activation in the precuneus, precentral and postcentral gyrus may be associated with the difficulty in shifting attention from stimuli of game-playing to face-detection task in the IGD subjects. These findings explain why IGD subjects failed in preventing the transition from voluntary game-playing to eventual IGD, while as a contrast, RGU subjects can play online games recreationally without developing online-gaming dependence.

ការបរិច្ចាគអ្នកនិពន្ធ

LxW analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. LxW, YW, HL, and XL contributed to experimental programming, data preprocessing. XD contributed to fMRI data collection. GD designed this research. GD and LdW revised and improved the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

ទំនាស់ផលប្រយោជន៍

អ្នកនិពន្ធបានប្រកាសថាការស្រាវជ្រាវត្រូវបានធ្វើឡើងដោយគ្មានទំនាក់ទំនងពាណិជ្ជកម្មឬហិរញ្ញវត្ថុណាមួយដែលអាចត្រូវបានបកស្រាយថាជាទំនាស់ផលប្រយោជន៍ដែលអាចមាន។

ការទទួលស្គាល់

GD was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (31371023). The funding agencies did not contribute to the experimental design or conclusions. The views presented in the manuscript are those of the authors and may not reflect those of the funding agencies.

ភក្ដិកំណត់ត្រាកំណត់

ឯកសារយោង

  • Achab S., Nicolier M., Mauny F., Monnin J., Trojak B., Vandel P., et al. (2011). Massively multiplayer online role-playing games: comparing characteristics of addict vs non-addict online recruited gamers in a French adult population. Psychiatry BMC 11:144 10.1186/1471-244X-11-144 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • សមាគមចិត្តសាស្ត្រអាមេរិច (2013) ។ សៀវភៅរោគវិនិច្ឆ័យនិងស្ថិតិនៃជំងឺផ្លូវចិត្ត: DSM-5TM 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
  • Arthur G. L., Brende J. O., Locicero K. A. (2001). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders and Text Revision: Psychology Psychiatry Autism Family Therapy 4th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
  • Blumenthal D., Gold M. S. (2009). The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Substance Abuse Treatment. វ៉ាស៊ីនតោនឌីស៊ីៈការបោះពុម្ពចិត្តសាស្ត្រអាមេរិច។
  • Bonson K. R., Grant S. J., Contoreggi C. S., Links J. M., Metcalfe J., Weyl H. L., et al. (2002). Neural systems and cue-induced cocaine craving. Neuropsychopharmacology 26 376–386. 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00371-2 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Bush G., Frazier J. A., Rauch S. L., Seidman L. J., Whalen P. J., Jenike M. A., et al. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop. Biol ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ 45 1542–1552. 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00083-9 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Carter B. L., Tiffany S. T. (1999). Cue-reactivity and the future of addiction research. ញៀន 94 349-351 ។ 10.1046 / j.1360-0443.1999.9433273.x [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Cavanna A. E., Trimble M. R. (2006). The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. ខួរក្បាល 129(Pt 3) 564–583. 10.1093/brain/awl004 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Chen CY, Huang MF, Yen JY, Chen CS, Liu GC, Yen CF, et al ។ (2015) ។ ខួរក្បាលទាក់ទងគ្នានឹងការទប់ស្កាត់ការឆ្លើយតបនៅក្នុងការលេងល្បែងតាមអ៊ិនធរណេត។ ចិត្តវិទ្យាគ្លីនិក។ Neurosci ។ 69 201 – 209 ។ 10.1111 / pcn.12224 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Cooney N. L., Litt M. D., Morse P. A., Bauer L. O., Gaupp L. (1997). Alcohol cue reactivity, negative-mood reactivity, and relapse in treated alcoholic men. J. Abnorm ។ Psychol ។ 106 243–250. 10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.243 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Cox L. S., Tiffany S. T., Christen A. G. (2001). Evaluation of the brief questionnaire of smoking urges (QSU-brief) in laboratory and clinical settings. Nicotine Tob ។ Res ។ 3 7-16 ។ 10.1080 / 14622200020032051 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Crockford DN, Goodyear B., Edwards J., Quickfall J., El-Guebaly N. (2005) ។ សកម្មភាពខួរក្បាលដែលបង្កឡើងដោយសញ្ញានៅក្នុងអ្នកលេងល្បែងរោគសាស្ត្រ។ Biol ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ 58 787 – 795 ។ 10.1016 / j.biopsych.2005.04.037 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., DeVito E. E., Du X., Cui Z. (2012a). Impaired inhibitory control in ‘internet addiction disorder’: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. ចិត្តសាស្រ្ត Res ។ 203 153 – 158 ។ 10.1016 / j.pscychresns.2012.02.001 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Hu Y., Lin X., Lu Q. (2013a). What makes internet addicts continue playing online even when faced by severe negative consequences? Possible explanations from an fMRI study. Biol ។ Psychol ។ 94 282-289 ។ 10.1016 / j.biopsycho.2013.07.009 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Hu Y., Xiao L. (2013b). Reward/punishment sensitivities among internet addicts: Implications for their addictive behaviors. Prog ។ Neuropsychopharmacol ។ Biol ។ ចិត្តសាស្រ្ត 46 139 – 145 ។ 10.1016 / j.pnpbp.2013.07.007 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Huang J., Du X. (2011a). Enhanced reward sensitivity and decreased loss sensitivity in Internet addicts: an fMRI study during a guessing task. J. ពេទ្យចិត្តសាស្ត្រ។ Res ។ 45 1525 – 1529 ។ 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2011.06.017 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Jie H., Du X. (2012b). Alterations in regional homogeneity of resting-state brain activity in Internet gaming addicts. Behav ។ ខួរក្បាល Funct ។ 8:41 10.1186/1744-9081-8-41 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Li H., Wang L., Potenza M. N. (2017b). Cognitive control and reward/loss processing in Internet gaming disorder: results from the comparison with recreational internet game-users. អឺ​រ៉ូ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ ២៣ ២១២–២១៨។ 44/j.eurpsy.30 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Lin X., Potenza M. N. (2015). Decreased functional connectivity in an executive control network is related to impaired executive function in Internet gaming disorder. Progr. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 57 76 – 85 ។ 10.1016 / j.pnpbp.2014.10.012 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Lin X., Zhou H., Du X. (2014a). Decision-making after continuous wins or losses in a randomized guessing task: implications for how the prior selection results affect subsequent decision-making. Behav ។ ខួរក្បាល Funct ។ 10:11 10.1186/1744-9081-10-11 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Lin X., Zhou H., Lu Q. (2014b). Cognitive flexibility in internet addicts: fMRI evidence from difficult-to-easy and easy-to-difficult switching situations. អ្នកញៀន។ Behav ។ 39 677-683 ។ 10.1016 / j.addbeh.2013.11.028 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Lu Q., Zhou H., Zhao X. (2010). Impulse inhibition in people with internet addiction disorder: electrophysiological evidence from a Go/NoGo study. Neurosci ។ Lett ។ 485 138-142 ។ 10.1016 / j.neulet.2010.09.002 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Lu Q., Zhou H., Zhao X. (2012c). Precursor or sequela: pathological disorders in people with internet addiction disorder. PLoS ONE 6: e14703 10.1371 / journal.pone.0014703 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Potenza MN (2014) ។ គំរូនៃការយល់ដឹង-អាកប្បកិរិយានៃជំងឺហ្គេមអ៊ីនធឺណិត៖ ទ្រឹស្ដី និងផលប៉ះពាល់ខាងគ្លីនិក។ J. ពេទ្យចិត្តសាស្ត្រ។ Res ។ 58 7 – 11 ។ 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2014.07.005 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Potenza M. N. (2016). Risk-taking and risky decision-making in Internet gaming disorder: implications regarding online gaming in the setting of negative consequences. J. ពេទ្យចិត្តសាស្ត្រ។ Res ។ 73 1 – 8 ។ 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2015.11.011 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Wang L., Du X., Potenza M. N. (2017a). Gaming increases craving to gaming-related stimuli in individuals with Internet gaming disorder. Biol ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ 2 404–412. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.01.002 [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Zhou H. (2010). Is impulse-control ability impaired in people with internet addiction disorder: electrophysiological evidence from ERP studies. Int ។ J. Psychophysiol ។ 77 334-335 ។ 10.1016 / j.ijpsycho.2010.06.271 [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Dong G., Zhou H., Zhao X. (2011b). Male Internet addicts show impaired executive control ability: evidence from a color-word Stroop task. Neurosci ។ Lett ។ 499 114-118 ។ 10.1016 / j.neulet.2011.05.047 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Elliott R., Dolan R. J., Frith C. D. (2000). Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Cereb ។ Cortex 10 308 – 317 ។ 10.1093 / Cercor / 10.3.308 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Engelmann J. M., Versace F., Robinson J. D., Minnix J. A., Lam C. Y., Cui Y., et al. (2012). Neural substrates of smoking cue reactivity: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neuroimage 60 252-262 ។ 10.1016 / j.neurimage.2011.12.024 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Feng D., Yuan K., Li Y., Cai C., Yin J., Bi Y., et al. (2015). Intra-regional and inter-regional abnormalities and cognitive control deficits in young adult smokers. រូបភាពខួរក្បាល Behav ។ 10 506–516. 10.1007/s11682-015-9427-z [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Filbey F. M., Claus E., Audette A. R., Niculescu M., Banich M. T., Tanabe J., et al. (2008). Exposure to the taste of alcohol elicits activation of the mesocorticolimbic neurocircuitry. Neuropsychopharmacology 33 1391–1401. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301513 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Franklin T. R., Wang Z., Wang J., Sciortino N., Harper D., Li Y., et al. (2007). Limbic activation to cigarette smoking cues independent of nicotine withdrawal: a perfusion fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology 32 2301–2309. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301371 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • George M. S., Anton R. F., Bloomer C., Teneback C., Drobes D. J., Lorberbaum J. P., et al. (2001). Activation of prefrontal cortex anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues. Arch ។ ចិត្តសាស្រ្ត 58 345-352 ។ 10.1001 / archpsyc.58.4.345 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Goldstein R. Z., Volkow N. D. (2002). Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am ។ J. ចិត្តសាស្រ្ត ១៦៥ ៣០៦–៣០៧។ 159/appi.ajp.1642 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND (2011) ។ ភាពមិនស៊ីគ្នានៃផ្នែកកណ្តាល prefrontal នៅក្នុងការញៀន: ការរកឃើញសរសៃប្រសាទនិងផលប៉ះពាល់ខាងគ្លីនិក។ ណត។ Rev. Neurosci ។ 12 652 – 669 ។ 10.1038 / nrn3119 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Goudriaan A. E., Ruiter M. B. D., Brink W. V. D., Oosterlaan J., Veltman D. J. (2010). Brain activation patterns associated with cue reactivity and craving in abstinent problem gamblers, heavy smokers and healthy controls: an fMRI study. អ្នកញៀន។ Biol ។ 15 491-503 ។ 10.1111 / j.1369-1600.2010.00242.x [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Griffiths M. (2005). Relationship between gambling and video-game playing: a response to Johansson and Gotestam. Psychol ។ តំណាង។ 96 644–646. 10.2466/pr0.96.3.644-646 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Han D. H., Bolo N., Daniels M. A., Arenella L., Lyoo I. K., Renshaw P. F. (2010). Brain activity and desire for Internet video game play. Compr ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ 52 88 – 95 ។ 10.1016 / j.comppsych.2010.04.004 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Han D. H., Lyoo I. K., Renshaw P. F. (2012). Differential regional gray matter volumes in patients with on-line game addiction and professional gamers. J. ពេទ្យចិត្តសាស្ត្រ។ Res ។ 46 507 – 515 ។ 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2012.01.004 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Hester R., Dan I. L., Yücel M. (2010). The role of executive control in human drug addiction. Curr ។ ខាងលើ។ Behav ។ Neurosci ។ 3 301–318. 10.1007/7854_2009_28 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Holden C. (2001). ‘Behavioral’ addictions: do they exist? វិទ្យាសាស្រ្ត 294 980 – 982 ។ 10.1126 / វិទ្យាសាស្ត្រ។ 294.5544.980 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Kerns J. G., Cohen J. D., Cho R. Y., Stenger V. A., Carter C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. វិទ្យាសាស្រ្ត 303 1023 – 1026 ។ 10.1126 / វិទ្យាសាស្ត្រ។ 1089910 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Ko C. H. (2014). Internet gaming disorder. Curr ។ អ្នកញៀន។ តំណាង។ 1 177–185. 10.1007/s40429-014-0030-y [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Ko C. H., Liu G. C., Hsiao S., Yen J. Y., Yang M. J., Lin W. C., et al. (2008). Brain activities associated with gaming urge of online gaming addiction. J. ពេទ្យចិត្តសាស្ត្រ។ Res ។ 43 739 – 747 ។ 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2008.09.012 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Ko C. H., Liu G. C., Yen J. Y., Yen C. F., Chen C. S., Lin W. C. (2013). The brain activations for both cue-induced gaming urge and smoking craving among subjects comorbid with Internet gaming addiction and nicotine dependence. J. ពេទ្យចិត្តសាស្ត្រ។ Res ។ 47 486 – 493 ។ 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2012.11.008 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Ko C. H., Yen J. Y., Chen S. H., Wang P. W., Chen C. S., Yen C. F. (2014). Evaluation of the diagnostic criteria of Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5 among young adults in Taiwan. J. ពេទ្យចិត្តសាស្ត្រ។ Res ។ 53 103 – 110 ។ 10.1016 / j.jpsychires.2014.02.008 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Kosten T. R., Scanley B. E., Tucker K. A., Oliveto A., Prince C., Sinha R., et al. (2005). Cue-induced brain activity changes and relapse in cocaine-dependent patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 31 644–650. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300851 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Kuss ឌីជេ, ហ្គីហ្វីត MD (2012) ។ ការញៀនលេងល្បែងតាមអ៊ិនធឺរណែតៈការពិនិត្យឡើងវិញជាប្រព័ន្ធនៃការស្រាវជ្រាវដែលមានលក្ខណៈជាក់ស្តែង។ Int ។ J. Ment ។ ការញៀនសុខភាព។ 10 278–296. 10.1007/s11469-011-9318-5 [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Lecrubier Y., Sheehan D. V., Weiller E., Amorim P., Bonora I., Sheehan K. H., et al. (1997). The mini international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI. អឺ​រ៉ូ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ 12 224–231. 10.1016/S0924-9338(97)83296-8 [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Lee H. W., Choi J. S., Shin Y. C., Lee J. Y., Jung H. Y., Kwon J. S. (2012). Impulsivity in internet addiction: a comparison with pathological gambling. Cyberpsychol ។ អាកប្បកិរិយា។ Soc បណ្តាញ។ 15 373-377 ។ 10.1089 / cyber.2012.0063 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Lin X., Dong G., Wang Q., Du X. (2015a). Abnormal gray matter and white matter volume in ‘Internet gaming addicts’. អ្នកញៀន។ Behav ។ 40 137-143 ។ 10.1016 / j.addbeh.2014.09.010 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Lin X., Zhou H., Dong G., Du X. (2015b). Impaired risk evaluation in people with Internet gaming disorder: fMRI evidence from a probability discounting task. Progr. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 56 142 – 148 ។ 10.1016 / j.pnpbp.2014.08.016 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Liu L., Yip S. W., Zhang J.-T., Wang L. J., Shen Z. J., Liu B., et al. (2016). Activation of the ventral and dorsal striatum during cue reactivity in Internet gaming disorder. អ្នកញៀន។ Biol ។ 69 794 – 804 ។ 10.1111 / adb.12338 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Maas L. C., Lukas S. E., Kaufman M. J., Weiss R. D., Daniels S. L., Rogers V. W., et al. (1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activation during cue-induced cocaine craving. Am ។ J. ចិត្តសាស្រ្ត 155 124–126. 10.1176/ajp.155.1.124 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Marissen M. A. E., Franken I. H. A., Waters A. J., Blanken P., Brink W. V. D., Hendriks V. M. (2006). Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following treatment. ញៀន 101 1306-1312 ។ 10.1111 / j.1360-0443.2006.01498.x [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Moeller S. J., Konova A. B., Parvaz M. A., Tomasi D., Lane R. D., Fort C., et al. (2013). Functional, structural, and emotional correlates of impaired insight in cocaine addiction. JAMA Psychiatry ៧១ ៨១២–៨២០។ 71/jamapsychiatry.61 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Myrick H., Anton R. F., Li X., Henderson S., Drobes D., Voronin K., et al. (2004). Differential brain activity in alcoholics and social drinkers to alcohol cues: relationship to craving. Neuropsychopharmacology 29 393–402. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300295 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Petry N. M., Rehbein F., Gentile D. A., Lemmens J. S., Rumpf H. J., Mößle T., et al. (2014). An international consensus for assessing Internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. ញៀន 109 1399 – 1406 ។ 10.1111 / បន្ថែម។ 12457 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Rolls E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb ។ Cortex 10 284 – 294 ។ 10.1093 / Cercor / 10.3.284 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Ruiter M. B. D., Oosterlaan J., Veltman D. J., Brink W. V. D., Goudriaan A. E. (2011). Similar hyporesponsiveness of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in problem gamblers and heavy smokers during an inhibitory control task. គ្រឿងស្រវឹងគ្រឿងស្រវឹងអាស្រ័យ។ 121 81 – 89 ។ 10.1016 / j.drugalcdep.2011.08.010 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Sadock B. J., Sadock V. A. (2007). Kaplan & Sadock’s Synopsis of Psychiatry: Behavioral Sciences/Clinical Psychiatry 10th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
  • Santangelo G., Barone P., Trojano L., Vitale C. (2013). Pathological gambling in Parkinson’s disease. A comprehensive review. ទំនាក់ទំនងផាកឃីនសាន់។ ភាពមិនប្រក្រតី។ 19 645–653. 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.02.007 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Schmidt A., Borgwardt S., Gerber H., Schmid O., Wiesbeck G. A., Riecherrössler A., et al. (2014). Altered prefrontal connectivity after acute heroin administration during cognitive control. Int ។ J. Neuropsychopharmacol ។ 17 1375 – 1385 ។ 10.1017 / S1461145714000297 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Schultz W., Tremblay L., Hollerman J. R. (2000). Reward processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Cereb ។ Cortex 10 272 – 284 ។ 10.1093 / Cercor / 10.3.272 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Seidman L. J., Valera E. M., Makris N., Monuteaux M. C., Boriel D. L., Kelkar K., et al. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging. Biol ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ 60 1071 – 1080 ។ 10.1016 / j.biopsych.2006.04.031 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Sinha R., Li S. (2007). Imaging stress- and cue-induced drug and alcohol craving: association with relapse and clinical implications. គ្រឿងស្រវឹងគ្រឿងស្រវឹង Rev. 26 25-31 ។ 10.1080 / 09595230601036960 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Sun Y., Huang Y., Seetohul R. M., Wang X., Zheng Y., Li Q., et al. (2012). Brain fMRI study of crave induced by cue pictures in online game addicts (male adolescents). Behav ។ ខួរក្បាល Res ។ 233 563 – 576 ។ 10.1016 / j.bbr.2012.05.005 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Tong C., Bovbjerg D. H., Erblich J. (2007). Smoking-related videos for use in cue-induced craving paradigms. អ្នកញៀន។ Behav ។ 32 3034-3044 ។ 10.1016 / j.addbeh.2007.07.010 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Tremblay L., Schultz W. (1999). Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. ធម្មជាតិ 398 704-708 ។ 10.1038 / 19525 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Viriyavejakul C. (2008). “Recreational gaming behavior of undergraduate students in Thailand,” in Proceedings of Society for Information Technology & Teacher Education International Conference 2008 eds McFerrin K., Weber R., Carlsen R., Willis D., editors. (Chesapeake, VA: Association for the Advancement of Computing in Education; ) 4948–4955.
  • Wang H., Jin C., Yuan K., Shakir T. M., Mao C., Niu X., et al. (2015). The alteration of gray matter volume and cognitive control in adolescents with Internet gaming disorder. មុខ។ Behav ។ Neurosci ។ 9: 64 10.3389 / fnbeh.2015.00064 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Wang L., Wu L., Lin X., Zhang Y., Zhou H., Du X., et al. (2016a). Altered brain functional networks in people with Internet gaming disorder: evidence from resting-state fMRI. ចិត្តសាស្រ្ត Res ។ 254 156 – 163 ។ 10.1016 / j.pscychresns.2016.07.001 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Wang L., Wu L., Lin X., Zhang Y., Zhou H., Du X., et al. (2016b). Dysfunctional default mode network and executive control network in people with Internet gaming disorder: independent component analysis under a probability discounting task. អឺ​រ៉ូ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ ២៣ ២១២–២១៨។ 34/j.eurpsy.36 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Wang L., Zhang Y., Lin X., Zhou H., Du X., Dong G. (2017). Group independent component analysis reveals alternation of right executive control network in Internet gaming disorder. វិសាលគម CNS (នៅក្នុងសារព័ត៌មាន) ។
  • Wang Y., Wu L., Wang L., Zhang Y., Du X., Dong G. (2016a). Impaired decision-making and impulse control in Internet gaming addicts: evidence from the comparison with recreational Internet game users. អ្នកញៀន។ Biol ។ 10.1111/adb.12458 [Epub ahead of print]. [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Wang Y., Wu L., Zhou H., Lin X., Zhang Y., Du X., et al. (2016b). Impaired executive control and reward circuit in Internet gaming addicts under a delay discounting task: independent component analysis. អឺ​រ៉ូ។ Arch ។ ចិត្តវិទ្យាគ្លីនិក។ Neurosci ។ 267 245–255. 10.1007/s00406-016-0721-6 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Weinstein A., Lejoyeux M. (2015). New developments on the neurobiological and pharmaco-genetic mechanisms underlying internet and videogame addiction. Am ។ J. ញៀន។ 24 117–125. 10.1111/ajad.12110 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Weinstein A., Livny A., Weizman A. (2017). New developments in brain research of internet and gaming disorder. Neurosci ។ Biobehav ។ បប។ 75 314-330 ។ 10.1016 / j.neubiorev.2017.01.040 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Wheelock M. D., Reid M. A., To H., White D. M., Cropsey K. L., Lahti A. C. (2014). Open label smoking cessation with varenicline is associated with decreased glutamate levels and functional changes in anterior cingulate cortex: preliminary findings. មុខ។ Pharmacol ។ 5:158 10.3389/fphar.2014.00158 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Widyanto L., Griffiths M. D., Brunsden V. (2011). A psychometric comparison of the internet addiction test, the internet-related problem scale, and self-diagnosis. Cyberpsychol ។ អាកប្បកិរិយា។ Soc បណ្តាញ។ 14 141-149 ។ 10.1089 / cyber.2010.0151 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Widyanto L., Mcmurran M. (2004). The psychometric properties of the internet addiction test. Cyberpsychol ។ Behav ។ 7 443-450 ។ 10.1089 / cpb.2004.7.443 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Wrase J., Grüsser S. M., Klein S., Diener C., Hermann D., Flor H., et al. (2002). Development of alcohol-associated cues and cue-induced brain activation in alcoholics. អឺ​រ៉ូ។ ចិត្តសាស្ត្រ 17 287–291. 10.1016/S0924-9338(02)00676-4 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • Young K. S. (1998). “Internet addiction: symptoms, evaluation, and treatment,” in Innovations in Clinical Practice: A Source Book Vol. 17 eds VandeCreek L., Jackson T., editors. (Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Press; ) 19–31.
  • ខេអេសអេស (2009) ។ Internet Addiction Test (IAT) [Online]. មាននៅ: http://www.globaladdiction.org/dldocs/GLOBALADDICTION-Scales-InternetAddictionTest.pdf
  • Zhang Y., Xiao L., Zhou H., Xu J., Du X., Dong G. (2016). Brain activity toward gaming-related cues in Internet gaming disorder during an addiction stroop task. មុខ។ Psychol ។ 7: 714 10.3389 / fpsyg.2016.00714 [អត្ថបទឥតគិតថ្លៃ PMC] [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]
  • ចូវអ៊ី, លីនអេហ្វ, ឌូអេស, ឃិនអិល, ហ្សូហ្សូ, ហ្សូជេ, អេល។ (2011) ។ ភាពមិនប្រក្រតីនៃបញ្ហាពណ៌ប្រផេះក្នុងការញៀនអ៊ិនធឺរណែតៈការសិក្សាអំពីម៉ូលេគុលដែលមានមូលដ្ឋានលើសត្វឆ្កែ។ អឺ​រ៉ូ។ J. Radiol ។ 79 92 – 95 ។ 10.1016 / j.ejrad.2009.10.025 [PubMed] [សម្រង់ឆ្លាក់]